Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 7/2011

01-07-2011 | Original Paper

Do High-Functioning People with Autism Spectrum Disorder Spontaneously Use Event Knowledge to Selectively Attend to and Remember Context-Relevant Aspects in Scenes?

Auteurs: Eva Loth, Juan Carlós Gómez, Francesca Happé

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 7/2011

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

This study combined an event schema approach with top-down processing perspectives to investigate whether high-functioning children and adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) spontaneously attend to and remember context-relevant aspects of scenes. Participants read one story of story-pairs (e.g., burglary or tea party). They then inspected a scene (living room) of which some objects were relevant in that context, irrelevant (related to the non-emphasized event) or neutral (scene-schema related). During immediate and delayed recall, only the (TD) groups selectively recalled context-relevant objects, and significantly more context-relevant objects than the ASD groups. Gaze-tracking suggests that one factor in these memory differences may be diminished top-down effects of event schemas on initial attention (first ten fixations) to relevant items in ASD.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
One exception was the street scene corresponding to Stories 2a and b, since it seemed somewhat unnatural for a busy street to be entirely devoid of people. However, except for the policeman, the other people depicted in the scene were not coded as relevant in either event context.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference American Psychological Association (APA). (1994). DSM-IV diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. (4th ed.). Washington, DC. American Psychological Association (APA). (1994). DSM-IV diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. (4th ed.). Washington, DC.
go back to reference Antes, J. R. (1974). The time course of picture viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 62–70.PubMedCrossRef Antes, J. R. (1974). The time course of picture viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 62–70.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baddley, A. (1999). Essentials of human memory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Baddley, A. (1999). Essentials of human memory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (2008). Autism, hypersystemizing, and truth. Quaterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Colchester), 61(1), 64–75.CrossRef Baron-Cohen, S. (2008). Autism, hypersystemizing, and truth. Quaterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Colchester), 61(1), 64–75.CrossRef
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.PubMedCrossRef Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Beversdorf, D. Q., Smith, B. W., Crucian, G. P., Anderson, J. M., Keillor, J. M., Barrett, A. M., et al. (2000). Increased discrimination of “false memories” in autism spectrum disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(15), 9734–9737.CrossRef Beversdorf, D. Q., Smith, B. W., Crucian, G. P., Anderson, J. M., Keillor, J. M., Barrett, A. M., et al. (2000). Increased discrimination of “false memories” in autism spectrum disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(15), 9734–9737.CrossRef
go back to reference Bird, G., Catmur, C., Silani, G., Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2006). Attention does not modulate neural response to social stimuli in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage, 31, 1614–1624.PubMedCrossRef Bird, G., Catmur, C., Silani, G., Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2006). Attention does not modulate neural response to social stimuli in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage, 31, 1614–1624.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Boucher, J., & Bowler, D. M. (2008). Memory in autism: Theory and evidence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Boucher, J., & Bowler, D. M. (2008). Memory in autism: Theory and evidence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Bowler, D. M. (1992). “Theory of mind” in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 877–893.PubMedCrossRef Bowler, D. M. (1992). “Theory of mind” in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 877–893.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bowler, D. M., Gaigg, S. B., & Gardiner, J. M. (2008). Subjective organisation in the free recall learning of adults with Asperger’s Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(1), 104–113.PubMedCrossRef Bowler, D. M., Gaigg, S. B., & Gardiner, J. M. (2008). Subjective organisation in the free recall learning of adults with Asperger’s Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(1), 104–113.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bowler, D. M., Gardiner, J. M., Grice, S., & Saavalainen, P. (2001). Memory illusions: False recall and recognition in adults with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 2000(109), 663–672. Bowler, D. M., Gardiner, J. M., Grice, S., & Saavalainen, P. (2001). Memory illusions: False recall and recognition in adults with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 2000(109), 663–672.
go back to reference Bowler, D. M., Matthews, N. J., & Gardiner, J. M. (1997). Asperger’s syndrome and memory: Similarity to autism but not amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 35(1), 65–70.PubMedCrossRef Bowler, D. M., Matthews, N. J., & Gardiner, J. M. (1997). Asperger’s syndrome and memory: Similarity to autism but not amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 35(1), 65–70.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brockmole, J. R., & Henderson, J. M. (2005). Prioritization of new objects in real-world scenes: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(5), 857–868.PubMedCrossRef Brockmole, J. R., & Henderson, J. M. (2005). Prioritization of new objects in real-world scenes: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(5), 857–868.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
go back to reference Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occuring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 479–485.PubMedCrossRef Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occuring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 479–485.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Freeth, M., Chapman, P., Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2010). Do gaze cues in complex scenes capture and direct the attention of high functioning adolescents with ASD? Evidence from eye-tracking. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(5), 534–547.PubMedCrossRef Freeth, M., Chapman, P., Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2010). Do gaze cues in complex scenes capture and direct the attention of high functioning adolescents with ASD? Evidence from eye-tracking. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(5), 534–547.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Friedman, A. (1979). Framing pictures: The role of knowledge in automatized encoding and memory for gist. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(578–88), 578–588. Friedman, A. (1979). Framing pictures: The role of knowledge in automatized encoding and memory for gist. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(578–88), 578–588.
go back to reference Gardiner, J. M., Bowler, D. M., & Grice, S. J. (2003). Further evidence of preserved priming and impaired recall in adults with Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(3), 259–269.PubMedCrossRef Gardiner, J. M., Bowler, D. M., & Grice, S. J. (2003). Further evidence of preserved priming and impaired recall in adults with Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(3), 259–269.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goodman, G. S. (1980). Picture memory: How the action schema affects retention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 473–495.PubMedCrossRef Goodman, G. S. (1980). Picture memory: How the action schema affects retention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 473–495.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.PubMedCrossRef Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Happé, F. G. E. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(6), 216–222.PubMedCrossRef Happé, F. G. E. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(6), 216–222.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Happé, F., & Vital, P. (2009). What aspects of autism predispose to talent? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1522), 1369–1375.PubMedCrossRef Happé, F., & Vital, P. (2009). What aspects of autism predispose to talent? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1522), 1369–1375.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Henderson, J. M. (2003). Human gaze control during real-world scene perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 498–504.PubMedCrossRef Henderson, J. M. (2003). Human gaze control during real-world scene perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 498–504.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Henderson, J. M., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). High-level scene perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 243–271.PubMedCrossRef Henderson, J. M., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). High-level scene perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 243–271.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Henderson, J. M., Weeks, P. A., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 210–228.CrossRef Henderson, J. M., Weeks, P. A., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 210–228.CrossRef
go back to reference Hillier, A., Campbell, H., Keillor, J., Phillips, N., & Beversdorf, D. Q. (2007). Decreased false memory for visually presented shapes and symbols among adults on the autism spectrum. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(6), 610–616.PubMedCrossRef Hillier, A., Campbell, H., Keillor, J., Phillips, N., & Beversdorf, D. Q. (2007). Decreased false memory for visually presented shapes and symbols among adults on the autism spectrum. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(6), 610–616.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hudson, J. A. (1988). Children’s memory for atypical actions in script-based stories: Evidence for a disruption effect. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 46, 159–173.CrossRef Hudson, J. A. (1988). Children’s memory for atypical actions in script-based stories: Evidence for a disruption effect. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 46, 159–173.CrossRef
