Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2009

01-09-2009 | Original Article

Do top and bottom contribute to object perception more than left and right?

Auteur: David Navon

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2009

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

To examine whether the top–bottom axis has an inherent advantage in object perception over the left–right one, three experiments were conducted. In all of them, on each trial one of two alternative stimuli, identical in shape but opposite in direction (viz, related by reflection), was presented. Both reflection about the vertical axis and reflection about the horizontal axis were applied, in different blocks. In Experiment 1, objects with an orientation-free definition (arrows, incomplete squares) were presented. Subjects were to respond when the stimulus pointed in a specific direction, and to refrain from responding when it was reflected, namely pointed in the opposite direction. Axis of reflection (vertical, horizontal) was varied between blocks. In Experiment 2, the object was a Hebrew character asymmetric on both axes, presented either in its normal appearance or reflected. Subjects were to respond only when the stimulus was normal. Both axis of reflection (vertical, horizontal) and orientation angle (upright, tilted by 90°) were varied between blocks. In Experiment 3, stimuli were the same as in Experiment 2, but the task explicitly asked for a binary reflection judgment (normal vs. reflected). No sign for the presence of an axis effect was observed in any of those experiments, which seems incompatible with the hypothesis of vertical advantage in object perception. It is suggested that most vertical advantage observed before is due to extra-perceptual processing.
Voetnoten
1
Symmetry about the sagittal axis (namely, front vs. back) is also quite rare. However, since visual information about that axis is very often sparse in most natural settings, it must have quite a modest weight in perception. Anyhow, this study focuses just on the difference between the principal axes of the frontal (coronal) plane, namely the horizontal and the vertical axes.
 
2
In an auxiliary experiment with another Hebrew character (Beth), conducted to gain some more generality, no evidence in favor of PVAH was observed either.
 
3
And even those are, in many people, not sufficient for realizing how one’s own mirror image is related with her real frontal image (Takano & Tanaka, 2007).
 
4
But Hollingworth (2005) reports degrees of accuracy in localizing objects in natural scenes that are about the same in the two frontal axes. See also Carr & Roskos-Ewoldsen (1999) for evidence that naming locations encoded from narratives was more accurate in the horizontal plane than in both vertical planes—sagittal and coronal.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Appelle, S. (1972). Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation—oblique effect in man and animals. Psychological Bulletin, 78, 266–278.PubMedCrossRef Appelle, S. (1972). Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation—oblique effect in man and animals. Psychological Bulletin, 78, 266–278.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Benton, A. L. (1959). Right left discrimination and nfinger localization: Development and pathology. New York: Hoeber-Harper. Benton, A. L. (1959). Right left discrimination and nfinger localization: Development and pathology. New York: Hoeber-Harper.
go back to reference Benton, A. L. (1992). Gerstmann’s syndrome. Archives of Neurology, 49, 445–447.PubMed Benton, A. L. (1992). Gerstmann’s syndrome. Archives of Neurology, 49, 445–447.PubMed
go back to reference Boone, D. R., & Prescott, T. E. (1968). Development of left–right discrimination in normal children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 26, 267–274.PubMed Boone, D. R., & Prescott, T. E. (1968). Development of left–right discrimination in normal children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 26, 267–274.PubMed
go back to reference Buchanan-Smith, H. M., & Heeley, D. W. (1993). Anisotropic axes in orientation perception are not retinotopically mapped. Perception, 22, 1389–1402.PubMedCrossRef Buchanan-Smith, H. M., & Heeley, D. W. (1993). Anisotropic axes in orientation perception are not retinotopically mapped. Perception, 22, 1389–1402.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Butler, J. (1964). Visual discrimination of shape by humans. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16, 272–276.CrossRef Butler, J. (1964). Visual discrimination of shape by humans. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16, 272–276.CrossRef
go back to reference Carr, W., & Roskos-Ewoldsen, B. (1999). Spatial orientation by mental transformation. Psychological Research, 62, 36–47.CrossRef Carr, W., & Roskos-Ewoldsen, B. (1999). Spatial orientation by mental transformation. Psychological Research, 62, 36–47.CrossRef
go back to reference Cooper, L. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1973). Chronometric studies of the rotation of mental images. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 75–176). New York: Academic Press. Cooper, L. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1973). Chronometric studies of the rotation of mental images. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 75–176). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Corballis, M. C. (2000). Much ado about mirrors. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 163–169. Corballis, M. C. (2000). Much ado about mirrors. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 163–169.
go back to reference Corballis, M. C. (2001). Why mirrors reverse left and right: Commentary on Navon on mirror reversal. Psycholoquy, 12 (032). Corballis, M. C. (2001). Why mirrors reverse left and right: Commentary on Navon on mirror reversal. Psycholoquy, 12 (032).
go back to reference Corballis, M. C., & Beale, I. L. (1976). The psychology of left and right. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Corballis, M. C., & Beale, I. L. (1976). The psychology of left and right. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Davidson, H. P. (1935). A study of the confusing letters b, d, p, q. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 47, 458–468. Davidson, H. P. (1935). A study of the confusing letters b, d, p, q. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 47, 458–468.
go back to reference Fisher, C. B., & Fracasso, M. P. (1987). The Goldmeier effect in adults and children: Environmental, retinal, and phenomenal influences on judgments of visual symmetry. Perception, 16, 29–39.PubMedCrossRef Fisher, C. B., & Fracasso, M. P. (1987). The Goldmeier effect in adults and children: Environmental, retinal, and phenomenal influences on judgments of visual symmetry. Perception, 16, 29–39.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goldmeier, E. (1972). Similarity in visually perceived forms. Psychological Issues, 8, Monograph 29, 1–135. Originally published: (1937) Uber Ahnslichkeit bei gesehenen figuren. Psychologische Forschung, 21, 146–208). Goldmeier, E. (1972). Similarity in visually perceived forms. Psychological Issues, 8, Monograph 29, 1–135. Originally published: (1937) Uber Ahnslichkeit bei gesehenen figuren. Psychologische Forschung, 21, 146–208).
go back to reference Gregory, R. L. (1987). Mirror reversal. In R. L. Gregory (Ed.), The Oxford companion to the mind (pp. 491–493). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Gregory, R. L. (1987). Mirror reversal. In R. L. Gregory (Ed.), The Oxford companion to the mind (pp. 491–493). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Gregory, R. (1997). Mirrors in mind. Oxford: Freeman. Gregory, R. (1997). Mirrors in mind. Oxford: Freeman.
go back to reference Harley, T. A. (2001). The psychology of language: From data to theory (2nd ed. ed.). Hove: Psychology Press. Harley, T. A. (2001). The psychology of language: From data to theory (2nd ed. ed.). Hove: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Hollingworth, A. (2005). Memory for object position in natural scenes. Visual Cognition, 12, 1003–1016.CrossRef Hollingworth, A. (2005). Memory for object position in natural scenes. Visual Cognition, 12, 1003–1016.CrossRef
go back to reference Hunt, E., & Agnoli, F. (1991). The Whorfian hypothesis: A cognitive psychological perspective. Psychological Review, 98, 377–389.CrossRef Hunt, E., & Agnoli, F. (1991). The Whorfian hypothesis: A cognitive psychological perspective. Psychological Review, 98, 377–389.CrossRef
go back to reference Jolicoeur, P. (1985). The time to name disoriented natural objects. Memory and Cognition, 13, 289–303. Jolicoeur, P. (1985). The time to name disoriented natural objects. Memory and Cognition, 13, 289–303.
go back to reference Jolicoeur, P., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1983). Coordinate systems in the long-term memory representation of three-dimensional shapes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 301–345.PubMedCrossRef Jolicoeur, P., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1983). Coordinate systems in the long-term memory representation of three-dimensional shapes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 301–345.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Konorski, J. (1964). On the mechanism of instrumental conditioning. In Proceedings of the 17th international congress of psychology. Amsterdam: North-Holland. Konorski, J. (1964). On the mechanism of instrumental conditioning. In Proceedings of the 17th international congress of psychology. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Kosslyn, S. M., & Rabin, C. (1999). The representation of left–right orientation: A dissociation between imagery and perceptual recognition. Visual Cognition, 6, 497–508.CrossRef Kosslyn, S. M., & Rabin, C. (1999). The representation of left–right orientation: A dissociation between imagery and perceptual recognition. Visual Cognition, 6, 497–508.CrossRef
go back to reference Landau, B. (2003). Axes and direction in spatial language and spatial cognition. In E. van der Zee & J. Slack (Eds.), Representing direction in language and space (pp. 18–38). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Landau, B. (2003). Axes and direction in spatial language and spatial cognition. In E. van der Zee & J. Slack (Eds.), Representing direction in language and space (pp. 18–38). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Levelt, W. J. M. (1984). Some perceptual limitations on talking about space. In A. J. van Doorn, W. A. van de Grind & J. J. Koenderink (Eds.), Limits in perception (pp. 323–358). Utrecht: VNU Science. Levelt, W. J. M. (1984). Some perceptual limitations on talking about space. In A. J. van Doorn, W. A. van de Grind & J. J. Koenderink (Eds.), Limits in perception (pp. 323–358). Utrecht: VNU Science.
go back to reference Louis, M., Huber, T., Benton, R., Sakmar, T. P, & Vosshall, L. B. (2007). Bilateral olfactory sensory input enhances chemotaxis behavior. Nature Neuroscience . doi:10.1038/nn2031. Louis, M., Huber, T., Benton, R., Sakmar, T. P, & Vosshall, L. B. (2007). Bilateral olfactory sensory input enhances chemotaxis behavior. Nature Neuroscience . doi:10.​1038/​nn2031.
go back to reference Mach, E. (1959). The analysis of sensations, and the relation of the physical to the psychical. New York: Dover Publications. First published in English: (1897) Chicago, IL: Open Court; originally published in German: (1886) Beiträge zur Analyse der Empfindungen. Jena: Fischer. Mach, E. (1959). The analysis of sensations, and the relation of the physical to the psychical. New York: Dover Publications. First published in English: (1897) Chicago, IL: Open Court; originally published in German: (1886) Beiträge zur Analyse der Empfindungen. Jena: Fischer.
go back to reference Navon, D. (1987). Why do we blame the mirror for reversing left and right? Cognition, 27, 275–283.PubMedCrossRef Navon, D. (1987). Why do we blame the mirror for reversing left and right? Cognition, 27, 275–283.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Navon, D. (2001). The puzzle of mirror reversal: A view from Clockland. Psycholoquy, 12 (017). Navon, D. (2001). The puzzle of mirror reversal: A view from Clockland. Psycholoquy, 12 (017).
go back to reference Navon, D. (2002). It’s not all in our mind: A reply to Corballis. Psycholoquy, 13 (012). Navon, D. (2002). It’s not all in our mind: A reply to Corballis. Psycholoquy, 13 (012).
go back to reference Onley, J. W., & Volkmann, J. (1958). The visual perception of perpendicularity. American Journal of Psychology, 71, 504–516.PubMedCrossRef Onley, J. W., & Volkmann, J. (1958). The visual perception of perpendicularity. American Journal of Psychology, 71, 504–516.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Park, D. C., & James, C. Q. (1983). Effect of encoding instructions on children’s spatial and color memory: Is there evidence for automaticity? Child Development, 54, 61–68.PubMedCrossRef Park, D. C., & James, C. Q. (1983). Effect of encoding instructions on children’s spatial and color memory: Is there evidence for automaticity? Child Development, 54, 61–68.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Piaget, J. (1959). Judgement and reasoning in the child. Paterson, N.J.: Littlefield, Adams. First published in English: (1928) London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Piaget, J. (1959). Judgement and reasoning in the child. Paterson, N.J.: Littlefield, Adams. First published in English: (1928) London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
go back to reference Poucet, B. (2007). Animal navigation and cognitive maps. In L. Nadel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of cognitive science (Vol. 1, pp. 150–156). London: Wiley. Poucet, B. (2007). Animal navigation and cognitive maps. In L. Nadel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of cognitive science (Vol. 1, pp. 150–156). London: Wiley.
go back to reference Quinn, P. C. (2004). Visual perception of orientation is categorical near vertical and continuous near horizontal. Perception, 33, 897–906.PubMedCrossRef Quinn, P. C. (2004). Visual perception of orientation is categorical near vertical and continuous near horizontal. Perception, 33, 897–906.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roberts, R. J., & Aman, C. J. (1993). Developmental differences in giving directions: Spatial frames of reference and giving directions. Child Development, 64, 1258–1270.PubMedCrossRef Roberts, R. J., & Aman, C. J. (1993). Developmental differences in giving directions: Spatial frames of reference and giving directions. Child Development, 64, 1258–1270.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roberson, D., Davies, I., & Davidoff, J. (2000). Color catergories are not universal: Replications and new evidence from stone-age culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 369–398.CrossRef Roberson, D., Davies, I., & Davidoff, J. (2000). Color catergories are not universal: Replications and new evidence from stone-age culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 369–398.CrossRef
go back to reference Rock, I. (1973). Orientation and form. New York: Academic Press. Rock, I. (1973). Orientation and form. New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Rudel, R. G., & Teuber, H. L. (1963). Discrimination of direction line in children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56, 892–898.PubMedCrossRef Rudel, R. G., & Teuber, H. L. (1963). Discrimination of direction line in children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56, 892–898.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Serpell, R. (1971). Discrimination of orientation by Zambian children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 75, 312–316.PubMedCrossRef Serpell, R. (1971). Discrimination of orientation by Zambian children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 75, 312–316.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shimron, J., & Navon, D. (1981). The distribution of information within letters. Perception and Psychophysics, 30, 483–491.PubMed Shimron, J., & Navon, D. (1981). The distribution of information within letters. Perception and Psychophysics, 30, 483–491.PubMed
go back to reference Slack, J., & Van der Zee, E. (2003). The representation of direction in language and space. In E. van der Zee & J. Slack (Eds.), Representing direction in language and space (pp. 1–17). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Slack, J., & Van der Zee, E. (2003). The representation of direction in language and space. In E. van der Zee & J. Slack (Eds.), Representing direction in language and space (pp. 1–17). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Standing, L., Conezio, J., & Haber, R. N. (1970). Perception and memory for pictures: Single-trial learning of 2500 visual stimuli. Psychonomic Science, 19, 73–74. Standing, L., Conezio, J., & Haber, R. N. (1970). Perception and memory for pictures: Single-trial learning of 2500 visual stimuli. Psychonomic Science, 19, 73–74.
go back to reference Tabata, T., & Okuda, S. (2000). Miror reversal simply explained without recourse to psychological processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 170–173. Tabata, T., & Okuda, S. (2000). Miror reversal simply explained without recourse to psychological processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 170–173.
go back to reference Takano, Y. (1998). Why does a mirror image look left–right reversed? A hypothesis of multiple processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 37–55. Takano, Y. (1998). Why does a mirror image look left–right reversed? A hypothesis of multiple processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 37–55.
go back to reference Takano, Y., & Tanaka, (2007). Mirror reversal: Empirical tests of competing accounts. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1555–1584.PubMed Takano, Y., & Tanaka, (2007). Mirror reversal: Empirical tests of competing accounts. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1555–1584.PubMed
go back to reference Tee, K. S., & Riesen, A. H. (1974). Visual left–right confusions in animal and man. In G. Newton & A. H. Riesen (Eds.), Advances in psychobiology (Vol. 2, pp. 241–265). New York: Wiley. Tee, K. S., & Riesen, A. H. (1974). Visual left–right confusions in animal and man. In G. Newton & A. H. Riesen (Eds.), Advances in psychobiology (Vol. 2, pp. 241–265). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Vogel, J. M. (1979). The influence of verbal descriptions versus orientation codes on kindergartners’ memory for the orientation of pictures. Child Development, 50, 239–242.PubMedCrossRef Vogel, J. M. (1979). The influence of verbal descriptions versus orientation codes on kindergartners’ memory for the orientation of pictures. Child Development, 50, 239–242.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vogel, J. M. (1980). Limitations on children’s short-term memory for left–right orientation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 473–495.PubMedCrossRef Vogel, J. M. (1980). Limitations on children’s short-term memory for left–right orientation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 473–495.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Weissenbacher, P., & Wiegrebe, L. (2003). Classification of virtual objects in the echolocating bat, Megaderma lyra. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 833–839.PubMedCrossRef Weissenbacher, P., & Wiegrebe, L. (2003). Classification of virtual objects in the echolocating bat, Megaderma lyra. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 833–839.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. New York: Wiley. Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. New York: Wiley.
Metagegevens
Titel
Do top and bottom contribute to object perception more than left and right?
Auteur
David Navon
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2009
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2009
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0179-x

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2009

Psychological Research 5/2009 Naar de uitgave