Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 9/2018

03-04-2018 | Original Paper

Do Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Benefit from Structural Alignment When Constructing Categories?

Auteurs: Simon Snape, Andrea Krott, Joseph P. McCleery

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 9/2018

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Individuals with ASD seem to construct categories via processes different to typically developing individuals. We examined whether individuals with ASD engage in structural alignment of exemplars when constructing categories. We taught children with ASD and typically developing children novel nouns for either single or multiple exemplars, and then examined their extensions of the learned nouns to objects that were either a perceptual or conceptual match to the original exemplar(s). Results indicated that, unlike typically developing participants, those with ASD gained no benefit from seeing multiple exemplars of the category and, thus, did not appear to engage in structural alignment in their formation of categories. However, they demonstrated superior performance compared to typically developing children when presented with a single exemplar.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Arunachalam, S., & Luyster, R. J. (2016). The integrity of lexical acquisition mechanisms in autism spectrum disorder: A research review. Autism Research, 9, 810–828.CrossRefPubMed Arunachalam, S., & Luyster, R. J. (2016). The integrity of lexical acquisition mechanisms in autism spectrum disorder: A research review. Autism Research, 9, 810–828.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6, 248–254.CrossRef Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6, 248–254.CrossRef
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). The hyper-systemizing, assertive mating theory of autism. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 30, 865–872.CrossRef Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). The hyper-systemizing, assertive mating theory of autism. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 30, 865–872.CrossRef
go back to reference Bott, L., Brock, J., Brockdorff, N., Boucher, J., & Lamberts, K. (2006). Perceptual similarity in autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1237–1254.CrossRefPubMed Bott, L., Brock, J., Brockdorff, N., Boucher, J., & Lamberts, K. (2006). Perceptual similarity in autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1237–1254.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Carlson, S., & Moses, L. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053.CrossRefPubMed Carlson, S., & Moses, L. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Church, B., Krauss, M., Lopata, C., Toomey, J., Thomeer, M., Coutinho, M., Volker, M., & Mercado, M. (2010). Atypical categorization in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 862–868.CrossRef Church, B., Krauss, M., Lopata, C., Toomey, J., Thomeer, M., Coutinho, M., Volker, M., & Mercado, M. (2010). Atypical categorization in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 862–868.CrossRef
go back to reference Clark, E. V. (1973). What’s in a word: On the child’s acquisition of semantics in his first language. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press. Clark, E. V. (1973). What’s in a word: On the child’s acquisition of semantics in his first language. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Craig, F., Margari, F., Legrottaglie, A. R., Palumbi, R., de Giambattista, C., & Margari, L. (2016). A review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 12, 1191–1202.PubMedPubMedCentral Craig, F., Margari, F., Legrottaglie, A. R., Palumbi, R., de Giambattista, C., & Margari, L. (2016). A review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 12, 1191–1202.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Diesendruck, G., Markson, L., & Bloom, P. (2003). Children’s reliance on creator’s intent in extending names for artifacts. Psychological Science, 14, 164–168.CrossRefPubMed Diesendruck, G., Markson, L., & Bloom, P. (2003). Children’s reliance on creator’s intent in extending names for artifacts. Psychological Science, 14, 164–168.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dunn, M., & Bates, J. (2005). Developmental change in neural processing of words by children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 361–376.CrossRefPubMed Dunn, M., & Bates, J. (2005). Developmental change in neural processing of words by children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 361–376.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dunn, M., Vaughan, H., Kreuzer, J., & Kurtzberg, D. (1999). Electrophysiologic correlates of semantic classification in autistic and normal children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16, 79–99.CrossRef Dunn, M., Vaughan, H., Kreuzer, J., & Kurtzberg, D. (1999). Electrophysiologic correlates of semantic classification in autistic and normal children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16, 79–99.CrossRef
go back to reference Ellawadi, A. B., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R. (2017). Category structure and processing in 6-year-old children with autism. Autism Research, 10, 327–336.CrossRefPubMed Ellawadi, A. B., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R. (2017). Category structure and processing in 6-year-old children with autism. Autism Research, 10, 327–336.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fay, W. H., & Schuler, A. L. (1980). Emerging Language in autistic children. Baltimore: University Park Press. Fay, W. H., & Schuler, A. L. (1980). Emerging Language in autistic children. Baltimore: University Park Press.
