Skip to main content
Article

“Look at Me” – Eye Movements During Autobiographical Retrieval in Face-to-Face Interactions

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000276

Abstract. There has been a surge in social cognition and social neurosciences research comparing laboratory and real eye movements. Eye movements during the retrieval of autobiographical memories (i.e., personal memories) in laboratory situations are also receiving more attention. We compared eye movements during the retrieval of autobiographical memories using a strict laboratory design versus a design mimicking social interactions. In the first design, eye movements were recorded during autobiographical memory retrieval while participants were looking at a blank screen; in the second design, participants wore eye-tracking glasses and communicated autobiographical memories to the experimenter. Compared with the “screen” design, the “glasses” design yielded more fixations (p < .05), shorter duration of fixations (p < .001), more saccades (p < .01), and longer duration of saccades (p < .001). These findings demonstrate how eye movements during autobiographical memory retrieval differ between strict laboratory design and face-to-face interactions.

References

  • Bartoli, E., & Smorti, A. (2018). Facing the language-memory problem in the study of autobiographical memory. Integrative Psychological Behavioral Sciences, 53(3), 374–396. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-018-9434-x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bauer, P. J., Pathman, T., Inman, C., Campanella, C., & Hamann, S. (2017). Neural correlates of autobiographical memory retrieval in children and adults. Memory, 25(4), 450–466. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2016.1186699 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bluck, S., Alea, N., Habermas, T., & Rubin, D. C. (2005). A tale of three functions: The self-reported uses of autobiographical memory. Social Cognition, 23(1), 91–117. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.23.1.91.59198 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. (1992). Statistical power analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1(3), 98–101. https://doi.org/10.2307/20182143 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(4), 594–628. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • El Haj, M., Antoine, P., & Nandrino, J. L. (2016). More emotional facial expressions during episodic than during semantic autobiographical retrieval. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(2), 374–381. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0397-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • El Haj, M., Antoine, P., & Nandrino, J. L. (2017). Facial expressions triggered by imagining the future. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience, 16(4), 483–492. https://doi.org/10.3233/JIN-170030 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • El Haj, M., Boutoleau-Bretonnière, C., & Janssen, S. M. J. (2020). Eye movements of recent and remote autobiographical memories: Fewer and longer lasting fixations during the retrieval of childhood memories. Psychological Research. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01403-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • El Haj, M., Daoudi, M., Gallouj, K., Moustafa, A. A., & Nandrino, J.-L. (2018). When your face describes your memories: Facial expressions during retrieval of autobiographical memories. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 29(8), 861–872. https://doi.org/10.1515/revneuro-2018-0001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • El Haj, M., Delerue, C., Omigie, D., Antoine, P., Nandrino, J. L., & Boucart, M. (2014). Autobiographical recall triggers visual exploration. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 7(5), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.7.5.1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • El Haj, M., & Lenoble, Q. (2017). Eying the future: Eye movement in past and future thinking. Cortex, 105, 97–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.05.024 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • El Haj, M., Nandrino, J. L., Antoine, P., Boucart, M., & Lenoble, Q. (2017). Eye movement during retrieval of emotional autobiographical memories. Acta Psychologica, 174, 54–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.02.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ellis, P. D. (2010). The essential guide to effect sizes: Statistical power, meta-analysis, and the interpretation of research results, Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ferreira, F., Apel, J., & Henderson, J. M. (2008). Taking a new look at looking at nothing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 405–410. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fivush, R., Habermas, T., Waters, T. E., & Zaman, W. (2011). The making of autobiographical memory: Intersections of culture, narratives and identity. International Journal of Psychology, 46(5), 321–345. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2011.596541 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gandolphe, M. C., Nandrino, J. L., Delelis, G., Ducro, C., Lavallee, A., Saloppe, X., Moustafa, A. A., & El Haj, M. (2018). Positive facial expressions during retrieval of self-defining memories. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience, 17(3–4), 367–376. https://doi.org/10.3233/JIN-170073 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grober, E., & Buschke, H. (1987). Genuine memory deficits in dementia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 3(1), 13–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565648709540361 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hart, M. B., Vockeroth, J., Schumann, F., Bartl, K., Schneider, E., König, P., & Einhäuser, W. (2009). Gaze allocation in natural stimuli: Comparing free exploration to head-fixed viewing conditions. Visual Cognition, 17(6–7), 1132–1158. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280902812304 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ho, S., Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A. (2015). Speaking and listening with the eyes: Gaze signaling during dyadic interactions. PLoS One, 10(8), Article e0136905. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136905 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kendon, A. (1967). Some functions of gaze-direction in social interaction. Acta Psychologica, 26, 22–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(67)90005-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., Lumley, M. A., Jain, E., & Heinze, H. (2003). Age and gender differences in cardiac reactivity and subjective emotion responses to emotional autobiographical memories. Emotion, 3(2), 115–126. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.3.2.115 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Laidlaw, K. E. W., Foulsham, T., Kuhn, G., & Kingstone, A. (2011). Potential social interactions are important to social attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(14), 5548–5553. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1017022108 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lenoble, Q., Janssen, S. M. J., & El Haj, M. (2019). Don’t stare, unless you don’t want to remember: Maintaining fixation compromises autobiographical memory retrieval. Memory, 27(2), 231–238. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2018.1501068 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mahr, J., & Csibra, G. (2017). Why do we remember? The communicative function of episodic memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, 1–93. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X17000012 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111(2), 486–511. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.2.486 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robertson, S. M., Swickert, R. J., Connelly, K., & Galizio, A. (2015). Physiological reactivity during autobiographical narratives in older adults: The roles of depression and anxiety. Aging and Mental Health, 19(8), 689–697. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2014.962010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenthal, R., & DiMatteo, M. R. (2001). Meta-analysis: Recent developments in quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 59–82. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.59 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rubin, D. C. (2005). A basic-systems approach to autobiographical memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(2), 79–83. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00339.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schaefer, A., & Philippot, P. (2005). Selective effects of emotion on the phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories. Memory, 13(2), 148–160. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210344000648 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • St Jacques, P. L., Carpenter, A. C., Szpunar, K. K., & Schacter, D. L. (2018). Remembering and imagining alternative versions of the personal past. Neuropsychologia, 110, 170–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M. C., & Levine, B. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44(12), 2189–2208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.023 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Westerhof, G. J., Bohlmeijer, E., & Webster, J. D. (2010). Reminiscence and mental health: A review of recent progress in theory, research and interventions. Ageing & Society, 30(04), 697–721. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X09990328 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wu, D. W. L., Bischof, W. F., & Kingstone, A. (2014). Natural gaze signaling in a social context. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(3), 211–218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.01.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar