The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective:

Although disasters are a major cause of mental health problems and typically affect large numbers of people and communities, little is known about how social structures affect mental health after a disaster. The authors assessed the extent to which mental health outcomes after disaster are associated with social network structures.

Method:

In a community-based cohort study of survivors of a major bushfire disaster, participants (N=558) were assessed for probable posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and probable depression. Social networks were assessed by asking participants to nominate people with whom they felt personally close. These nominations were used to construct a social network map that showed each participant’s ties to other participants they nominated and also to other participants who nominated them. This map was then analyzed for prevailing patterns of mental health outcomes.

Results:

Depression risk was higher for participants who reported fewer social connections, were connected to other depressed people, or were connected to people who had left their community. PTSD risk was higher if fewer people reported being connected with the participant, if those who felt close to the participant had higher levels of property loss, or if the participant was linked to others who were themselves not interconnected. Interestingly, being connected to other people who in turn were reciprocally close to each other was associated with a lower risk of PTSD.

Conclusions:

These findings provide the first evidence of disorder-specific patterns in relation to one’s social connections after disaster. Depression appears to co-occur in linked individuals, whereas PTSD risk is increased with social fragmentation. These patterns underscore the need to adopt a sociocentric perspective of postdisaster mental health in order to better understand the potential for societal interventions in the wake of disaster.

There is overwhelming evidence that disasters result in increased rates of mental health problems, including posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and depression (1, 2). Although much evidence indicates that this impact is moderated by social support (35), the exact role of social relationships and psychosocial resources in protecting against mental health deficits has been the subject of debate. Although diminished social participation has been reported as a risk factor for poor mental health after disaster (6, 7), there is also evidence that PTSD hinders subsequent positive social support and causes withdrawal from social connections (8, 9). It has also been shown that disaster can alter the perceived availability of social structures (10). Despite these findings, the specific nature of how people interact in networks after disaster and how different patterns of connectedness affect mental health have yet to be articulated.

A major reason for our limited understanding of the role of societal factors in postdisaster mental health has been the focus to date on individual responses at the expense of understanding relationships between individuals and societal subgroups. Specifically, postdisaster studies that take into account social factors have typically measured individuals’ perceptions of social support relationships (69). This approach can be contrasted with the sociocentric framework of social network approaches (11, 12). Social networks can be regarded as a web of one-to-one links or ties that exist among individuals (or groups or organizations). This approach allows researchers to map the extent to which the social structure of person-to-person relationships is related to health and other outcomes. In turn, through various methods referred to generally as social network analysis, it is possible to analyze the co-occurrence of health outcomes in a sociospatial sense, as well as how these outcomes may relate to specific ideas of structural position within a network (13). Network research methods can show how a particular outcome may depend not only on the number of one’s immediate social relationships, but also on 1) the particular outcomes observed concurrently in others to whom one is linked either directly or indirectly, and 2) on larger substructures of network ties involving multiple individuals. Increasingly, work in this area has shown that where one is positioned within a social network, and the connectedness of that social network, moderates many general health outcomes, including obesity (1416), tobacco use (17, 18), and alcohol consumption (19). Moreover, the nature of one’s social networks has been shown to have an impact on a range of psychological functions, including depression (2023), happiness and positive mood (24, 25), and loneliness (26). Together, these methods open the door to consideration of social selection and social influence mechanisms in the prevalence of mental health conditions, and of how these outcomes may spread over social ties or influence whom people choose to associate with (21). Furthermore, in addition to mapping one-to-one ties between pairs of individuals, social network analysis allows for the identification of many types of network structures, such as when a person is an intermediary between others who are not interconnected themselves (20). An example of this approach is that in the context of social isolation, women having friends who are not friends with each other is predictive of suicidal ideation (27). This situation may be contrasted with scenarios in which a person is part of an integrated network in which the people they know also know each other.

However, despite the increasing popularity of networks as a general conceptual tool and its increasingly sophisticated use in health research generally, exceedingly few studies have employed social network research methods to investigate social determinants of postdisaster mental health (2830). Moreover, these studies have relied exclusively on personal (egocentric) network methods, in which each participant is surveyed about his or her direct social relationships (as he or she perceives them), rather than placing individuals within a larger community network structure of direct and indirect ties (a sociocentric, or whole-network, approach). This dearth of network investigations of postdisaster mental health is unfortunate because one distinguishing feature of most disasters is their collective nature, with many disasters (e.g., hurricanes, earthquakes) having an impact on a community of individuals who are known to each other (31). Such disasters therefore not only traumatize individuals, they also cause widespread community and infrastructure disruption, instigating social upheaval and the relocation of affected individuals away from the community (32). A pivotal role is therefore afforded to social factors in postdisaster recovery (33). Accordingly, the goal of the present study was to apply social network methodology to examine 1) how the presence and configuration of social ties between survivors of a large disaster are associated with mental health outcomes, and 2) how mental health outcomes may occur within connected social networks. We predicted that mental health conditions (PTSD and depression) would be inversely related to close social ties, defined in three different ways: close ties to others as nominated by the participant (termed the ego), close ties to the participant as nominated by other participants (termed alters), and mutual (reciprocal) ties as nominated by a pair of participants. Next, given previous evidence that depression can spread within a social network (20), we expected that people with depression and PTSD would be disproportionately linked to one another within the network. Furthermore, we explored the possible influence of more complex network patterns involving multiple ties in various configurations. Finally, we considered the impact that disaster-related experiences within one’s immediate social network have on one’s own mental health.

Method

Participants

In February 2009, Australia suffered one of its most devastating disasters as severe bushfires affected most of the state of Victoria, with the worst occurring on Saturday, February 7. Commonly referred to as the “Black Saturday” fires, this disaster resulted in 173 fatalities and 3,500 buildings damaged or destroyed, and it had a massive adverse impact on community infrastructures (34).

The “Beyond Bushfires: Community, Resilience, and Recovery” study (35) included adults (at least 18 years of age) living in 25 communities in 10 rural locations that were selected because they were variably affected by the fires. The 2006 census data indicated a total adult population of 7,693 in the selected communities. The Victorian Electoral Commission provided contact details for both current residents and those who had relocated since the fires (N=7,467 adults), and a letter was sent to them inviting them to participate in the study, with a postage-paid reply envelope. In total, 14.1% of eligible individuals completed the interview (N=1,056). (Local requirements dictated that potential participants could not be telephoned until they had contacted the research team by mail to indicate their willingness to participate.) Relative to available census data, the sample who participated in this study were, on average, older and more educated than the general community, and more likely to be female (35).

Measures

Network name generator.

Respondents were initially asked to nominate the persons to whom they felt “particularly close.” Participants were then asked to provide a full name, age, gender, address, and role relationship to that person (various family relations, friend, workmate, neighbor, etc.). It was on this basis that record matching was performed. These ties pertain to those that are the most intimate and frequent, likely serving as a source of emotional support and normative sanctioning.

PTSD.

Probable PTSD was assessed using an abbreviated version of the Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist–Civilian Version (36) comprising four items, each scored on a 5-point severity scale, that index key symptoms of PTSD and refer to the previous 4 weeks. Adopting a cutoff score of 7 on the abbreviated version achieves an efficient estimation of PTSD diagnosis relative to the full version of the instrument (Cronbach’s alpha=0.85) (36).

Depression.

Depression was assessed using the depression module of the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) (37). To determine presence of some mood disturbance, we used a score of 5 as the cutoff to determine presence of depression (Cronbach’s alpha=0.85) (37).

Disaster exposure.

A composite measure of severity of bushfire exposure was assessed in terms of fear for life (yes/no), death of loved ones (yes/no), property loss (ranging from 0=nothing to 10=everything, recoded into approximate quartiles [0, 1–4, 5–9, 10]).

Subsequent life events.

Major life events occurring since the bushfires were assessed, including an index of nontraumatic stressors (negative changes in employment, loss of income, livelihood) and traumatic stressors (assault/violence, serious accident). Relocation away from community after the fires was indicated by comparing past and current residential addresses (38).

Procedure

The study was approved by the University of Melbourne Human Research Ethics Committee. Data collection occurred between December 2011 and January 2013. The interview was conducted in a combination of telephone and web-based formats. After the participant provided informed consent, the interviewer initially asked a broad array of sociodemographic questions, then asked questions about social connections, events that have occurred since the fires, and events that occurred on the day of the Black Saturday fires, and then administered the Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist–Civilian Version and the PHQ-9. Additional questions relating to physical health, mental health, resilience, and connection to community were asked but are not included in the analyses reported here.

Network Data and Data Analysis

Social network data were constructed by means of an extensive record-matching process in which personal information (name, address, gender, and age) of participants and their social network nominations were linked. Furthermore, the network data were directed, with individuals represented as sending ties to and receiving from others. This lends an additional level of precision in the interpretation of data, as emotional closeness can be ascribed to the feelings of one individual toward another. This approach is a major improvement on previous (egocentric) studies that have only asked about the numbers of other people a participant is connected to, along with a few demographic characteristics, without key information on the concurrent mental health outcomes and disaster experiences of those people.

Several criteria determined inclusion in the final network sample: participants nominated someone else in the network; were nominated by someone in the network; or did not name any close ties, without declining to participate (referred to as “isolates”). Excluded were participants who both declined to name close ties and were not nominated by other participants, as well as those whose network ties were not participants in the study (N=413). An additional set of participants (N=85) was included in the analysis in a strictly exogenous (predictive) fashion, with their network ties and mental health outcomes only serving as predictors for other individuals: these individuals had declined to nominate their own network contacts (N=34), and/or had missing data on individual-level attributes (N=31), or had moved into a study community since the bushfires (N=20). One participant had a missing value for presence or absence of probable PTSD, which was imputed from other data. The final sample for analyses comprised 558 individuals.

The class of network statistical model used here is an autologistic actor attribute model (ALAAM) approach (3941). ALAAMs allow examination of how individual behavior, attitudes, or other outcomes may be constrained and enabled by the structure of surrounding social network relationships. As in a conventional logistic regression, an ALAAM may include a range of individual-level factors (e.g., disaster experiences, sociodemographic characteristics) to predict a dichotomous outcome at the level of the individual. However, unlike conventional regression methods, which carry an assumption of independence of observations, ALAAMs take interdependence into account. Through the use of Monte Carlo Markov chain maximum likelihood estimation, ALAAMs can test a principled set of assumptions (referred to as conditional dependence assumptions) that detail how various configurations of network ties affect the individual outcome (41). As social ties are by definition interdependent, the use of ALAAMs aligns the data with the method of analysis and is therefore a much more theoretically precise approach to studying social networks. In particular, the sociocentric (whole-network) approach to network analysis, on which the ALAAM is based, allows us to examine several network effects of particular interest that are not possible with conventional statistical methods. First, it permits us to examine how one’s network partner’s outcome affects one’s own outcome. A positive co-occurrence effect indicates a higher likelihood of having a given outcome if one has a network partner who also has that outcome; this suggests social processes of contagion or social selection. Second, it allows us to consider the impact of incoming network ties, and thus the importance of how others feel toward the focal individual. Third, a sociocentric approach allows us to consider the association between an outcome and network ties that the focal individual is not directly involved in. For instance, an individual’s mental health may depend in part on the degree to which his or her network partners know one another, communicate with one another, and can influence one another. In this respect, a network position of particular interest is that of brokerage, or the position of being connected to two (or more) individuals who have no direct ties between them.

For both depression and PTSD, we conducted a conventional binary logistic regression containing individual factors (personal attributes and events) to serve as a counterpoint to the more principled network ALAAMs. Next, for each outcome, we estimated an ALAAM. All dyadic network effects were specified to focus on the impact of outgoing ties, incoming ties, reciprocal ties, and the co-occurrence of either outcome (depression, PTSD) across these ties. Furthermore, more complex network configurations involving three or more individuals were specified through a backward elimination procedure. While backward elimination carries the risk of potentially overfitting the data, the general lack of a comprehensive theoretical framework of networks and mental health necessitates a partially exploratory approach. We therefore started with a full complement of network configurations (i.e., activity/popularity effects, brokerage effects, and triangular closure) and removed more complex (nonsignificant) network effects. We then calculated simpler network configurations, re-estimating the model at each step (the initial full models can be seen in Tables S1 and S2 in the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article). Furthermore, the models each included a set of “alter” effects, which address how being connected to other individuals who have certain attributes or experiences (i.e., other people leaving the community, other people’s property damage) may additionally influence ego’s mental health outcomes.

ALAAMs were estimated using the MPNet software program (42). Each ALAAM model was assessed by means of a goodness-of-fit test; this is a stringent test of the data to ensure that the estimated model can reproduce both within-model and out-of-model effects reliably. By convention, significance testing is analogous to a Wald test, with a value of 2 considered to be significant (and analogous to a parametric test statistic of 1.96).

Results

Of the 558 participants included in the estimation, there were 241 men and 317 women; their mean age was 57.1 years (SD=12.9). The rate of probable PTSD was 15.6% (N=87), and the rate of probable depression was 38.5% (N=215).

Network Analysis

Depression.

Both a conventional binary logistic regression and an ALAAM were applied to the occurrence of depression within the network. As shown in Table 1, female gender, fear for life, the loss of someone close, financial adversity, and subsequent traumatic events each predicted depression. Property damage was also a significant predictor in the logistic regression model, but it did not reach significance in the network model. Beyond the individual-level effects, the ALAAM indicated that nominating others as personally close network ties was associated with less depression (Figure 1A). However, being linked to someone with depression positively predicted being depressed oneself (Figure 1C); this effect is displayed in Figure 2. Finally, the model demonstrated that having a close tie to someone who moved away from the community predicted greater depression (Figure 1F).

TABLE 1. Model Results for Probable Depression in a Study of Social Networks and Postdisaster Mental Health Outcomesa

Logistic RegressionALAAM
θSEθSEWald χ2
Individual effects
 Female0.370.20*0.430.211.99
 Age0.000.010.000.010.18
 Postsecondary education–0.140.20–0.140.210.69
 Fear for life0.530.20**0.550.222.53
 Interpersonal loss0.450.21*0.480.212.27
 Property loss0.150.07*0.170.101.62
 Financial stressors0.440.10***0.460.104.55
 Traumatic events1.210.27***1.200.274.43
 Relocation–0.360.32–0.690.381.81
 Network density–1.670.51***–1.650.533.10
Network effects
 Sender (see Figure 1A)–0.490.212.34
 Receiver–0.260.231.13
 Reciprocity0.380.450.85
 Co-occurrence (see Figure 1C)0.670.332.06
 Co-occurrence × reciprocity–0.980.800.23
Alter effects
 Sender × relocation (alter) (see Figure 1F)1.460.722.03
 Receiver × relocation (alter)0.090.740.12
 Reciprocity × relocation (alter)0.221.240.18

aProbable depression was assessed using the depression module of the Patient Health Questionnaire. Values ≥2 on the Wald test are considered to be significant. ALAAM=autologistic actor attribute model.

***p≤0.001. **p≤0.01. *p≤0.05.

TABLE 1. Model Results for Probable Depression in a Study of Social Networks and Postdisaster Mental Health Outcomesa

Enlarge table
FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Summary of Social Network Effects for Depression and PTSD After Disaster

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. Odds of Depression, by Ego and Alter’s Depression, Among Participants in a Study of Social Networks and Postdisaster Mental Health Outcomes

PTSD.

As with depression, a conventional binary logistic regression and an ALAAM were conducted for probable PTSD (see Table 2). In terms of individual-level effects, fear for life, the loss of someone close, financial adversity, subsequent traumatic events, and property damage predicted probable PTSD. In the ALAAM, receiving nominations from alters predicted less risk of PTSD (Figure 1B). At the same time, however, ego’s risk of PTSD increased in association with receiving nominations from alters with higher levels of property loss. These effects can be rephrased from another perspective: overall, ego nominating an alter was associated with a reduction in the alter’s risk of PTSD. However, as ego’s own property loss increased, so did the risk of PTSD for alters to whom ego felt close (Figure 3). There were no co-occurrence effects regarding PTSD in either model (Figure 1C)

TABLE 2. Model Results for Probable Posttraumatic Stress Disorder in a Study of Social Networks and Postdisaster Mental Health Outcomesa

Logistic RegressionALAAM
θSEθSEWald χ2
Individual effects
 Female0.200.270.250.270.92
 Age–0.010.01–0.010.01–0.81
 Postsecondary education–0.250.26–0.290.29–0.99
 Fear for life0.710.28**0.760.282.69
 Interpersonal loss0.720.26**0.910.283.25
 Property loss0.270.09**0.320.152.11
 Financial stressors0.460.13***0.480.133.74
 Traumatic events1.050.31***1.060.323.31
 Relocation–0.360.41–0.420.43–0.98
 Network density–3.110.72***–2.580.75–3.46
Network effects
 Sender (see Figure 1A)–1.030.57–1.79
 Receiver (see Figure 1B)–1.410.58–2.41
 Reciprocity1.651.021.61
 Co-occurrence–0.610.49–1.24
 Co-occurrence × reciprocityb
 Brokerage (see Figure 1D)0.670.232.88
 Cyclic closure (see Figure 1E)–2.961.44–2.06
Alter effects
 Sender × property loss (alter)0.180.300.61
 Receiver × property loss (alter) (see Figure 1G)0.630.292.20
 Reciprocity × property loss (alter)–0.880.56–1.58

aProbable posttraumatic stress disorder was assessed using an abbreviated version of the Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist–Civilian Version. Values ≥2 on the Wald test are considered to be significant. ALAAM=autologistic actor attribute model.

bEffect not estimable (not observed in network).

***p≤0.001. **p≤0.01. *p≤0.05.

TABLE 2. Model Results for Probable Posttraumatic Stress Disorder in a Study of Social Networks and Postdisaster Mental Health Outcomesa

Enlarge table
FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3. Odds of Probable PTSD, by Incoming Ties and Alters’ Property Loss, Among Participants in a Study of Social Networks and Postdisaster Mental Health Outcomes

In terms of higher-order network effects (i.e., network effects involving more than one tie), the ALAAM showed that being positioned between individuals who were not connected with each other was associated with an increase in risk for probable PTSD (i.e., brokerage; Figure 1D). However, the risk for PTSD was reduced when the person was connected in a larger cyclic configuration suggesting that they are part of a connected social network (i.e., indirect reciprocity; Figure 1E).

Supplementary analyses.

In recognition that the sample was small in relation to the entire population affected by the disaster, and that the sample differed in terms of gender and age in relation to the broader community, we conducted supplementary analyses to test the robustness of the overall results in representative subsamples. Specifically, we used resampling procedures using subsets of the sample that were representative of the age and gender of the communities (see the online data supplement). These analyses indicated that the key findings for depression are representative of the wider population of affected communities under the age of 75 years (see Table S1 in the data supplement). Given the lower rates of PTSD as compared with depression in these communities, it is less conclusive that the results for PTSD are fully representative, given the limits on statistical power. However, the reanalyses indicate that the PTSD results are generalizable to middle-aged individuals (ages 45–75), with the exception of the cyclic closure results (see Tables S2 and S3 in the data supplement).

Discussion

Although commentators have often emphasized the role of social support on postdisaster mental health (6, 43, 44), the patterns by which mental health occurs across social networks has not been explored in a postdisaster context. This study represents one of the first applications of social network analysis to the study of postdisaster mental health outcomes, and the first use of complex statistical models for social networks in the field. The sociocentric approach, in combination with the use of ALAAM, allows analyses to extend beyond an individual’s own perception of his/her own social ties to provide a more integrated investigation of how social networks intersect with mental health outcomes following disaster.

The novel finding in this study is that after the disaster, the less others (alters) nominated a focal individual (ego) as someone they were close to, the greater the likelihood of the focal individual (ego) suffering PTSD. The exception to this pattern was if the nominating tie came from individuals with higher property loss: the more that ego (the focal individual) was nominated by alters (other participants) who had higher property loss, the higher ego’s associated risk of PTSD. Put another way, as one’s own property loss increased, so did the risk of PTSD for one’s close social contacts, even after controlling for their own property loss. It appears that whereas participants’ depressive symptoms were increased if they reported less connectedness to others, reduced close ties from others was specifically associated with PTSD. The finding that PTSD was specifically associated with fewer ties from other people may be explained by the well-documented pattern for people with PTSD to become socially detached and nonresponsive to their environments (4547). It is possible that people with more severe PTSD reactions retreated from social connections with others, thereby isolating them from otherwise available networks. The interpretation is further supported by the finding that receiving a tie from an alter with higher property loss was associated with a higher probability of having PTSD, suggesting that contact with others who have experienced their own trauma and social disruption may compound PTSD symptoms in the individual. Over the long term, this may prompt the affected individual to withdraw from social interactions with that person, thereby enacting the avoidance component of PTSD and leading to a weakening or dissolution of the relationship. Alternatively, it is feasible that diminished receipt of social connectedness directly led to greater PTSD because it deprived these disaster survivors of social support. It is also possible that these two processes are mutually reinforcing.

The other core finding from this study related to depression. We found that participants who reported fewer ties with others were at greater risk for probable depression. This is hardly a surprising finding given that this self-report is largely consistent with the methodology of previous studies that have relied on self-reported social support in demonstrating its relationship with mental health. The novel finding with respect to the postdisaster literature was that people who were depressed were socially connected with others who were also depressed. Several explanations may account for this observation. It is possible that one’s depression increased the more one had connections with other depressed people because of an influence effect of the depressed state. That is, being depressed may affect the mental state of people one interacts with because one’s mood can directly affect other people’s mood states. This interpretation accords with a previous study of a general population that depression can spread along social networks (20). However, drawing this inference would require longitudinal data, so other possibilities need to be considered. It is possible that depressed people were socially connected because they were drawn to each other. Alternatively, common exposures to environmental factors may link people who are depressed; for example, factors associated with their social structure, community, or experience of the disaster that link people to each other may independently contribute to depression. In this context, we note that previous research outside the disaster context has suggested that depression may co-occur within a network as a function of withdrawal from other social networks, which in turn results in depressed individuals being more connected with each other because of their limited social networks (21).

Interestingly, the pattern of depressed people being linked to each other was not one seen for PTSD. PTSD is a condition primarily driven by anxiety maintained by distressing memories of a triggering traumatic experience (48, 49), and accordingly it is predominantly driven by internal experiences. It could be argued that the maintaining factors of PTSD are internally generated and are rooted in a personally threatening experience; the capacity for PTSD to spread along social connections is less than that for depression because PTSD is more susceptible to the effects of the traumatic experience than to ongoing mood effects. Furthermore, since PTSD is often characterized by social withdrawal (50), it may be less likely that the effects of PTSD will be communicated to others because people with PTSD are prone to avoiding social interactions; this interpretation is supported by the finding that PTSD severity is associated with fewer connections with other people.

Another finding was that although people who left their community were at no greater risk of depression, for those remaining in the affected community, feeling close to those who had left predicted depression. This accords with evidence that separation from a close other increases risk for depression (51). Moreover, a delay in connecting with social supports has been shown to be associated with an increase in risk for mental problems following a mass disaster (52). It is possible that feeling close to another who is not proximally available enhances depression risk because of a sense of loneliness.

Another key observation was that PTSD was associated with “brokerage” patterns, indicating that PTSD was more likely when people had both incoming and outgoing social ties, indicating an in-between social position. Crucially, this effect was countered when the brokerage pattern was part of a cyclic pattern. This finding accords with previous patterns of impaired mental health being linked to fractured social networks with competing role demands coming from unconnected portions of their social network (27). By contrast, cyclic network structures are sites of generalized social exchange, affording equal status, cohesiveness and solidarity, and robust norms of reciprocity among several people (53). It is possible that by virtue of their interconnections, these individuals are likely better situated to coordinate support for one another, leading to a reduced risk of PTSD. An alternative interpretation is that brokerage structures may be more likely for PTSD-affected individuals as a result of their avoidance strategies. Brokerage positions imply that the person is part of two different social settings that are relatively private with respect to one another. The affected individual may withdraw to one setting when confronted with upsetting stimuli or interactions within the other. Accordingly, if the unified setting implied by the triadic configuration is marked by reminders of the trauma, the PTSD-affected individual may ultimately withdraw.

We recognize several methodological limitations. First, these conclusions are based on cross-sectional data, and longitudinal social network analysis is needed to more confidently map the causal directions of social effects. Second, it is possible that unmeasured characteristics influenced how disaster survivors were linked to each other and how mental health status may co-occur between individuals. Third, we assessed probable PTSD and depression rather than full diagnostic caseness. Fourth, as would be expected in a community study of this nature, there are missing data. For this new class of network model, the extent to which missingness might affect statistical power is not yet fully known; nor is the effect of extensive missing network data on the precision of parameter estimates.

The overall response of the sample was only 16% of potential participants. Although this is a low response rate relative to studies that engage in random sampling in population studies, it is consistent with studies that are ethically required to adopt an opt-in approach that involves consent prior to researchers contacting participants; for example, one previous population study employing this approach (54) achieved a 20% response rate, and among participants who were sociodemographically comparable to the population reported here, the rate was 16%, the same rate achieved in the present study. This pattern accords with much evidence of lower response rates for studies requiring that participants contact the research team to provide consent to interview them (5557). Reviews of response rates across jurisdictions requiring initial consent have robustly demonstrated markedly lower rates than in regions that do not have this requirement (58). This issue can complicate the representative nature of the data because nonresponse at the initial consent phase can be moderated by age, gender, and sociodemographic factors (54). We qualify these cautions with the recognition that network analyses are less susceptible to the effects of bias than conventional epidemiological studies because they examine the social interdependence of the sample, and hence the independence assumptions of traditional population studies are not adopted (59). Nonetheless, biases may influence results, so we replicated our major findings by limiting the sample to participants who were age- and gender-matched with community census data (for a full discussion, see the online data supplement). The present findings warrant replication with samples that can achieve larger response rates to limit the possibility of biases affecting results.

In conclusion, this study provides important new evidence concerning postdisaster social structures and how they are associated with mental health outcomes. Depression appears to occur in people who are connected, which may point to the importance of social interactions in maintaining depressive responses. Moreover, PTSD was associated with more fractured social networks. Taken together, these findings highlight the need to look beyond individual effects if posttraumatic mental health is to be adequately understood. Delineating social structures after disaster and how these moderate mental health trajectories can shed light on social interventions that may facilitate adjustment after disaster.

From the School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney; the Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Melbourne; the Jack Brockhoff Child Health and Wellbeing Program, Melbourne School of Population and Global Health, University of Melbourne, Melbourne; the Office of the Deputy Vice-Chancellor (Education), University of Sydney, Sydney; the Southgate Institute for Health, Society, and Equity, Flinders University, Adelaide; the Department of Social Work, University of Melbourne, Melbourne; the Victorian Department of Health and Human Services, Melbourne; the Centre for Disaster Management and Public Safety, University of Melbourne, Melbourne; Emergency Services, Australian Red Cross, Melbourne; Phoenix Australia–Centre for Posttraumatic Mental Health, University of Melbourne, Melbourne; and the Centre for Transformative Innovation, Swinburne University of Technology, Melbourne.
Address correspondence to Dr. Bryant ().

Supported by an Australian Research Council Linkage Grant (LP100200164), with additional support from the Australian Red Cross, Australian Rotary Health, the Victorian Department of Health, Centrelink, Phoenix Australia–Centre for Posttraumatic Mental Health, and these Primary Care Partnerships: Central West Gippsland, Bendigo Loddon, North East, Outer East, Lower Hume, and Central Hume.

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

The authors are grateful for the contributions of the participating individuals and communities, as well as for comments from Garry Robins and Peng Wang and technical assistance from George Fedorov and David Rolls.

References

1 Norris FH, Friedman MJ, Watson PJ, et al.: 60,000 disaster victims speak, part I: an empirical review of the empirical literature, 1981–2001. Psychiatry 2002; 65:207–239Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Norris FH, Friedman MJ, Watson PJ: 60,000 disaster victims speak, part II: summary and implications of the disaster mental health research. Psychiatry 2002; 65:240–260Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Ozer EJ, Best SR, Lipsey TL, et al.: Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 2003; 129:52–73Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Brewin CR, Andrews B, Valentine JD: Meta-analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults. J Consult Clin Psychol 2000; 68:748–766Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Liu B, Tarigan LH, Bromet EJ, et al.: World Trade Center disaster exposure-related probable posttraumatic stress disorder among responders and civilians: a meta-analysis. PLoS One 2014; 9:e101491Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Anwar J, Mpofu E, Matthews LR, et al.: Risk factors of posttraumatic stress disorder after an earthquake disaster. J Nerv Ment Dis 2013; 201:1045–1052Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 McDermott B, Berry H, Cobham V: Social connectedness: a potential aetiological factor in the development of child post-traumatic stress disorder. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2012; 46:109–117Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Kaniasty K, Norris FH: A test of the social support deterioration model in the context of natural disaster. J Pers Soc Psychol 1993; 64:395–408Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 King DW, Taft C, King LA, et al.: Directionality of the association between social support and posttraumatic stress disorder: a longitudinal investigation. J Appl Soc Psychol 2006; 36:2980–2992CrossrefGoogle Scholar

10 Palinkas LA, Downs MA, Petterson JS, et al.: Social, cultural, and psychological impacts of the Exxon Valdez oil spill. Hum Organ 1993; 52:1–13CrossrefGoogle Scholar

11 Valente T: Network Models of the Diffusion of Innovations. Creskill, NJ, Hampton Press, 1995Google Scholar

12 Borgatti SP, Mehra A, Brass DJ, et al.: Network analysis in the social sciences. Science 2009; 323:892–895Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Valente T: Network models and methods for studying the diffusion of innovations, in Models and Methods in Social Network Analysis. Edited by Carrington PJ, Scott J, Wasserman S. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 2005, pp 98–116Google Scholar

14 Christakis NA, Fowler JH: The spread of obesity in a large social network over 32 years. N Engl J Med 2007; 357:370–379Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Schaefer DR, Simpkins SD: Using social network analysis to clarify the role of obesity in selection of adolescent friends. Am J Public Health 2014; 104:1223–1229Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Simpkins SD, Schaefer DR, Price CD, et al.: Adolescent friendships, BMI, and physical activity: untangling selection and influence through longitudinal social network analysis. J Res Adolesc 2013; 23:537–549CrossrefGoogle Scholar

17 Christakis NA, Fowler JH: The collective dynamics of smoking in a large social network. N Engl J Med 2008; 358:2249–2258Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Schaefer DR, Haas SA, Bishop NJ: A dynamic model of US adolescents’ smoking and friendship networks. Am J Public Health 2012; 102:e12–e18Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Rosenquist JN, Murabito J, Fowler JH, et al.: The spread of alcohol consumption behavior in a large social network. Ann Intern Med 2010; 152:426–433Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Rosenquist JN, Fowler JH, Christakis NA: Social network determinants of depression. Mol Psychiatry 2011; 16:273–281Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Schaefer DR, Kornienko O, Fox AM: Misery does not love company: network selection mechanisms and depression homophily. Am Sociol Rev 2011; 76:764–785CrossrefGoogle Scholar

22 Cheadle JE, Goosby BJ: The small-school friendship dynamics of adolescent depressive symptoms. Soc Ment Health 2012; 2:99–119Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Hatzenbuehler ML, McLaughlin KA, Xuan Z: Social networks and risk for depressive symptoms in a national sample of sexual minority youth. Soc Sci Med 2012; 75:1184–1191Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Fowler JH, Christakis NA: Dynamic spread of happiness in a large social network: longitudinal analysis over 20 years in the Framingham Heart Study. BMJ 2008; 337:a2338Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Hill E, Griffiths F, House T: Spreading of healthy mood in adolescent social networks Proc R Soc 2015; 282(1813):20151180Google Scholar

26 Cacioppo JT, Fowler JH, Christakis NA: Alone in the crowd: the structure and spread of loneliness in a large social network. J Pers Soc Psychol 2009; 97:977–991Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Bearman PS, Moody J: Suicide and friendships among American adolescents. Am J Public Health 2004; 94:89–95Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Haines VA, Beggs JJ, Hurlbert JS: Contextualizing health outcomes: do effects of network structure differ for women and men? Sex Roles 2008; 59:164–175CrossrefGoogle Scholar

29 Jones EC, Tobin G, McCarty C, et al: Articulation of personal network structure with gendered well-being in disaster and relocation settings, in Issues of Gender and Sexual Orientation in Humanitarian Emergencies. Edited by Roeder LW. London, Springer, 2014, pp 19–31Google Scholar

30 Varda DM, Forgette R, Banks D, et al.: Social network methodology in the study of disasters: issues and insights prompted by post-Katrina research. Popul Res Policy Rev 2009; 28:11–29CrossrefGoogle Scholar

31 Lindy J, Grace M: The recovery environment: continuing stressor versus a healing psychosocial space, in Disasters and Mental Health: Selected Contemporary Perspectives. Edited by Sowder B, Lystad M. Rockville, Md, National Institute of Mental Health, 1986, pp 147–160Google Scholar

32 Hobfoll SE, Watson P, Bell CC, et al.: Five essential elements of immediate and mid-term mass trauma intervention: empirical evidence. Psychiatry 2007; 70:283–315Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Norris FH, Kaniasty K: Received and perceived social support in times of stress: a test of the social support deterioration deterrence model. J Pers Soc Psychol 1996; 71:498–511Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 2009 Victorian Bushfires Royal Commission Final Report: Summary. Melbourne, Parliament of Victoria, 2009 Victorian Bushfires Royal Commission, July 2010Google Scholar

35 Gibbs L, Waters E, Bryant RA, et al.: Beyond Bushfires: Community, Resilience, and Recovery: a longitudinal mixed method study of the medium to long term impacts of bushfires on mental health and social connectedness. BMC Public Health 2013; 13:1036Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Bliese PD, Wright KM, Adler AB, et al.: Validating the Primary Care Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Screen and the Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist with soldiers returning from combat. J Consult Clin Psychol 2008; 76:272–281Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB: The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med 2001; 16:606–613Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Uscher-Pines L: Health effects of relocation following disaster: a systematic review of the literature. Disasters 2009; 33:1–22Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Robins G, Pattison P, Elliott P: Network models for social influence processes. Psychometrika 2001; 66:161–189CrossrefGoogle Scholar

40 Kashima Y, Wilson S, Lusher D, et al.: The acquisition of perceived descriptive norms as social category learning in social networks. Soc Networks 2013; 35:711–719CrossrefGoogle Scholar

41 Daraganova G, Robins G: Autologistic actor attribute models, in Exponential Random Graph Models for Social Networks: Theory, Methods, and Applications. Edited by Lusher D, Koskinen J, Robins G. New York, Cambridge University Press, 2013, pp 102–114Google Scholar

42 MPNet: Program for the Simulation and Estimation of (p*) Exponential Random Graph Models for Multilevel Networks [computer program], version 1.04. Melbourne, Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, 2014Google Scholar

43 Harvey SB, Hatch SL, Jones M, et al.: Coming home: social functioning and the mental health of UK Reservists on return from deployment to Iraq or Afghanistan. Ann Epidemiol 2011; 21:666–672Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

44 Yang J, Yao S, Zhu X, et al.: The impact of stress on depressive symptoms is moderated by social support in Chinese adolescents with subthreshold depression: a multi-wave longitudinal study. J Affect Disord 2010; 127:113–121Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45 Kashdan TB, Elhai JD, Frueh BC: Anhedonia and emotional numbing in combat veterans with PTSD. Behav Res Ther 2006; 44:457–467Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

46 Litz BT, Gray MJ: Emotional numbing in posttraumatic stress disorder: current and future research directions. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2002; 36:198–204Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47 Rhoads J, Pearman T, Rick S: Clinical presentation and therapeutic interventions for posttraumatic stress disorder post-Katrina. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 2007; 21:249–256Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

48 Ehlers A, Clark DM: A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav Res Ther 2000; 38:319–345Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49 Pitman RK, Shalev AY, Orr SP: Posttraumatic stress disorder: emotion, conditioning, and memory, in The New Cognitive Neuroscience, 2nd ed. Edited by Corbetta MD, Gazzaniga M. New York, Plenum Press, 2000Google Scholar

50 Foa EB, Riggs DS, Gershuny BS: Arousal, numbing, and intrusion: symptom structure of PTSD following assault. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:116–120LinkGoogle Scholar

51 O’Connor TG, Cheng H, Dunn J, et al.: Factors moderating change in depressive symptoms in women following separation: findings from a community study in England. Psychol Med 2005; 35:715–724Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

52 Rubin GJ, Brewin CR, Greenberg N, et al.: Psychological and behavioural reactions to the bombings in London on 7 July 2005: cross sectional survey of a representative sample of Londoners. BMJ 2005; 331:606Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

53 Bearman PS: Generalized exchange. Am J Sociol 1997; 102:1383–1415CrossrefGoogle Scholar

54 Angus VC, Entwistle VA, Emslie MJ, et al.: The requirement for prior consent to participate on survey response rates: a population-based survey in Grampian. BMC Health Serv Res 2003; 3:21Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

55 Armstrong D, Kline-Rogers E, Jani SM, et al.: Potential impact of the HIPAA privacy rule on data collection in a registry of patients with acute coronary syndrome. Arch Intern Med 2005; 165:1125–1129Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

56 McCarthy DB, Shatin D, Drinkard CR, et al.: Medical records and privacy: empirical effects of legislation. Health Serv Res 1999; 34:417–425MedlineGoogle Scholar

57 Junghans C, Feder G, Hemingway H, et al.: Recruiting patients to medical research: double blind randomised trial of “opt-in” versus “opt-out” strategies. BMJ 2005; 331:940Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

58 Harris MA, Levy AR, Teschke KE: Personal privacy and public health: potential impacts of privacy legislation on health research in Canada. Can J Public Health 2008; 99:293–296Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

59 Robins G: Doing Social Network Research: Network-Based Research Design for Social Scientists. Los Angeles, Sage, 2015Google Scholar