Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000006

Contexts are sometimes informative about relationships that occur within them and sometimes not. The goal of this experiment was to determine the effect of that information value on the context-specificity of learning. Participants performed an instrumental task within a computer game in which they defended different Andalucía beaches (contexts) by destroying several attackers (planes or tanks) by clicking on them (responses) with the mouse. A colored sensor (discriminative stimulus) indicated to participants which attacker could be destroyed in a given trial – that is, which of the instrumental responses would be reinforced. Three groups of participants received training on a discrimination between two discriminative stimuli (X and Y) in Context A. The discrimination was reversed in Context B for Group I (informative). Group NI1 received the same X-Y discrimination in Context B. Group NI2 did not receive training with X and Y in Context B. Additionally, participants received training with cue Z in Context A, which consistently signaled the same outcome. A single test trial with Z revealed a lower response rate in Context B than in Context A in Group I, while no differences across contexts were found in Groups NI1 and NI2. Results suggest that when the context is informative about relationships within the experimental setting, even those relationships for which the context is not informative become context-dependent.

References

  • Abad, M. J. F. , Ramos-Álvarez, M. M. , Rosas, J. M. (2009). Partial reinforcement and context switch effects in human predictive learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 174–188). First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Domains of recollection. Psychological Review, 89, 708–792. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bonardi, C. , Honey, R. C. , Hall, G. (1990). Context specificity of conditioning in flavour-aversion learning: Extinction and blocking tests. Animal Learning & Behavior, 18, 229–237. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bouton, M. E. (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 80–99. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bouton, M. E. (1997). Signals for whether versus when an event will occur. In M. E. Bouton, M. S. Fanselow, (Eds.), Learning, motivation and cognition: The functional behaviourism of Robert C. Bolles (pp. 385–409). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bouton, M. E. , Bolles, R. C. (1979). Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear. Learning and Motivation, 10, 445–466. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bouton, M. E. , Ricker, S. T. (1994). Renewal of extinguished responding in a second context. Animal Learning & Behavior, 22, 317–324. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bouton, M. E. , Sunsay, C. (2001). Contextual control of appetitive conditioning: Influence of a contextual stimulus generated by a partial reinforcement procedure. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54B, 109–125. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bouton, M. E. , Swartzentruber, D. (1986). Analysis of the associative and occasion-setting properties of contexts participating in a Pavlovian discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 12, 333–350. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Crump, M. J. C. , Vaquero, J. M. M. , Milliken, B. (2008). Context-specific learning and control: The roles of awareness, task relevance, and relative salience. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 22–36. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Eich, J. E. (1985). Context, memory, and integrated item/context imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 764–770. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gámez, A. M. , Rosas, J. M. (2005). Transfer of stimulus control across instrumental responses is attenuated by extinction in human instrumental conditioning. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 5, 207–222. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gámez, A. , Rosas, J. M. (2007). Associations in human instrumental conditioning. Learning and Motivation, 38(3), 242–261. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hall, G. , Honey, R. C. (1989). Contextual effects in conditioning, latent inhibition, and habituation: Associative and retrieval functions of contextual cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 15, 232–241. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hall, G. , Honey, R. C. (1990). Context-specific conditioning in the conditioned-emotional-response procedure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 271–278. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • León, S. P. , Abad, M. J. F. , & Rosas, J. M. (in press). The effect of context change on simple acquisition disappears with increased training. Psicológica. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Myers, C. , Gluck, M. (1994). Context, conditioning and hippocampal re-representation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108(5), 835–847. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Nelson, J. B. (2002). Context specificity of excitation and inhibition in ambiguous stimuli. Learning and Motivation, 33, 284–310. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nelson, J. B. , Callejas-Aguilera, J. E. (2007). The role of interference produced by conflicting associations in contextual control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 33, 314–326. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Neumann, D. L. (2007). The resistance of renewal to instructions that devalue the role of contextual cues in a conditioned suppression task with humans. Learning and Motivation, 38, 105–127. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Paredes-Olay, C. , Rosas, J. M. (1999). Within-subjects extinction and renewal in predictive judgments. Psicológica, 20, 195–210. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pearce, J. M. (1987). A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Review, 94, 61–73. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Pineño, O. , Miller, R. R. (2004). Signalling a change in cue-outcome relations in human associative learning. Learning & Behavior, 32, 360–375. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Preston, G. C. , Dickinson, A. , Mackintosh, N. J. (1986). Contextual conditional discriminations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 38B, 217–237. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rosas, J. M. , Bouton, M. E. (1998). Context change and retention interval have additive, rather than interactive, effects after taste aversion extinction. Psychonomic Bulletin y Review, 5, 79–83. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosas, J. M. , Callejas-Aguilera, J. E. (2006). Context switch effects on acquisition and extinction in human predictive learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 461–474. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Rosas, J. M. , Callejas-Aguilera, J. E. (2007). Acquisition of a conditioned taste aversion becomes context dependent when it is learned alter extinction. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 9–15. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Rosas, J. M. , Callejas-Aguilera, J. E. , Ramos-Álvarez, M. M. , Abad, M. J. F. (2006). Revision of retrieval theory of forgetting: What does make information context-specific? International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 6, 147–166. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rosas, J. M. , García-Gutiérrez, A. , Callejas-Aguilera, J. E. (2006). Effects of context change upon retrieval of first and second-learned information in human predictive learning. Psicológica, 27, 35–56. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rosas, J. M. , García-Gutiérrez, A. , Callejas-Aguilera, J. E. (2007). AAB and ABA renewal as a function of the number of extinction trials in conditioned taste aversion. Psicológica, 28, 129–150. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rosas, J. M. , Vila, N. J. , Lugo, M. , & López, L. (2001). Combined effect of context change and retention interval upon interference in causality judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 153–164. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Thomas, B. L. , Larsen, N. , & Ayres, J. J. B. (2003). Role of context similarity in ABA, ABC, and AAB renewal paradigms: Implications for theories of renewal and for treating human phobias. Learning and Motivation, 34, 410–436. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tulving, E. , Osler, S. (1968). Effectiveness of retrieval cues in memory for words. Journal of Psychology, 77, 593–601. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar