Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2023

01-03-2022 | Original Article

Does framing an assignment as involving one or multiple components influence subjective experiences of attentional engagement?

Auteurs: Emilie E. Caron, Allison C. Drody, Brandon C. W. Ralph, Jonathan S. A. Carriere, Daniel Smilek

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2023

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Across two studies, we explored whether framing an assignment as involving either multitasking or single-tasking (Srna et al. Psychol Sci 29(12):1942–1955, 2018) leads to differences in both subjective ratings of attentional engagement (i.e., depth of concentration and attentional control) and performance during the assignment. In Experiment 1, we manipulated task framing in the context of an assignment in which participants (Ncollected = 238) simultaneously completed a word-search and an anagram task (Srna et al. Psychol Sci 29(12):1942–1955, 2018). While we replicated prior findings that participants who receive multitasking instructions perform better than those who receive single-tasking instructions, we did not find any influence of task framing on participants’ subjective evaluations of their attentional engagement. Exploratory analyses, however, revealed that regardless of group assignment, those who believed they were multitasking reported greater levels of attentional engagement than those who believed they were single-tasking. In Experiment 2 (Ncollected = 238), task framing was varied in the context of the 2-back task (Kirchner J Exp Psychol 55(4): 352, 1958). Unexpectedly, we found that, relative to participants who received single-tasking instructions, those who received multitasking instructions reported exerting less attentional control over their thoughts and showed a greater number of incorrect responses to non-target trials on the 2-back. Taken together, the results do not support a straightforward conclusion regarding the influence of task framing on either subjective reports of attentional engagement or task performance. Nevertheless, they provide insight into our understanding of the role of task framing in contexts ranging from commonly performed real-world tasks to typical laboratory tasks.
Voetnoten
1
In the present paper, we detail two of four experiments conducted to investigated whether task framing leads to differences in performance and attention. We chose not to include the first two experiments because they included different methodology than the present studies and an error in one of our primary measures of subjective engagement. However, the methods and results of these experiments can be found on OSF in the Preliminary Experiments folder (https://​osf.​io/​v2kjx/​).
 
2
We felt this was an acceptable deviation from Srna et al.’s Study 2b design given that the authors obtained the same pattern of results in their Study 2a, in which participants completed the same paradigm with a fixed 4-min time frame.
 
3
In our pre-registration, we had specified that we would remove those with too few words submitted to the assignment. Importantly, during the process of collecting data, we became aware that participants might have been using anagram decoders to obtain higher scores on the assignment. To address this, we also removed those with greater than 42 correct words submitted.
 
4
Results of Levene’s test indicated that the assumption of homogeneity of variance was violated, F(1, 224) = 15.44, p < .001. The outcome of the ANOVA did not change however, when correcting for unequal variances, Welch’s F(1, 210.88) = 42.76, p < .001.
 
5
We did not analyze the number of incorrect words submitted as the word-search was designed in such a way that participants could not submit incorrect words. Once a correct word of four letters or longer was selected, the word would appear in a darker colour on the word-search and would be registered as a response.
 
6
Results of Levene’s test indicated that the assumption of homogeneity of variance was violated, F(1, 224) = 7.05, p = .009.The outcome of the ANOVA did not change however, when correcting for unequal variances, Welch’s F(1, 209.2) = 5.44, p = .021.
 
7
Levene’s test revealed that the assumption of homogeneity of variance was violated, F(1, 180) = 9.70, p = .002. Nevertheless, the outcome of the ANOVA did not change when correcting for unequal variances, Welch’s F(1, 168.96) = 19.44, p < .001.
 
8
For a different configuration of the 2-back that also failed to yield an effect of task framing on 2-back performance, see our preregistration documents https://​osf.​io/​v2kjx).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Aral, S., Brynjolfsson, E., & Van Alstyne, M. (2012). Information, technology, and information worker productivity. Information Systems Research, 23(3-part-2), 849–867.CrossRef Aral, S., Brynjolfsson, E., & Van Alstyne, M. (2012). Information, technology, and information worker productivity. Information Systems Research, 23(3-part-2), 849–867.CrossRef
go back to reference Armstrong, G., & Chung, L. (2000). Background television and reading memory in context: Assessing TV interference and facilitative context effects on encoding versus retrieval processes. Communication Research, 27(3), 327–352.CrossRef Armstrong, G., & Chung, L. (2000). Background television and reading memory in context: Assessing TV interference and facilitative context effects on encoding versus retrieval processes. Communication Research, 27(3), 327–352.CrossRef
go back to reference Bachmann, O., Grunschel, C., & Fries, S. (2019). Multitasking and feeling good? Autonomy of additional activities predicts affect. Journal of Happiness Studies, 20(3), 899–918.CrossRef Bachmann, O., Grunschel, C., & Fries, S. (2019). Multitasking and feeling good? Autonomy of additional activities predicts affect. Journal of Happiness Studies, 20(3), 899–918.CrossRef
go back to reference Beatty, J., & Lucero-Wagoner, B. (2000). The pupillary system. Handbook of psychophysiology (4th ed., pp. 142–162) Beatty, J., & Lucero-Wagoner, B. (2000). The pupillary system. Handbook of psychophysiology (4th ed., pp. 142–162)
go back to reference Bertolotti, F., Mattarelli, E., Vignoli, M., & Macrì, D. M. (2015). Exploring the relationship between multiple team membership and team performance: The role of social networks and collaborative technology. Research Policy, 44(4), 911–924.CrossRef Bertolotti, F., Mattarelli, E., Vignoli, M., & Macrì, D. M. (2015). Exploring the relationship between multiple team membership and team performance: The role of social networks and collaborative technology. Research Policy, 44(4), 911–924.CrossRef
go back to reference Borger, R. (1963). The refractory period and serial choice reactions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15(1), 1–12.CrossRef Borger, R. (1963). The refractory period and serial choice reactions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15(1), 1–12.CrossRef
go back to reference Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A., & Lang, P. J. (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology, 45(4), 602–607.CrossRef Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A., & Lang, P. J. (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology, 45(4), 602–607.CrossRef
go back to reference Buser, T., & Peter, N. (2012). Multitasking. Experimental Economics, 15(4), 641–655.CrossRef Buser, T., & Peter, N. (2012). Multitasking. Experimental Economics, 15(4), 641–655.CrossRef
go back to reference Creamer, L. R. (1963). Event uncertainty, psychological refractory period, and human data processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(2), 187–194.CrossRef Creamer, L. R. (1963). Event uncertainty, psychological refractory period, and human data processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(2), 187–194.CrossRef
go back to reference Ewolds, H., Broeker, L., de Oliveira, R. F., Raab, M., & Künzell, S. (2021). No impact of instructions and feedback on task integration in motor learning. Memory and Cognition, 49(2), 340–349.CrossRef Ewolds, H., Broeker, L., de Oliveira, R. F., Raab, M., & Künzell, S. (2021). No impact of instructions and feedback on task integration in motor learning. Memory and Cognition, 49(2), 340–349.CrossRef
go back to reference Forrin, N. D., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2019). On the relation between reading difficulty and mind-wandering: A section-length account. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(3), 485–497.CrossRef Forrin, N. D., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2019). On the relation between reading difficulty and mind-wandering: A section-length account. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(3), 485–497.CrossRef
go back to reference Franklin, M., Broadway, J., Mrazek, M., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. (2013). Window to the wandering mind: Pupillometry of spontaneous thought while reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 66(12), 2289–2294.CrossRef Franklin, M., Broadway, J., Mrazek, M., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. (2013). Window to the wandering mind: Pupillometry of spontaneous thought while reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 66(12), 2289–2294.CrossRef
go back to reference Furnham, A., & Bradley, A. (1997). Music while you work: The differential distraction of background music on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11(5), 445–455.CrossRef Furnham, A., & Bradley, A. (1997). Music while you work: The differential distraction of background music on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11(5), 445–455.CrossRef
go back to reference Hill, L., Rybar, J., Styer, T., Fram, E., Merchant, G., & Eastman, A. (2015). Prevalence of and attitudes about distracted driving in college students. Traffic Injury Prevention, 16(4), 362–367.CrossRef Hill, L., Rybar, J., Styer, T., Fram, E., Merchant, G., & Eastman, A. (2015). Prevalence of and attitudes about distracted driving in college students. Traffic Injury Prevention, 16(4), 362–367.CrossRef
go back to reference Hoeks, B., & Levelt, W. J. (1993). Pupillary dilation as a measure of attention: A quantitative system analysis. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 25(1), 16–26.CrossRef Hoeks, B., & Levelt, W. J. (1993). Pupillary dilation as a measure of attention: A quantitative system analysis. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 25(1), 16–26.CrossRef
go back to reference Junco, R. (2012). In-class multitasking and academic performance. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(6), 2236–2243.CrossRef Junco, R. (2012). In-class multitasking and academic performance. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(6), 2236–2243.CrossRef
go back to reference Junco, R., & Cotten, S. R. (2012). No A 4 U: The relationship between multitasking and academic performance. Computers and Education, 59(2), 505–514.CrossRef Junco, R., & Cotten, S. R. (2012). No A 4 U: The relationship between multitasking and academic performance. Computers and Education, 59(2), 505–514.CrossRef
go back to reference Kahneman, D., & Beatty, J. (1966). Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science, 154(3756), 1583–1585.CrossRef Kahneman, D., & Beatty, J. (1966). Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science, 154(3756), 1583–1585.CrossRef
go back to reference Katz, B., Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Shah, P., & Jonides, J. (2018). The effect of monetary compensation on cognitive training outcomes. Learning and Motivation, 63, 77–90.CrossRef Katz, B., Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Shah, P., & Jonides, J. (2018). The effect of monetary compensation on cognitive training outcomes. Learning and Motivation, 63, 77–90.CrossRef
go back to reference Kieras, D. E., Meyer, D. E., Ballas, J. A., & Lauber, E. J. (2000). Modern computational perspectives on executive mental processes and cognitive control: Where to from here. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance, XVIII, 681–712. Kieras, D. E., Meyer, D. E., Ballas, J. A., & Lauber, E. J. (2000). Modern computational perspectives on executive mental processes and cognitive control: Where to from here. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance, XVIII, 681–712.
go back to reference Kurapati, S., Lukosch, H., Eckerd, S., Verbraeck, A., & Corsi, T. (2017). Relating planner task performance for container terminal operations to multi-tasking skills and personality type. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 51, 47–64.CrossRef Kurapati, S., Lukosch, H., Eckerd, S., Verbraeck, A., & Corsi, T. (2017). Relating planner task performance for container terminal operations to multi-tasking skills and personality type. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 51, 47–64.CrossRef
go back to reference Marchewka, M. (2018). The effects and determinants of multitasking. Aspirare: an International Journal of Commerce and Management, 5, 1–7. Marchewka, M. (2018). The effects and determinants of multitasking. Aspirare: an International Journal of Commerce and Management, 5, 1–7.
go back to reference Marshall, S. P. (2005). Assessing cognitive engagement and cognitive state from eye metrics. Foundations of Augmented Cognition, 11, 312–320. Marshall, S. P. (2005). Assessing cognitive engagement and cognitive state from eye metrics. Foundations of Augmented Cognition, 11, 312–320.
go back to reference Schooler, J. W., Reichle, E. D., & Halpern, D. V. (2004). Zoning out while reading: Evidence for dissociations between experience and metaconsciousness. Thinking and seeing: visual metacognition in adults and children, pp. 203–226. http://www.azmonyar.com/DownloadPDF/ Schooler, J. W., Reichle, E. D., & Halpern, D. V. (2004). Zoning out while reading: Evidence for dissociations between experience and metaconsciousness. Thinking and seeing: visual metacognition in adults and children, pp. 203–226. http://​www.​azmonyar.​com/​DownloadPDF/​
go back to reference Unsworth, N., Robison, M. K., & Miller, A. L. (2018). Pupillary correlates of fluctuations in sustained attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30(9), 1241–1253.CrossRef Unsworth, N., Robison, M. K., & Miller, A. L. (2018). Pupillary correlates of fluctuations in sustained attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30(9), 1241–1253.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Does framing an assignment as involving one or multiple components influence subjective experiences of attentional engagement?
Auteurs
Emilie E. Caron
Allison C. Drody
Brandon C. W. Ralph
Jonathan S. A. Carriere
Daniel Smilek
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2022
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2023
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-022-01651-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2023

Psychological Research 1/2023 Naar de uitgave