go back to reference Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Research, 40, 1489–1506.PubMedCrossRef Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Research, 40, 1489–1506.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Johnson, M. H., & Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development: The case of face recognition. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell. Johnson, M. H., & Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development: The case of face recognition. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.
go back to reference Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358, 345–360.PubMedCrossRef Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358, 345–360.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002a). Defining and quantifying the social phenotype in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 895–908.PubMedCrossRef Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002a). Defining and quantifying the social phenotype in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 895–908.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002b). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816.PubMedCrossRef Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002b). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature, 459(7244), 257–261.PubMedCrossRef Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature, 459(7244), 257–261.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Koch, C., & Ullman, S. (1985). Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards the underlying neural capacity. Human Neurobiology, 4, 219–227.PubMed Koch, C., & Ullman, S. (1985). Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards the underlying neural capacity. Human Neurobiology, 4, 219–227.PubMed
go back to reference Loftus, G. R., & Mackworth, N. H. (1978). Cognitive determinants of fixation location during picture viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 565–572.PubMedCrossRef Loftus, G. R., & Mackworth, N. H. (1978). Cognitive determinants of fixation location during picture viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 565–572.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communciation deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communciation deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Loth, E., Gómez, J. C., & Happé, F. (2008a). Event representation in autism: The role of theory of mind and weak coherence. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(3), 449–463.PubMedCrossRef Loth, E., Gómez, J. C., & Happé, F. (2008a). Event representation in autism: The role of theory of mind and weak coherence. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(3), 449–463.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Loth, E., Gómez, J. C., & Happé, F. (2008b). Detecting changes in naturalistic scenes: Contextual inconsistency does not influence spontaneous attention in high-functioning people with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 1(3), 179–188.PubMedCrossRef Loth, E., Gómez, J. C., & Happé, F. (2008b). Detecting changes in naturalistic scenes: Contextual inconsistency does not influence spontaneous attention in high-functioning people with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 1(3), 179–188.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Loth, E., Gómez, J. C., & Happé, F. (2010a). When seeing depends on knowing: Adults with autism spectrum conditions show diminished top-down processes in the visual perception of degraded faces but not degraded objects. Neuropsychologia, 48(5), 1227–1236.PubMedCrossRef Loth, E., Gómez, J. C., & Happé, F. (2010a). When seeing depends on knowing: Adults with autism spectrum conditions show diminished top-down processes in the visual perception of degraded faces but not degraded objects. Neuropsychologia, 48(5), 1227–1236.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Loth, E., Happé, F., & Gómez, J. C. (2010b). Variety is not the spice of life for people wih autism spectrum disorders: Frequency ratings of central, variable, and inappropriate aspects of common real-life events. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(6), 730–742.PubMedCrossRef Loth, E., Happé, F., & Gómez, J. C. (2010b). Variety is not the spice of life for people wih autism spectrum disorders: Frequency ratings of central, variable, and inappropriate aspects of common real-life events. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(6), 730–742.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Loveland, K. A., & Tunali, B. (1991). Social scripts for conversational interactions in autism and Down’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 177–186.PubMedCrossRef Loveland, K. A., & Tunali, B. (1991). Social scripts for conversational interactions in autism and Down’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 177–186.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mackworth, N. H., & Morandi, A. J. (1967). The gaze selects informative details within pictures. Perception and Psychophysics, 2, 547–552.CrossRef Mackworth, N. H., & Morandi, A. J. (1967). The gaze selects informative details within pictures. Perception and Psychophysics, 2, 547–552.CrossRef
go back to reference Mandler, J. M. (1979). Categorical and schematic organization in memory. Memory organization and structure. New York: Academic Press. Mandler, J. M. (1979). Categorical and schematic organization in memory. Memory organization and structure. New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Nelson, K. (1986). Event knowledge. Structure and function in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Nelson, K. (1986). Event knowledge. Structure and function in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.PubMedCrossRef Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Palmer, S. (1975). The effects of contextual scenes on the identification of objects. Memory and Cognition, 3, 519–526.CrossRef Palmer, S. (1975). The effects of contextual scenes on the identification of objects. Memory and Cognition, 3, 519–526.CrossRef
go back to reference Parkhurst, D., Law, K., & Niebur, E. (2002). Modeling the role of salience in the allocation of overt visual selective attention. Vision Research, 42, 422–426.CrossRef Parkhurst, D., Law, K., & Niebur, E. (2002). Modeling the role of salience in the allocation of overt visual selective attention. Vision Research, 42, 422–426.CrossRef
go back to reference Pitchert, J., & Anderson, R. C. (1977). Taking different perspectives on a story. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 309–315.CrossRef Pitchert, J., & Anderson, R. C. (1977). Taking different perspectives on a story. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 309–315.CrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.PubMedCrossRef Posner, M. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rayner, K., Smith, T. J., Malcolm, G. L., & Henderson, J. M. (2009). Eye movements and visual encoding during scene perception. Psychological Science, 20(1), 6–10.PubMedCrossRef Rayner, K., Smith, T. J., Malcolm, G. L., & Henderson, J. M. (2009). Eye movements and visual encoding during scene perception. Psychological Science, 20(1), 6–10.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8, 368–373.CrossRef Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8, 368–373.CrossRef
go back to reference Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2002). Shape constancy in autism: The role of prior knowledge and perspective cues. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(5), 647–653.PubMedCrossRef Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2002). Shape constancy in autism: The role of prior knowledge and perspective cues. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(5), 647–653.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rosenholtz, R. (1999). A simple saliency model predicts a number of motion popout phenomena. Vision Research, 39, 3157–3163.PubMedCrossRef Rosenholtz, R. (1999). A simple saliency model predicts a number of motion popout phenomena. Vision Research, 39, 3157–3163.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schank, R., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Schank, R., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Scott, F. J., Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., & Brayne, C. (2002). The CAST (childhood Asperger syndrome test): Preliminary development of a UK screen for mainstream primary-school-age children. Autism, 6(1), 9–31.PubMedCrossRef Scott, F. J., Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., & Brayne, C. (2002). The CAST (childhood Asperger syndrome test): Preliminary development of a UK screen for mainstream primary-school-age children. Autism, 6(1), 9–31.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Silva, M. M., Groeger, J. A., & Bradshaw, F. (2006). Attention-memory interactions in scene perception. Spatial Vision, 19(1), 9–19.PubMedCrossRef Silva, M. M., Groeger, J. A., & Bradshaw, F. (2006). Attention-memory interactions in scene perception. Spatial Vision, 19(1), 9–19.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tager-Flusberg, H. (1991). Semantic processing in the free recall of autistic children: Further evidence for a cognitive deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 417–430. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1991). Semantic processing in the free recall of autistic children: Further evidence for a cognitive deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 417–430.
go back to reference Trillingsgaard, A. (1999). The script model in relation to autism. European Journal of Adolescent Psychiatry, 8(1), 45–49.CrossRef Trillingsgaard, A. (1999). The script model in relation to autism. European Journal of Adolescent Psychiatry, 8(1), 45–49.CrossRef
go back to reference Volden, J., & Johnston, J. (1999). Cognitive scripts in autistic children and adolescents. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(3), 203–211.PubMedCrossRef Volden, J., & Johnston, J. (1999). Cognitive scripts in autistic children and adolescents. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(3), 203–211.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment. Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
go back to reference World Health Organization (WHO). (1993). ICD-10 : The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines, Geneva, Switzerland. World Health Organization (WHO). (1993). ICD-10 : The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines, Geneva, Switzerland.
go back to reference Yarbus, A. (1967). Eye movements and vision. New York: Plenum Press. Yarbus, A. (1967). Eye movements and vision. New York: Plenum Press.
Metagegevens
Titel
Do High-Functioning People with Autism Spectrum Disorder Spontaneously Use Event Knowledge to Selectively Attend to and Remember Context-Relevant Aspects in Scenes?
Auteurs
Eva Loth
Juan Carlós Gómez
Francesca Happé
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 7/2011
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1124-6

Andere artikelen Uitgave 7/2011

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 7/2011 Naar de uitgave