Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.56.6.409

Completing a goal directed behavior in a safe and efficient manner requires that a perceiver-actor is sensitive to the various constraints on performing that behavior and adjust his or her movements accordingly. When attempting to pass under a barrier, people adjust their ducking behavior based on the likelihood and potential costs of a collision (van der Meer, 1997). In three experiments, we investigated whether participants are sensitive to geometric (standing height), kinetic-kinematic (anticipated movement speed), and intentional (material properties of the barrier) constraints on passing under a barrier even before attempting to perform this behavior. Although Experiment 1 failed to show that anticipated movement speed influenced perception of whether a barrier could be passed under, Experiment 2 found that this factor influences willingness to attempt the behavior. Experiments 3a and 3b found that the material properties of the barrier itself also influence willingness to attempt the behavior. Together, the results highlight the contribution of geometric, kinetic-kinematic, and intentional constraints to perception.

References

  • Alexander, R. M. (1992). The human machine: How the body works. New York, NY: Columbia University Press. Google Scholar

  • Bhalla, M. , Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1076–1096. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Google Scholar

  • Fajen, B. R. (2007). Rapid recalibration based on optic flow in visually guided braking. Experimental Brain Research, 183, 61–71. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor control system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 381–391. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Google Scholar

  • Higuchi, T. , Cinelli, M. E. , Grieg, M. A. , Patla, A. E. (2006). Locomotion through apertures when wider space for locomotion is necessary: Adaptation to artificially altered body states. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 50–59. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Hommel, B. , Muesseler, J. , Aschersleben, G. , Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding: A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Konczak, J. , Meeuwsen, H. , & Cress, M. E. (1992). Changing affordances in stair climbing: The perception of maximum climbability in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 691–697. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Lee, C. R. , Farley, C. T. (1998). Determinants of the center of mass trajectory in human walking and running. Journal of Experimental Biology, 201, 2935–2944. MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Malek, E. A. , Wagman, J. B. (2008). Kinetic potential influences visual and remote haptic perception of affordances for standing on an inclined surface. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1813–1826. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Mark, L. S. (1987). Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: A study of sitting and stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 361–370. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Mechsner, F. (2004). A psychological approach to human voluntary movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 355–370. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Noë, A. (2004). Action in perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Google Scholar

  • Oudejans, R. , Michaels, C. F. , Bakker, F. C. , Dolné, M. A. (1996). The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: Perception of catchableness of fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 879–891. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 110–122. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Proffitt, D. R. , Stefanucci, J. , Banton, T. , & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in perceiving distance. Psychological Science, 14, 106–112. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Regia-Corte, T. , Wagman, J. B. (2008). Perception of affordances for standing on an inclined surface depends on height of center of mass. Experimental Brain Research, 191, 25–35. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Shaw, R. E. , Kinsella-Shaw, J. (1988). Ecological mechanics: A physical geometry for intentional constraints. Human Movement Science, 7, 155–200. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shaw, R. E. , Turvey, M. T. (1999). Ecological foundations of cognition II: Degrees of freedom and conserved quantities in animal-environment systems. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 111–124. Google Scholar

  • Turvey, M. T. (1992). Affordances and prospective control: An outline of the ontology. Ecological Psychology, 4, 173–187. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Turvey, M. T. (2004). Space and its perception: The first and final frontier. Ecological Psychology, 16, 25–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van der Meer, A. L. H. (1997). Visual guidance of passing under a barrier. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 149–157. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wagman, J. B. (2008). Perception-action as reciprocal, continuous, and prospective. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 219–220. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wagman, J. B. , Malek, E. A. (2007). Perception of whether an object can be carried through an aperture depends on anticipated speed. Experimental Psychology, 54, 54–61. LinkGoogle Scholar

  • Wagman, J. B. , Malek, E. A. (2008). Perception of affordances for walking under a barrier from proximal and distal points of observation. Ecological Psychology, 20, 65–83. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Warren, W. H. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 683–703. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Warren, W. H. , Whang, S. (1987). Visual guidance of walking through apertures: Body scaled information for affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 371–383. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Witt, J. K. , Proffitt, D. R. , Epstein, W. (2004). Perceiving distance: A role of effort and intent. Perception, 33, 577–590. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Witt, J. K. , Proffitt, D. R. , Epstein, W. (2005). Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 880–888. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar