Skip to main content
Articles

Aging and Error Processing

Time-Frequency Analysis of Error-Related Potentials

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803.19.4.289

Abstract: Objectives: To determine age-related changes in the frequency content, temporal dynamics, and functional reactivity of error-related potentials. Methods: Two groups of subjects, young (mean age 22.5 years) and older (mean age 58.3 years) were studied with a four-choice reaction time task. Response-related potentials (RRPs) to correct and error responses were decomposed by means of wavelet transform. Time-frequency (TF) components of correct and error responses were identified. Effects of aging were evaluated for the phase-locked TF power at FCz. Results: (1) Error-related potentials in both groups were characterized by phase-locked activity from the delta (1.5-3.5 Hz) frequency band that was error-specific and was significantly enhanced for errors. (2) In contrast to young adults, no pronounced phase-locked theta (4-7 Hz) RRP components were generated at FCz for either correct or error responses, nor did the theta power manifest any sensitivity to error processing in older subjects. Conclusion: In older subjects, RRPs at FCz do not contain a theta TF component, thus, demonstrating that the frequency content of error-related potentials changes with aging. A common error-specific delta component is present in the two age groups, but its reactivity is significantly reduced and delayed with aging. These qualitative and quantitative differences between young and older subjects may reflect a functional suppression of the underlying frontomedial structures involved in performance and movement monitoring.

References

  • Babiloni, F. , Babiloni, C. , Carducci, F. , Fattorini, L. , Onorati, P. , Urbano, A. (1996). Spline Laplacian estimate of EEG potentials over a realistic magnetic resonance-constructed scalp surface model. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 98, 363– 373 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Band, G. , Kok, A. (2000). Age effects on response monitoring in a mental-rotation task. Biological Psychology, 51, 201– 221 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging, 17, 85– 100 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cabeza, R. , Anderson, N. , Kester, J. , McIntosh, A. (2002). Aging gracefully: Compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. NeuroImage, 17, 1394– 1402 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carter, C.S. , Braver, T.S. , Barch, D.M. , Botvinick, M.M. , Noll, D. , Cohen, J.D. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science, 280, 747– 749 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dehaene, S. , Posner, M.I. , Tucker, D.M. (1994). Localization of a neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 5, 303– 305 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Falkenstein, M. , Hohnsbein, J. , Hoormann, J. , Blanke, L. (1990). Effects of errors in choice reaction tasks on the ERP under focused and divided attention. In C.H.M. Brunia, A.W.K. Gaillard, & A. Kok (Eds.), Psychophysiological brain research 1990(pp. 192-195). Tilburg: Tilburg University Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Falkenstein, M. , Hohnsbein, J. , Hoormann, J. , Blanke, L. (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 78, 447– 455 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Falkenstein, M. , Hoormann, J. , Christ, S. , Hohnsbein, J. (2000). ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: A tutorial. Biological Psychology, 51, 87– 107 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Falkenstein, M. , Hoormann, J. , Hohnsbein, J. (2001). Changes of error-related ERPs with age. Experimental Brain Research, 138, 258– 262 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ford, J.M. (1999). Schizophrenia: The broken P300 and beyond. Psychophysiology, 36, 667– 682 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gehring, W.J. , Goss, B. , Coles, M.G.H. , Meyer, D.E. , Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4, 385– 390 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gehring, W.J. , Knight, R.T. (2000). Prefrontal-cingulate interactions in action monitoring. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 421– 423 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gratton, G. , Coles, M.G.H. , Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 468– 484 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gruber, T. , Malinowski, P. , Müller, M.M. (2004). Modulation of oscillatory brain activity and evoked potentials in a repetition priming task in the human EEG. European Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 1073– 1082 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jensen, O. , Gelfand, J. , Kounios, J. , Lisman, J.E. (2002). Oscillations in the alpha band (9-12 Hz) increase with memory load during retention in a short-term memory task. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 877– 882 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Luu, P. , Tucker, D.M. (2001). Regulating action: Alternating activation of midline frontal and motor cortical networks. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, 1295– 1306 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mallat, S. (1999). A wavelet tour of signal processing (2nd ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mathalon, D.H. , Bennett, A. , Askari, N. , Gray, E.M. , Rosenbloom, M.J. , Ford, J.M. (2003). Response-monitoring dysfunction in aging and Alzheimer's disease: An event-related potential study. Neurobiology of Aging, 24, 675– 685 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Niedermeyer, E. (1993). Maturation of the EEG: Development of waking and sleep patterns. In E. Niedermeyer & F.H. Lopes da Silva (Eds.),Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields (pp. 167-191). Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nieuwenhuis, S. , Ridderinkhof, K.R. , Talsma, D. , Coles, M.G. , Holroyd, C.B. , Kok, A. , van der Molen, M.W. (2002). A computational account of altered error processing in older age: Dopamine and the error-related negativity. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 19– 36 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nunez, P.L. , Srinivasan, R. , Westdorp, A.F. , Wijesinghe, R.S. , Tucker, D.M. , Silberstein, R.B. , Cadusch, P.J. (1997). EEG coherency. I. Statistics. reference electrode, volume conduction, Laplacians, cortical imaging, and interpretation at multiple scales. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 103, 499– 515 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perrin, F. , Pernier, J. , Bertrand, O. , Echallier, J. (1989). Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 72, 184– 187 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rabbitt, P.M.A. (1997). Introduction: Methodologies and models in the study of executive function. In P.M.A. Rabbitt (Ed.), Methodology of frontal and executive function (pp. 1-38). Hove, UK: Psychology Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2002). New visions of the aging mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 394– 400 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ridderinkhof, K.R. , Ullsperger, M. , Crone, E.A. , Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443– 447 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tallon-Baudry, C. , Bertrand, O. , Delpuech, C. , Permier, J. (1997). Oscillatory gamma-band (30-70 Hz) activity induced by a visual search task in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 722– 734 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ullsperger, M. , von Cramon, D.Y. (2001). Subprocesses of performance monitoring: A dissociation of error processing and response competition revealed by event-related fMRI and ERPs. NeuroImage, 14, 1387– 1401 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ullsperger, M. , Falkenstein, M. Eds. (2004). Errors, conflicts, and the brain. Current opinions on performance monitoring . Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Vidal, F. , Hasbroucq, T. , Grapperon, J. , Bonnet, M. (2000). Is the “error negativity” specific to errors?. Biological Psychology, 51, 109– 128 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yeung, N. , Cohen, J.D. , Botvinick, M.M. (2004). The neural basis of error detection: Conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 111, 931– 959 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yordanova, J. , Falkenstein, M. , Hohnsbein, J. , Kolev, V. (2004). Parallel systems of error processing in the brain. NeuroImage, 22, 590– 602 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yordanova, J. , Kolev, V. , Hohnsbein, J. , Falkenstein, M. (2004). Sensorimotor slowing with aging is mediated by a functional dysregulation of motor-generation processes: Evidence from high-resolution ERPs. Brain, 127, 351– 362 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar