Introduction
Sensorimotor knowledge from the previous interactions with the environment plays an important role in planning and predicting everyday actions. For example, imagining object rotations is aided by hand gestures that simulate the manipulation of those objects (Chu & Kita,
2011; see also Alibali, Spencer, Knox, & Kita,
2011; Boncoddo, Dixon, & Kelly,
2010). It has been suggested that, by recruiting sensorimotor routines, remembered information of the previous interactions with objects becomes available that can support the predictability of the environment (Hostetter & Boncoddo,
2017; Pouw & Hostetter,
2016). Relevant to the present study, it has been found that gestures can also strengthen sensorimotor information in memory, which makes the information about object manipulation more prominent, and affects subsequent action on objects (e.g., Goldin-Meadow & Beilock,
2010). In the current study, we test this sensorimotor strengthening effect by exploring how co-speech hand gestures affect memory for sensorimotor information through the application of a well-known visual-haptic illusion known as the size-weight illusion.
The size-weight illusion (SWI) occurs when participants perceive a difference in the heaviness when lifting two differently sized objects that are, in fact, the same weight (i.e., haptic perception; for a review, see Buckingham,
2014). Specifically, a smaller object is experienced to be heavier than a larger object with the same weight. The exact mechanisms of the SWI are still under debate and may relate to the veridical perception of wieldability of an object (e.g., Amazeen & Turvey,
1996; Zhu & Bingham,
2011) and/or top–down expectations that small objects should be lighter than larger objects leading to expectation errors that bias experience (see Buckingham,
2014). The SWI is so robust that it persists even when participants have been told that the objects are the same weight and have been allowed to lift the objects with their eyes closed and feel the equivalent weight (Buckingham,
2014). The SWI is also present on the motor level, as it initially affects the gripping strength participants use to lift objects (Flanagan & Beltzner,
2000).
However, despite the persistence of the consciously experienced illusion, participants’ motor coordination eventually attunes to the lack of difference in weights after interacting with the objects multiple times. After lifting the objects 20 times, participants no longer overestimate the strength needed to lift a smaller object relative to a larger object (Flanagan & Beltzner,
2000). It appears that the perceptuo-motor system comes to attune to the veridical sensory feedback from lifting the objects, even while the conscious experience that the smaller object is heavier than the larger object remains unaltered. Therefore, after repeatedly (> 20 times) lifting the objects, there seems to be a disconnect in the sensorimotor knowledge available to the motor system as it plans a hand-grasp and the sensorimotor knowledge that is consciously available to the participant. This disassociation between implicit motor knowledge and explicit conscious knowledge has been explained by the different functional roles of the dorsal and ventral neural pathways (see Goodale & Milner,
1992). Importantly, because implicit motor knowledge and explicit knowledge diverge, the SWI is an interesting phenomenon for understanding how gestures might strengthen different kinds of sensorimotor knowledge.
Gestures differ from real actions, because they do not involve objects, and they differ from pantomimes, because they accompany speech. There is some evidence suggesting that gestures can affect how speakers think about the weight of the objects which they describe (Beilock & Goldin-Meadow,
2010; Cooperrider, Wakefield, & Goldin-Meadow,
2015; Trofatter, Kontra, Beilock, & Goldin-Meadow,
2015). Beilock and Goldin-Meadow (
2010) had participants solve the Tower of Hanoi (TOH) task, in which participants manipulate discs of different sizes and corresponding weights according to specific rules. After solving the task, some participants were asked to explain how they solved the task and to use gestures as they did so. Then, all participants were asked to solve the task a second time in one of two conditions. In the No Switch condition, the second task was physically identical to the previous task. In the Switch condition, the discs’ weights were switched, so that the smallest disc was now the heaviest and could no longer be lifted with one hand. Beilock and Goldin-Meadow (
2010) showed that the more participants gestured about lifting the smallest disc with one hand, the more impaired they were on the second version of the task when the weights had been switched. The basic effect has been replicated several times with similar methodological procedures and sample sizes (Cooperrider et al.,
2015; Trofatter et al.,
2015; but see Wassenburg, de Koning, & van der Schoot,
2018 for contrasting evidence), and the general explanation provided for it is that “using gesture to describe physical interactions with the environment generates strong mental representations that involve physical properties of the action and/or the environment (properties like weight)” (Trofatter et al.,
2015, p. 8).
If this explanation is extended to gestures about objects that induce an SWI, participants’ memories for the weights of the objects should be affected by whether they gestured about lifting them. This could be manifest in either of two ways, depending on the nature of the sensorimotor knowledge that gestures strengthen (Flanagan & Beltzner,
2000). On one hand, gestures may bring the conscious perception of the objects’ weights more in line with the sensorimotor knowledge that is available to the motor system as participants actually lift the objects. For example, there is evidence that manual pantomimes (i.e., enactment of an interaction without a present object) approximate the kinematics of normal grasping actions to a high degree (Weiss, Jeannerod, Paulignan, & Freund,
2000) and that they simulate action-specific knowledge such as weight (Ansuini et al.,
2016). Furthermore, some evidence suggests that gestures can help problem solvers to gain conscious awareness of their implicit
knowledge (e.g., Church & Goldin-Meadow,
1986; Perry, Church, & Goldin-Meadow,
1988). Under this view, gesturing about lifting the objects repeatedly may align participants’ conscious perception of the weights of the objects more closely with the sensorimotor knowledge which they possess about how to actually lift them. This would result in a smaller SWI after gesturing about lifting the objects than after not gesturing.
On the other hand, it is also possible that gesturing about lifting the objects could strengthen the size of the illusion. Under this view, gesturing about lifting the objects could reflect and strengthen participants’ memory of the conscious perception that the smaller object is heavier, rather than their sensorimotor knowledge about how to actually lift the objects that are of the same weight. Pantomimes appear to be coordinated by a system concerned with perceptual aspects of objects rather than implicit action-specific (motor-relevant) properties of objects (Goodale, Jakobson, & Keillor,
1994). For example, when participants are asked to pantomime grasping a previously seen stick—that is designed to induce a visual illusion (i.e., Müller-Lyer illusion)—the hand aperture is biased by their illusory perception of the stick length. In contrast, when participants reach to grasp the actual sticks, the kinematics of their hand aperture do not reflect this illusory perception to similar degress and are, instead, more attuned to the actual lengths of the sticks (Westwood, Heath, & Roy,
2000). Thus, it is possible that gesturing about lifting the objects will further cement the illusory rendering that the smaller object is heavier in weight than the larger object.
In the present study, we test these possibilities by first having all participants practice solving a problem with pieces that induce a size-weight illusion. The problem involved physically moving the pieces 30 times, which is enough lifting experience with the pieces for the perceptuo-motor system to attune to the equal weights of the objects. Thus, at the end of the problem-solving task (see “
Methods” for details), all participants were assumed to have accurate implicit sensorimotor knowledge that the weights of the pieces were equal while still experiencing an illusory conscious experience that the smaller object is heavier than the larger object (Flanagan & Beltzner,
2000). Then, participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions. In the Control condition, participants performed a non-related task (i.e., solving a Sudoku puzzle). In the Gesture condition, participants were asked to explain the solution of the problem-solving task while gesturing. In the No-Gesture condition, participants were asked to sit on their hands while explaining the problem solution. It should be noted that, in gesture research, it is difficult to find a no-gesture explaining condition without confounding variables. On one hand, research has shown that prohibiting gestures can negatively affect the semantic richness of explanations (e.g., Hostetter, Alibali, & Kita,
2007). On the other hand, it is difficult to find participants who do not gesture spontaneously (e.g., Eielts et al.,
2018) when explaining tasks involving spatial and motor skills, and such participants may have different spatial and motor skills than participants who do use co-speech gestures. In the present study, a prohibiting gesture condition was included to rule out an effect of explaining in and of it. Importantly, we did not expect semantic richness of verbal explanations to influence the SWI as the previous studies have shown no effects of gesture on speech content in this type of problem-solving task (e.g., Beilock & Goldin-Meadow,
2010). Similar exploratory analyses of speech content are included in the present paper to verify this (for results see Analysis 5 in “
Appendix 2”). Finally, all participants provided two estimates of how heavy the task-relevant pieces felt using a magnitude estimation procedure commonly used to ascertain the magnitude of the SWI (e.g., Buckingham, Goodale, White, & Westwood,
2016). They were first asked to recall heaviness from memory (i.e., main variable of interest) and subsequently provided an estimate while holding the objects (i.e., this more direct estimate of the SWI serves as a materials check). Participants who experience the SWI should report the small cube as being heavier than the large cube in both measures. Furthermore, we predicted that the magnitude of the experienced SWI (i.e., recalled heaviness) might depend on whether participants had gestured about the task. Compared to participants who have not gestured about lifting the objects, participants who have gestured about lifting the objects should experience either a larger SWI (if gestures strengthen the consciously perceived illusion that the smaller object is heavier) or a smaller SWI (if gestures allow access to implicit sensorimotor knowledge about actual lifting).
In addition to these pre-registered hypotheses (
https://www.osf.io/9uh6q/), we conducted a series of exploratory analyses pertaining to how gestures are related to specific aspects of sensorimotor knowledge. In a recent conceptual replication of the original TOH studies, researchers failed to obtain the original sensorimotor strengthening effect (Wassenburg et al.,
2018). However, they did find that task-relevant gestures (i.e., movements from left-to-right) were related to relatively slower task performance in the switched condition (solving TOH from right to left), similar to the correlation between amount of one-handed movements and task solution reported by Beilock and Goldin-Meadow (
2010) in the Switch condition. These results may be explained by the hypothesis that gestures reflect (rather than affect) the way that participants think about the task. In the present study, we addressed this hypothesis by examining how gesture form is related to the magnitude of either the recalled or the perceived illusion. We examined how often gestures were produced that closely mirrored the lifting motion involved in the actual task (e.g., lifting with two hands, rather than one), to see if producing such congruent gestures would be associated with a larger or smaller illusion. We also used a Frame Differencing Method (FDM; Brookshire, Lu, Nusbaum, Goldin-Meadow, & Cassasanto,
2017; Romero et al.,
2017; Pouw et al.,
2018) to measure the velocity of two-handed lifting gestures to explore whether participants who report that the objects are heavier would move their hands more slowly as they gestured about lifting them. If gestures reflect such sensorimotor knowledge in their kinematics, this would provide strong evidence that gestures are based in sensorimotor know how.
Discussion
The present pre-registered study assessed how gestures support memory for sensorimotor information of the weight of task-relevant objects during problem solving. The pieces were designed to induce the visual-haptic size-weight illusion (SWI), which is a consciously experienced perception that smaller objects are heavier than larger objects when the two are actually the same weight. Because gestures have been suggested to affect sensorimotor knowledge (Beilock & Goldin-Meadow,
2010; Cooperrider et al.,
2015; Goldin-Meadow & Beilock,
2010; Trofatter et al.,
2015), it was predicted that the magnitude of the SWI would be affected when participants gestured about the task-relevant objects. On one hand, gestures could increase the size of the illusion, suggesting that gestures strengthen, in memory, the consciously perceived illusion that the pieces differ in weight. On the other hand, gestures could decrease the size of the illusion, suggesting that gestures strengthen the implicit sensorimotor knowledge that the pieces have the same weight. We found no evidence to support the prediction that gesturing about the problem pieces affected the weight estimates of the pieces in either direction. Instead, our results show that judgments about the weight of the cubes were unaffected by whether participants gestured.
Interestingly, our exploratory analyses revealed that speakers who produced the most gestures mimicking the form of the two-handed lifting and moving actions involved in the task were also the most likely to experience a large inverted SWI in their memory for the pieces. Namely, the number of two-handed grasp gestures produced per minute by participants who were told to gesture as they explained the task was reliably associated with their memory of how heavy the pieces were. This finding, thus, replicates the previous reports that action-relevant gestures are reflective of sensorimotor cognition (e.g., Beilock & Goldin-Meadow,
2010; Wassenburg et al.,
2018). Furthermore, we provide an additional evidence that the velocity with which speakers lifted their hands as they gestured is related to their memory of the objects’ weight. When speakers thought of the objects as heavy in their memory, they gestured about them as though they would be more difficult to lift, as indicated by lower average velocity traces. This provides evidence that speakers embody weight information in the kinematics of their co-speech gestures (as predicted by Mangelsdorf, Cooperrider, & Goldin-Meadow,
2017; see also Ansuini et al.,
2016 for related findings with pantomimes; see also Cook & Tanenhaus,
2009, for how gesture kinematics is affected by the previous task experiences). Future research could specifically focus on how the kinematics of gestures about interactions with objects might be similar to and different from the kinematics of actually interacting with the objects. Indeed, although gestures might approximate object-oriented actions in some respects (as shown here), they are very likely to diverge on the other aspects wherein kinematics are constrained by dynamics that arise by actual wielding of objects (e.g., Runeson & Frykholm,
1983).
Furthermore, this is the first evidence that gestures reflect sensorimotor information that is consciously perceived, rather than the precise kinematics of a previous action. Participants who believed the objects were heavier gestured about them with slower lifting velocities than participants who believed that they were lighter, even though all objects in the present study were of exactly the same weight and required identical lifting velocities as participants solved the task. This is interesting, because it suggests that gestures are not direct recreations of the previous actions; rather, they are actions that are filtered through the participants’ conscious beliefs about those actions. This finding aligns with claims that gestures are representational (Novack & Goldin-Meadow,
2017) or simulated (Hostetter & Alibali,
2008) actions. Thus, actions and gestures are critically different, because gestures cannot and do not attune to action-relevant information in the environment the way that actual actions on objects do (Laimgruber, Goldenberg, & Hermsdörfer,
2005; Kuntz, Karl, Doan, & Whishaw,
2018; Runeson & Frykholm,
1983), at least not when the objects are not present (cf. Chu & Kita,
2016). Although gestures may have their origin in the action system, their execution is affected by the producer’s sensorimotor expectations about how they would act in the world, rather than by an implicit memory of how that action was actually produced.
In the current task, it is possible that participants were not judging the weight of the objects on the basis of attuning to the previous experience via memory at all. It might be that no such memory about weight was available, or, otherwise, explicable. Instead, a rational choice was made on the fly based on a heuristic that larger/smaller objects are heavier/lighter. It is unclear, however, why the use of such a strategy would be related to how speakers gesture about lifting the pieces. Instead, we argue that participants were most likely accessing a haptic knowledge for a simulation for what it felt like to lift the pieces when they made their weight judgments. Indeed, it has been found that, when judging an object’s weight, people actually imagine holding the object rather than using some kind of propositional rule (Klatzky, Lederman, & Matula,
1991). Furthermore, it has been found that previously experienced haptic information can be attuned to with some success, as haptic memory of objects even allows one to discover new properties of the object in imagery (Pouw, Aslanidou, Kamermans, & Paas,
2017; see also Kamermans et al., under review). Thus, although there is a possibility that participants only remembered visual properties of the objects and then used these visual properties to make heuristic inferences about the objects’ haptic properties, research suggests that this is not how people tend to judge haptic properties from memory.
We speculate that both gestures and weight estimates in the current context result from simulations that are neither the result of a “pure memory” of a previous action, nor the result of a rule-like “heuristic” that “small/large objects are lighter/heavier”. Rather, a simulation involves generation of information that abides by regularities observed in the real-world, and is not the simple replay of a previously performed action. This is based on the argument that actions are not represented by some motor plan that incorporates a list of muscle activations needed to perform the action successfully (Bernstein,
1966). Indeed, research (e.g., Kelso, Tuller, Vatikiotis-Bateson, & Fowler,
1984) has shown that when an action is initiated (e.g., speaking), perturbation of the trajectory of that action (e.g., locking the jaw in place) leads to fluid adjustments which recruit the other muscles than would, otherwise, be the case (e.g., lip adjustments), yielding a new sensorimotor solution to the same goal (e.g., speaking a syllable “|baeb|”). Thus, if gesture is derived from practical action, it is likely based on the broader ability to construct sensorimotor solutions constrained by contextual demands, rather than on memory of sensorimotor particulars. Of course, gestures are far less constrained by contextual demands than actions, because they do not involve real objects. This is precisely why gestures may reflect stereotypical sensorimotor solutions—gestures act out actions in ways that would fit most contexts (e.g., slower lifting gestures for heavier objects). A simulation is, therefore, a
constructive process based on the accumulated sensorimotor knowledge that “smaller/larger objects are lighter/heavier”, but it goes beyond a simple heuristic, because it involves knowledge of real-world sensorimotor contingencies. We suggest that both producing gestures about interacting with the pieces and imagining the pieces so as to judge their weight rely on the simulation of expectations about how it would feel to interact with the pieces.
Importantly, however, in our study, we find no evidence that gesturing about the pieces in a certain way caused a change in how participants thought about the weight of the pieces. Experimentally manipulating gesture did not affect participants’ recollection or perception of the weights of the cubes. The fact that no significant differences between conditions were found and speech content did not affect perceived heaviness indicates that it is unlikely that our choice of conditions impacted our results. Instead, we observed an effect within the gesture condition—a correlation between how participants thought about the cubes and how they gestured about them. Participants’ utilization of two-handed gestures was related to the magnitude of the SWI they experienced, and the velocity with which they gestured was related to their recalled heaviness of the cubes. We speculate that, in both cases, how people thought about the task as they described it was reflected in gesture. Participants may have imagined the motor kinematics of the task to a greater or lesser degree as they were explaining the problem. Those more inclined to simulate the specifics of interacting with the cubes were more likely to produce two-handed gestures that showed the specific action required to manipulate the cubes, including the relative velocity of lifting light/heavy objects. Furthermore, participants who thought more specifically about the motor processes involved in the task as they were describing were also likely to form a weight judgment that was based on a sensorimotor judgment when asked to judge the objects’ weight. Such detailed sensorimotor imagination of the cubes (both during the description task and during the rating task) was particularly likely to be distorted by the sensorimotor know-how that large objects are generally heavy. Under this explanation, two-handed lifting gestures were an embodiment of detailed sensorimotor imagery about lifting, but were not a driving agent in whether participants adopted a detailed sensorimotor imagining or not.
An interesting aspect of this finding is how the judgments were biased when they were made from memory. In the classic SWI, participants judge a smaller cube that they are holding as heavier than a larger cube when the two are actually the same weight. Although we replicated this traditional SWI when the cubes were compared from immediate experience (the smaller cube was perceived as 43% heavier than the larger cube), when participants were asked to judge the heaviness of the cubes from memory, they seemed to judge heaviness rationally—that is, they reported that the larger cube had felt 21% heavier than the smaller cube.
4 This effect was not predicted, and to our knowledge, this is the first study that has revealed an inverted SWI when weight judgments are made from memory (as the SWI is generally not studied in relation to the memory system). The previous research has indicated that people actually imagine holding an object when judging the object’s weight and other haptic properties (Klatzky et al.,
1991), yet the observed inversion of the SWI in memory suggests that this active imagining is not operating with accurate implicit memories of how these particular objects were perceived as they were manipulated (which would have led to no illusion) or with accurate explicit memories of how these particular objects were perceived as they were manipulated (which would have led to the traditional smaller-is-heavier illusion). Instead, the judgment seems to be based on a stereotypical understanding that larger objects are typically heavier than smaller objects.
One of the reviewers of the current paper suggested that there is a possibility that the memory of sensorimotor experiences may be affected by sensorimotor expectations. Under this view, it is not so much that participants are drawing from a memory of a sensorimotor experience (e.g., small object being heavy), but rather from a transformed memory where unexpected aspects of the sensorimotor event (e.g., light objects feeling heavier) are “washed out”. The degree to which a memory is resolved in favor of a sensorimotor expectancy might then explain why gesture is related to heaviness ratings from memory. The task-relevant two-handed gesturing effectively is related to expectancy, not so much a memory. We are very favourable to this idea as we have argued that gestures are governed by a system that operates on a history of sensorimotor contingencies, rather than on the sensorimotor specifics of a moment ago, though the scope of the current results deems this idea still too speculative, and more research is needed to directly test this idea. Note that research on the SWI has confirmed that repeated interactions with objects can affect the illusion, suggesting that the sensorimotor history can be manipulated and is not some unchangeable pre-given. Namely, Flanagan, Bittner, and Johansson (
2008) have found that after a multi-day training with interacting with objects that induce the SWI (240 lifts for 11 days), the SWI not only dissipates, it is inverted. Extensively trained participants judged larger objects as heavier than smaller objects that weighed the same. If gestures are indeed based on expectations that operate on slow timescales, we would expect that multi-day training with new sensorimotor contingencies would be (especially) apparent in the way that people gesture about such contingencies. Further research can explore our hypothesis that gestures attune to a history of sensorimotor interactions by assessing effects of multi-day training vs. immediate previous experiences when we gesture about object manipulations. Furthermore, perhaps, in the current paradigm, repeated sensorimotor imagining of the objects in gesture may come to affect sensorimotor expectations given enough time (Pouw & Hostetter,
2016).
To conclude, the present pre-registered study has offered novel insights into how gestures are related to memory of sensorimotor information. Although gesturing about the cubes did not affect participants’ memories of how heavy the objects were or their judgments of perceived heaviness when the objects were held again, the kinematics of gestures were related to speakers’ thinking about sensorimotor properties of the cubes. The results indicate that gestures reflect rather than affect the way that we think about and remember objects’ sensorimotor properties, at least in the context examined here. It appears that gestures do not just reveal information about the actions which a speaker performed; instead, they reveal how the speaker thinks about what they did.