Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000299

Abstract. Empirical evidence suggests that the color red acts like an implicit avoidance cue in food contexts. Thus specific colors seem to guide the implicit evaluation of food items. We built upon this research by investigating the implicit meaning of color (red vs. green) in an approach-avoidance task with healthy and unhealthy food items. Thus, we examined the joint evaluative effects of color and food: Participants had to categorize food items by approach-avoidance reactions, according to their healthfulness. Items were surrounded by task-irrelevant red or green circles. We found that the implicit meaning of the traffic light colors influenced participants’ reactions to the food items. The color red (compared to green) facilitated automatic avoidance reactions to unhealthy foods. By contrast, approach behavior toward healthy food items was not moderated by color. Our findings suggest that traffic light colors can act as implicit cues that guide automatic behavioral reactions to food.

References

  • Balcombe, K., Fraser, I. & Falco, S. D. (2010). Traffic lights and food choice: A choice experiment examining the relationship between nutritional food labels and price. Food Policy, 35, 211–220. doi: 10.1016/j.foodpol.2009.12.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bellizzi, J. A. & Hite, R. E. (1992). Environmental color, consumer feelings, and purchase likelihood. Psychology and Marketing, 9, 347–363. doi: 10.1002/mar.4220090502 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brignell, C., Griffiths, T., Bradley, B. P. & Mogg, K. (2009). Attentional and approach biases for pictorial food cues. Influence of external eating. Appetite, 52, 299–306. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.10.007 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bruno, N., Martani, M., Corsini, C. & Oleari, C. (2013). The effect of the color red on consuming food does not depend on achromatic (Michelson) contrast and extends to rubbing cream on the skin. Appetite, 71, 307–313. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.08.012 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Chen, M. & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 215–224. doi: 10.1177/0146167299025002007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. doi: 10.4324/9780203771587 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Craeynest, M., Crombez, G., De Houwer, J., Deforche, B., Tanghe, A. & De Bourdeaudhuij, I. (2005). Explicit and implicit attitudes towards food and physical activity in childhood obesity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1111–1120. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.07.007 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Eder, A. B. & Hommel, B. (2013). Anticipatory control of approach and avoidance: An ideomotor approach. Emotion Review, 5, 275–279. doi: 10.1177/1754073913477505 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eder, A. B. & Rothermund, K. (2008). When do motor behaviors (mis)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 262–281. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.262 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A. J., Greitemeyer, T. & Pazda, A. D. (2013). Women’s use of red clothing as a sexual signal in intersexual interaction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 599–602. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.10.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A. J. & Maier, M. A. (2007). Color and psychological functioning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 250–254. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00514.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A. J. & Niesta, D. (2008). Romantic red: Red enhances men’s attraction to women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1150–1164. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1150 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G. & Buchner, A. (2007). GPower 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Feltman, R. & Elliot, A. J. (2011). The influence of red on perceptions of relative dominance and threat in a competitive context. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33, 308–314. First citation in articleMedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Genschow, O., Reutner, L. & Wänke, M. (2012). The color red reduces snack food and soft drink intake. Appetite, 58, 699–702. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.12.023 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Gigerenzer, G. & Gaissmaier, W. (2011). Heuristic decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 451–482. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145346 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Grunert, K. G. & Wills, J. M. (2007). A review of European research on consumer response to nutrition information on food labels. Journal of Public Health, 15, 385–399. doi: 10.1007/s10389-007-0101-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Houben, K., Roefs, A. & Jansen, A. (2010). Guilty pleasures. Implicit preferences for high calorie food in restrained eating. Appetite, 55, 18–24. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.03.003 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Kliger, D. & Gilad, D. (2012). Red light, green light: Color priming in financial decisions. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 41, 738–745. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2012.07.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Koenigstorfer, J., Groeppel-Klein, A. & Kamm, F. (2014). Healthy food decision making in response to traffic light color-coded nutrition labeling. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 33, 65–77. doi: 10.1509/jppm.12.091 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krieglmeyer, R., De Houwer, J. & Deutsch, R. (2013). On the nature of automatically triggered approach-avoidance behavior. Emotion Review, 5, 280–284. doi: 10.1177/1754073913477501 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lichtenfeld, S., Elliot, A. J., Maier, M. A. & Pekrun, R. (2012). Fertile green: Green facilitates creative performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 784–797. doi: 10.1177/0146167212436611 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Maier, M. A., Barchfeld, P., Elliot, A. J. & Pekrun, R. (2009). Context specificity of implicit preferences: The case of human preference for red. Emotion, 9, 734–738. doi: 10.1037/a0016818 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Mehta, R. & Zhu, R. (2009). Blue or red? Exploring the effect of color on cognitive task performances. Science, 323, 1226–1229. doi: 10.1037/e621092012-121 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Meier, B. P., D’Agostino, P. R., Elliot, A. J., Maier, M. A. & Wilkowski, B. M. (2012). Color in context: Psychological context moderates the influence of red on approach- and avoidance-motivated behavior. PLoS One, 7, 1–5. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040333 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moller, A. C., Elliot, A. J. & Maier, M. A. (2009). Basic hue-meaning associations. Emotion, 9, 898–902. doi: 10.1037/a0017811 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Papies, E. K., Stroebe, W. & Aarts, H. (2009). Who likes it more? Restrained eaters’ implicit attitudes towards food. Appetite, 53, 279–287. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.07.001 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Paulus, A. & Wentura, D. (2014). Threatening Joy: Approach and avoidance behaviors to emotions are influenced by the group membership of the expresser. Cognition & Emotion, 28, 656–677. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.849659 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Perkins, A. & Forehand, M. (2010). Implicit social cognition and indirect measures in consumer behavior. In B. GawronskiB. PayneEds., Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 535–547). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Popkin, B. M., Duffey, K. & Gordon-Larsen, P. (2005). Environmental influences on food choice, physical activity and energy balance. Physiology & Behavior, 86, 603–613. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.08.051 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Roefs, A. & Jansen, A. (2002). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward high-fat foods in obesity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 517–521. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.111.3.517 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Roefs, A., Quaedackers, L., Werrij, M. Q., Wolters, G., Havermans, R., Nederkoorn, C., … Jansen, A. (2006). The environment influences whether high-fat foods are associated with palatable or with unhealthy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 715–736. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.05.007 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Schuldt, J. P. (2013). Does green mean healthy? Nutrition label color affects perceptions of healthfulness. Health Communication, 28, 814–821. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2012.725270 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Schwarz, S. & Singer, M. (2013). Romantic red revisited: Red enhances men’s attraction to young, but not menopausal women. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 161–164. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.08.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Setchell, J. M. & Wickings, E. J. (2005). Dominance, status signals and coloration in male mandrils (Mandrillus sphinx). Ethology, 111, 25–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.01054.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Solarz, A. K. (1960). Latency of instrumental responses as a function of compatibility with the meaning of eliciting verbal signs. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 239–245. doi: 10.1037/h0047274 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Tobler, C., Visschers, V. H. M. & Siegrist, M. (2011). Eating green. Consumers’ willingness to adopt ecological food consumption behaviors. Appetite, 57, 674–682. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.08.010 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Veenstra, E. M. & de Jong, P. J. (2010). Restrained eaters show enhanced automatic approach tendencies towards food. Appetite, 55, 30–36. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.03.007 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Veenstra, E. M. & De Jong, P. J. (2011). Reduced automatic motivational orientation towards food in restricting anorexia nervosa. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 708–718. doi: 10.1037/a0023926 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Wentura, D., Rothermund, K. & Bak, P. (2000). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of approach- and avoidance-related social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1024–1037. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.6.1024 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Wiers, R. W., Houben, K., Roefs, A., de Jong, P., Hofmann, W. & Stacy, A. W. (2010). Implicit cognition in health psychology: Why common sense goes out the window. In B. GawronskiB. K. PayneEds., Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 463–488). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wilcox, R. R. (1997). Introduction to robust estimation and hypothesis testing. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wood, W. & Neal, D. T. (2009). The habitual consumer. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19, 579–592. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2009.08.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yuen, K. K. (1974). The two-sample trimmed t for unequal population variances. Biometrika, 61, 165–170. doi: 10.2307/2334299 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar