There have been numerous reports in recent years pointing to serious concerns about the mental health and wellbeing of children and young people and specifically to the increase in the incidence of anxiety (e.g., Bor et al.
2014; Collishaw
2014; Gore et al.
2011). Numerous reasons for the increase in anxiety have been speculated including the use of social media, increased exposure to violence, trauma, conflict, and excessive pressure to succeed at school (Davey
2018). With regards to the latter, results from the 2015 PISA survey, for instance, showed that 55% of adolescent students from participating OECD countries indicated frequently worrying about poor performance in school even if they had prepared well for a test (OECD
2017). Studies, however, are only beginning to empirically address possible reasons for the contemporary increases in anxiety (Viner et al.
2019). In the present study, the link between excessive academic pressures (measured using test anxiety as an indicator of excessive perceived academic pressure) and subsequent risk for emotion disorders (i.e., anxiety and depression) is examined within a sample of students aged 16–19 years in an upper tier of secondary education (referred to colloquially in England as ‘6th form’) studying for examinations on which university entrance is based. Test anxiety, school-related wellbeing, and risk for emotion disorders were measured over two waves near the beginning and end of the academic school year.
Test Anxiety: An Indicator of Perceived Academic Pressure
Test anxiety is experienced when a person appraises performance-evaluative situations as threatening. This can refer to specific episodes of state anxiety or, the approach we adopt in the present study, enduring trait-like individual differences in the threat appraisal of performance-evaluative situations. In the Self-referent Executive Processing (S-REF) model (Zeidner and Matthews
2005), elevated test anxiety arises from the negative self-beliefs (e.g., self-doubt and anticipated failure) combining with executive processing strategies (including coping and metacognition) that magnify threat, and maladaptive person-situation interactions (e.g., avoiding opportunities to improve). Various studies have provided strong empirical support for the various processes outlined in the S-REF model (e.g., Matthews et al.
1999; Putwain
2019). Meta-analyses have evidenced a negative relation between test anxiety, especially its cognitive component (worries about failure and its consequences), and educational achievement (e.g., Hembree
1988; von der Embse et al.
2018). This negative relation is often used to justify the relevance of test anxiety as a construct of practical and theoretical significance. An often overlooked outcome within the academic literature is how test anxiety may relate to student wellbeing and mental health.
Test Anxiety, Subjective, and School-Related Wellbeing
There is a burgeoning literature on the wellbeing of children and young people. The term ‘subjective wellbeing’, however, is used in a hazy and ill-defined way (Statham and Chase
2010). In the present study, we define
subjective wellbeing as the presence of happiness, satisfaction, and positive emotions (Diener
1994; Diener et al.
2003). Subjective wellbeing could refer to one’s life in general or to specific elements of one’s life, such as schooling (Diener et al.
2018). Hascher (
2003,
2008) specifically defines school-related wellbeing as the perceived balance between three positive and three negative aspects of school life. The three positive elements are: Attitudes towards school (e.g., valuing school), enjoyment in school (e.g., receiving a good grade), and academic self-concept (e.g., meeting school standards). The three negative elements are: Worries about school (e.g., poor academic progress), physical complaints in school (e.g., headaches and dizziness), and social problems at school (e.g., poor relationships with peers).
High test anxiety could potentially lower wellbeing directly via worry about failing tests and examinations, and indirectly through tainting the positive elements of school life (e.g., reduced confidence in one’s ability and positive attitudes towards school). Few studies, however, have investigated relations between test anxiety and subjective or school-related wellbeing. In cross-sectional studies, negative relations between test anxiety and school-related wellbeing were reported by Hascher (
2007;
rs −0.15 to −0.46) and by Putwain et al. (
2020a,
b;
rs = −0.03 to −0.33) in secondary school students, and for subjective wellbeing by Lin and McKeachie (
1971;
rs −0.26 to −0.59) in undergraduate students. In a two-wave study of upper-secondary school students, the cognitive dimension of test anxiety was negatively related to subjective wellbeing twelve months later (positive mood: β = −0.21; life satisfaction: β = −0.12) having controlled for autoregressive and concurrent relations (Steinmayr et al.
2016).
The aforementioned studies suggest that test anxiety may indeed lead to lower school-related wellbeing. It is, however, plausible to assume that test anxiety and school-related wellbeing are related in a bidirectional fashion. That is, not only might higher test anxiety lead to lower wellbeing over time, but lower wellbeing might also lead to higher test anxiety over time. Theoretically speaking, negative self-beliefs (a negative element of school life) are a key antecedent of high test anxiety in the S-REF model and there is substantial evidence to suggest that lower academic self-concept predicts subsequent higher test anxiety (e.g., Arens et al.
2017; Putwain et al.
2020a,
b). Furthermore, a tendency to experience somatic complaints (another negative element of school life) is related to higher test anxiety (Beidel
1988; Chin et al.
2017), and could become a precursor to test anxiety (see Hagtvet and Benson
1997).
In short, there are good reasons for assuming that lower school-related wellbeing, as defined by Hascher (
2007), would predict subsequent higher test anxiety. In the aforementioned study by Steinmayr et al. (
2016), however, the relations between subjective wellbeing to test anxiety were not statistically significant. A specific school-related measure of wellbeing, however, might be expected to show stronger relations with subsequent test anxiety than a general measure of subjective wellbeing that is concerned with both school and non-school influences. Accordingly, in the present study we used a specific measure of school-related wellbeing.
Test Anxiety and Emotion Disorder
Emotion disorder is an omnibus term used for anxiety and mood disorders that overlap in symptomology and etiology, and are underpinned by common biological, psychological, and experiential, vulnerabilities (Barlow et al.
2016). Emotion disorders are discrete (i.e., diagnosed as absent or present) whereas test anxiety is represented on a continuum. Persons with high scores on trait test anxiety (and also general trait anxiety; see Chambers et al.
2004), however, may show levels of distress, dysfunction, and symptomology, that correspond to those of emotion disorders (Gerwing et al.
2015; Pekrun and Loderer
2020). A binary approach to the conceptualisation of discrete emotion disorders is not universally accepted and there may be many benefits to adopting a dimensional approach to emotion disorders (e.g., Goldberg
2000; Shear et al.
2007). Although DSM-5 includes dimensional assessment of emotion disorders (e.g., Möller et al.
2014; Möller and Bögels
2016), the assumption of categorigically distinct emotion disorders remains.
The integrative network approach proposes that symptoms of anxiety disorders are represented as nodes in distributed systems of associations (Hereen and McNally
2016,
2018). Central nodes are those with more dense connections to other symptoms and accordingly occupy an influential role. Activation of these nodes will spread to associated nodes more quickly and powerfully than nodes with fewer, and less dense, connections to others and play an important role in the development and maintenance of a disorder. The integrated network approach can not only account for comorbidity across emotion disorders by reconceptualising symptoms as systems of clustered associations but also highlights the role of trait anxiety as risk factor for developing an anxiety disorder through activating central nodes (Hereen et al.
2018).
In highly test anxious elementary school children (Test Anxiety Scale for Children [TASC] scores at the 40th scale percentile for males and 53rd scale percentile for females), 60% of 3rd to 6th Grade students (Beidel and Turner
1988) and 57% of students with a mean age of 10 years (Beidel et al.
1994) met DSM criteria for an anxiety disorder following diagnostic interview. Furthermore, Weems et al. (
2010) showed that highly anxious 4th to 8th grade students (TASC scores ≥ 44th scale percentile) reported higher anxiety (
ds = 0.71 to 0.95) and depression (
d = 0.86) symptoms than their low test anxious students (TASC scores < 44th scale percentile).
In highly test anxious secondary school students (top 5% of the distribution of TASC scores; 60th scale percentile for males and 77th scale percentile for females), 61% of students in Grades 9 and 10 (aged 15 years) met DSM criteria for an anxiety disorder following diagnostic interview (King et al.
1995). Furthermore, large differences in anxiety (
d = 2.06) and depression (
d = 1.56) symptoms were shown compared to those reporting in the bottom 5% of the distribution of TASC scores. In a sample of secondary school students aged 9 to 16 years, Warren and colleagues (
1996) found that that those scoring in the upper 60th scale percentile of the Test Anxiety Inventory (TAI) reported significant higher anxiety (
ds = 0.72 to 2.67) and depression (
ds = 0.57 to 1.88) symptoms than their low test anxiety counterparts (33rd scale percentile). Owens et al. (
2012) found, in a sample of secondary school students aged 12–13 years that the cognitive (Worry) component of the Children’s Test Anxiety Scale strongly correlated with RCADs depression scores (
r = 0.62).
More recently, Herzer et al. (
2014) found that among a sample of adults aged 20 to 25 years, studying for university or vocational examinations, those reporting in the upper 66th scale percentile of the German TAI were diagnosed (with 96.6% accuracy) with an emotion disorder following diagnostic interview. Putwain et al. (
2020c) reported that test anxiety was positively correlated (
rs = 0.13 to 0.46 for the different components of test anxiety) with an elevated risk of developing an emotion disorder (assessed via the Social, Academic, and Emotional, Behavior Risk Screener [SAEBRS; von der Embse et al.
2016]) in a sample of secondary school students aged 11–19 years.
Although the aforementioned studies used different criteria with which to establish the cut-point for ‘high’ test anxiety on continuous scales with no rationale or explanation, they do provide compelling evidence that high levels of test anxiety can overlap with emotion disorders. These studies do not, however, address questions of directionality. That is, the question remains to what extent test anxiety may predispose students to a greater risk of developing emotional disorders and/or vice versa. Following the integrative network approach, we propose that bidirectional relations between test anxiety and enhanced risk of developing emotion disorders (henceforth referred to as
emotion risk for brevity) are plausible. Test anxious cognitions (e.g., worrying about negative outcomes), for instance, could generalise from testing to social situations through activating networks of associated anxiety and depression symptom nodes (see Hereen et al.
2018). Similarly, cognitive biases that underpin emotion disorders (e.g., selective abstraction, catastrophising, and overgeneralisation) could influence the executive processes specified in the S-REF model to magnify fear of failure and increase subsequent test anxiety.
The Dual-Factor Model of Mental Health
In the present study we have theorised separate bidirectional relations between test anxiety and school-related wellbeing, and between test anxiety and emotion risk. Our reasoning for considering school-related wellbeing
and emotion risk was informed by the Dual-Factor Model of Mental Health (DFM: Suldo and Shaffer
2008). The DFM is based on the premise that mental health is not simply the absence of a mental disorder, but must be accompanied by the presence of subjective markers of wellbeing (Diener et al.
2002). Wellbeing and psychopathology are related but distinct constructs and therefore it is possible that some persons may experience moderate or even high subjective wellbeing despite suffering from a mental disorder and, conversely, low subjective wellbeing in the absence of any mental disorder (e.g., Antaramian et al.
2010; Greenspoon and Saklofske
2001; Lyons et al.
2013; Suldo et al.
2016).
Hence, if test anxiety does show bidirectional relations with greater emotion risk, as theorised, it cannot be assumed this would necessarily predict lower school-related wellbeing and vice versa. Accordingly, we included both school-related wellbeing
and emotion risk in the present study. Analysing data in a single analytic model presents an opportunity to also assess bidirectional relations between school-related wellbeing and emotion risk. Notwithstanding the rationale underpinning the DFM, all things being equal, we hypothesised that higher emotion risk would be related to lower school-related wellbeing and vice versa. The presence of an emotion disorder is likely to contribute to the negative aspects of school life, namely, worries about school, physical complaints in school, and social problems that contribute to school-related wellbeing, and vice versa (e.g., Crawford and Manassis
2011; Millings et al.
2012; Oldfield et al.
2016).
Aim of the Present Study
The aim of the present study was to investigate bidirectional relations between test anxiety, school-related wellbeing, and emotion risk, in a sample of upper secondary school students following a programme of study for university entrance examinations. Since previous studies have shown differences in test anxiety, subjective wellbeing, and emotion disorders for gender, age, and economic deprivation (e.g., Asher and Aderka
2018; −
2007; Rees and Bradshaw
2016), we controlled for the influence of demographic variables in our analyses. The following hypotheses were tested:
Hypothesis 1: Higher test anxiety will be related to greater subsequent emotion risk and lower school-related wellbeing.
Hypothesis 2: Higher school-related wellbeing will be related to lower subsequent test anxiety and lower emotion risk.
Hypothesis 3: Higher emotion risk will be related to higher subsequent test anxiety and lower school-related wellbeing.
Discussion
The aim of this study was to examine bidirectional relations between test anxiety, school-related wellbeing, and emotion risk, in a sample of adolescent students. Self-reported data for test anxiety, school-related wellbeing, and emotion risk were collected twice from participants in a tier of upper secondary education (referred to as 6th form) over a single academic year (separated by approximately seven months). Bidirectional relations were examined using a structural equation model. After controlling for the concurrent relations, gender, and age, higher T1 test anxiety predicted higher T2 emotion risk but was unrelated to T2 school-related wellbeing (partly supporting Hypothesis 1). Higher T1 school-related wellbeing was related to lower T2 test anxiety and T2 emotion risk (supporting Hypothesis 2). Finally, higher T1 emotion risk was related to higher T2 test anxiety, and lower T2 school-related wellbeing (supporting Hypothesis 3). In summary, bidirectional relations were found between test anxiety and emotion risk, and between school-related wellbeing and emotion risk. Test anxiety and school-related wellbeing were related in a unidirectional rather than bidirectional fashion.
Based on the integrative network approach (Hereen and McNally
2016,
2018) and S-REF model (Zeidner and Matthews
2005) we theorized that test anxiety would be related to emotion risk in a bidirectional fashion. Test anxiety might predispose persons to emotion risk by acting as ‘hidden generators’ that activate related clusters of anxiety and depression symptoms and the cognitive biases that underpin emotion disorders would also lead to enhanced threat of failure in tests and exams. Extant research has shown that test anxiety and emotion disorders are related (e.g., King et al.
1995; Owens et al.
2012; Weems et al.
2010) but has yet to examine the question of directionality. The findings of the present study address this gap in the literature and reveal bidirectional relations; higher test anxiety was related to greater subsequent emotion risk, and greater emotion risk was related to higher subsequent test anxiety.
It is notable that the path from test anxiety to emotion risk (β = 0.34) was considerably stronger than the path from emotion risk to test anxiety (β = 0.10). This is perhaps not surprising given that test anxiety was conceptualized as being trait-like and was highly stable across the two waves of measurement in the present study. In comparison, emotion risk was conceptualized as being more state-like (participants were asked to report on the last four weeks) and less stable; the autoregressive path across the two waves of measurement for emotion risk was not statistically significant. In addition, the second wave of measurement took place approximately one month prior to end-of-Year 12 examinations which have a significant bearing on being made an offer of university study. This may have contributed to an elevated emotion risk in highly test anxious persons. Test anxiety might therefore be considered a substantive risk factor for the development of subsequent emotion disorders.
Based on the DFM (Suldo and Shaffer
2008), we reasoned that school-related wellbeing should be considered as distinct to emotion risk. That is, low school-related wellbeing cannot necessarily be inferred solely from the presence of emotion risk. We theorized that negative elements of school-related wellbeing (e.g., negative beliefs about school and somatic complaints) could become a precursor to greater test anxiety and that higher test anxiety could taint the positive elements of school life, leading to lower wellbeing. Previous research using cross-sectional designs has confirmed negative relations between test anxiety and school-related and subjective wellbeing (Putwain et al.
2020c; Hascher
2007; Lin and McKeachie
1971). Only one study has examined bidirectional relations, however. In this study, higher cognitive test anxiety predicted lower subjective wellbeing but not vice versa (Steinmayr et al.
2016). We reasoned that relations with test anxiety would be higher with a school-specific measure of wellbeing than a general measure of subjective wellbeing.
Our data did not support reciprocal relations. Higher school-related wellbeing predicted lower test anxiety, but test anxiety was unrelated to subsequent wellbeing. Thus, negative elements of school life, such as negative beliefs about school, may indeed contribute to subsequent test anxiety. This is consistent with findings from other studies showing how negative beliefs contribute to subsequent text anxiety (e.g., Arens et al.
2017; − et al.
2020a,
b), although not those of Steinmayr et al. (
2016). It is important, however, not to overstate this finding as the relation was only small (β = −0.06). Although we theorized that higher test anxiety would taint positive aspects of school life and lead to lower wellbeing, this was not supported by the data and contradicts Steinmayr et al.’s (
2016) finding.
Since school-related wellbeing is malleable and responsive to environmental conditions in school, such as the quality of peer and teacher relationships (e.g., Goswami
2012; Lee and Yoo
2015), it is possible that relations between test anxiety and school-related wellbeing were temporally sensitive. Test anxiety may influence immediate school-related wellbeing but over a longer term period, such as the seven month interval used in the present study, positive aspects of wellbeing may reassert. This may also partly explain the contrasted findings with Steinmayr et al. (
2016) who used a more trait-like measure of subjective being. Experience sampling approaches (see Zirkel et al.
2015), using real-time measures of school-related wellbeing, would be an effective approach to map relations with test anxiety over time. It likely that relations between test anxiety and school-related wellbeing would differ depending on the emotion regulation strategies used (see Balzarotti et al.
2016) and could be a fruitful avenue for future research studies. It is clear, however, that simply using a school-specific measure of wellbeing does not result in larger relations than a more general measure of subjective wellbeing.
The inclusion of school-related wellbeing, as well as emotion risk, also offered the opportunity to examine relations between these two constructs over time. We theorized relations would be reciprocal; lower school-related wellbeing would be related to higher subsequent emotion risk as a result of worries about school, physical complaints in school, and social problems and emotion risk would contribute to the negative elements of school-related wellbeing (e.g., Crawford and Manassis
2011; Millings et al.
2012; Oldfield et al.
2016). Results supported this hypothesising and confirmed directional relationships between school-related wellbeing and emotion risk. An assumption of the DFM is that not all students with high emotion risk will have lower subsequent wellbeing (Suldo and Shaffer
2008). Our results, however, showed the overall relation is a negative one. Higher emotion risk might therefore be considered a high risk factor for the subsequent low school-related wellbeing and vice versa.
Limitations and Suggestions for Future Studies
Although in the present study, we used a strong design and an appropriately powered robust analytic approach (see Bentler and Chou
1987) to model bidirectional relations between test anxiety, emotion risk, and school-related wellbeing, there are nonetheless three principal limitations to highlight. First, we did not measure emotion disorder symptomology directly, but relied on a proxy measure of risk for developing emotion disorders. Future studies could potentially utilise more direct measures of emotion disorders. The benefits of this approach, however, must be weighed against the likelihood of longer measures resulting in a greater number of spoilt or incomplete student responses. Second, although we theorised relations between test anxiety and emotion risk partly on the basis of integrative network approach (Hereen and McNally
2016,
2018) we did not directly examine networks of test anxiety and emotion risk. Future studies could examine the strength and centrality of test anxiety and emotion disorders indicators using network analysis (see Epskamp et al.
2017).
Third, based on the DFM (Suldo and Shaffer
2008) we included both school-related wellbeing and emotion risk as independent but related constructs and demonstrated that they were negatively related in a bidirectional fashion using variable-centred analyses. However, we did not examine the subtleties in the relations between school-related wellbeing and emotion risk as predicted by DFM. Future studies could use cuts-score or person-centred analyses (e.g., Rose et al.
2017) to examine whether there are groups of students with higher school-related wellbeing with higher emotion risk (i.e., symptomatic but content), and lower school-related wellbeing with lower emotion risk (i.e., vulnerable).
We would also like to briefly comment on the conceptualisation of test anxiety, emotion disorder, and school-related wellbeing. In the present study, these constructs were treated as conceptually distinct, although empirically related. However, there are alternative perspectives. Rather than test anxiety being distinct from that of emotion disorder it is possible that test anxious symptoms are a manifestation of one or more emotion disorder (e.g., LeBeau et al.
2010). Fears about testing, for example, are included within the social phobia subscale of the Revised Child Anxiety and Depression Scale (Chorpita et al.
2005). Thus, test anxiety and emotion disorder could be related in a part-whole fashion rather than representing distinct categories. Furthermore, test anxiety could be considered as an indicator of low subjective or school-related wellbeing through contributing to school-related worries (Hascher
2003), rather than as separate construct. The purpose of the present study was not to unpick these thorny conceptual issues and data collected not designed in such a way as to address these issues. Rather, we wished to highlight that future studies may wish to address conceptual distinctions between test anxiety, emotion disorder, and school-related wellbeing.
Implications for Practice
Returning to the question posed in the tile of this paper, test anxiety can indeed predispose adolescents for enhanced risk of developing emotion disorders, but may not necessarily substantially impact their school-related wellbeing within a relatively brief timespan covering about seven months of schooling. This would suggest that interventions designed to address test anxiety would have downstream benefits for mental health by reducing emotion risk. There are well-established and impactful psychological interventions for test anxiety (Ergene
2003; von der Embse et al.
2013) and recent evidence has shown that a relatively brief six-session cognitive-behavioural intervention (CBIs) is effective in reducing both test anxiety and anxiety disorder symptoms (Putwain and Prescod
2018; Putwain et al.
2020c). In periods of testing when academic pressures are heightened, test anxiety interventions may be especially helpful in ensuring that anxieties regarding examinations do not become sufficiently ingrained and intensify into emotion disorders. However, as shown in the findings of this study, there would also be downstream benefits to raising subsequent school-related wellbeing and lowering subsequent test anxiety by addressing emotion disorders.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.