go back to reference Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell Science. Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell Science.
go back to reference Gastgeb, H. Z., Dundas, E. M., Minshew, N. J., & Strauss, M. S. (2012). Category formation in autism: Can individuals with autism form categories and prototypes of dot patterns? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1694–1704.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gastgeb, H. Z., Dundas, E. M., Minshew, N. J., & Strauss, M. S. (2012). Category formation in autism: Can individuals with autism form categories and prototypes of dot patterns? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1694–1704.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Gastgeb, H. Z., Strauss, M. S., & Minshew, N. J. (2006). Do individuals with autism process categories differently? The effect of typicality and development. Child Development, 77, 1717–1729.CrossRefPubMed Gastgeb, H. Z., Strauss, M. S., & Minshew, N. J. (2006). Do individuals with autism process categories differently? The effect of typicality and development. Child Development, 77, 1717–1729.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gentner, D. (1982). A study of early word meaning using artificial objects: What looks like a jiggy but acts like a zimbo? In J. Gardner (Ed.), Readings in developmental psychology. Boston: Little Brown. Gentner, D. (1982). A study of early word meaning using artificial objects: What looks like a jiggy but acts like a zimbo? In J. Gardner (Ed.), Readings in developmental psychology. Boston: Little Brown.
go back to reference Gentner, D. (2003). Why we’re so smart. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 195–235). Cambridge: MIT Press. Gentner, D. (2003). Why we’re so smart. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 195–235). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Gentner, D., & Markman, A. B. (1997). Structure mapping in analogy and similarity. American Psychologist, 52, 45–56.CrossRef Gentner, D., & Markman, A. B. (1997). Structure mapping in analogy and similarity. American Psychologist, 52, 45–56.CrossRef
go back to reference Gentner, D., & Namy, L. L. (1999). Comparison in the development of categories. Cognitive Development, 14, 487–513.CrossRef Gentner, D., & Namy, L. L. (1999). Comparison in the development of categories. Cognitive Development, 14, 487–513.CrossRef
go back to reference Gentner, D., & Namy, L. L. (2006). Analogical processes in language learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 297–301.CrossRef Gentner, D., & Namy, L. L. (2006). Analogical processes in language learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 297–301.CrossRef
go back to reference Gentner, D., Rattermann, M. J., & Forbus, K. D. (1993). The roles of similarity in transfer: Separating retrievability and inferential soundness. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 524–575.CrossRefPubMed Gentner, D., Rattermann, M. J., & Forbus, K. D. (1993). The roles of similarity in transfer: Separating retrievability and inferential soundness. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 524–575.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gershkoff-Stowe, L., & Smith, L. B. (2004). Shape and the first hundred nouns. Child Development, 75, 1098–1114.CrossRefPubMed Gershkoff-Stowe, L., & Smith, L. B. (2004). Shape and the first hundred nouns. Child Development, 75, 1098–1114.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1–38.CrossRef Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1–38.CrossRef
go back to reference Goldstone, R. L., Medin, D. L., & Gentner, D. (1991). Relational similarity and the nonindependence of features in similarity judgments. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 222–264.CrossRefPubMed Goldstone, R. L., Medin, D. L., & Gentner, D. (1991). Relational similarity and the nonindependence of features in similarity judgments. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 222–264.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Graham, S. A., Williams, L. D., & Huber, J. F. (1999). Preschoolers’ and adults’ reliance on object shape and object function for lexical extension. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74, 128–151.CrossRefPubMed Graham, S. A., Williams, L. D., & Huber, J. F. (1999). Preschoolers’ and adults’ reliance on object shape and object function for lexical extension. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74, 128–151.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Happe, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 216–222.CrossRefPubMed Happe, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 216–222.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak central coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.CrossRefPubMed Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak central coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hartley, C., & Allen, M. L. (2014). Generalisation of word-picture relations in children with autism and typically developing children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 2064–2071.CrossRefPubMed Hartley, C., & Allen, M. L. (2014). Generalisation of word-picture relations in children with autism and typically developing children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 2064–2071.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Imai, M., Gentner, D., & Uchida, N. (1994). Children’s theories of word meaning: The role of shape similarity in early acquisition. Cognitive Development, 9, 45–75.CrossRef Imai, M., Gentner, D., & Uchida, N. (1994). Children’s theories of word meaning: The role of shape similarity in early acquisition. Cognitive Development, 9, 45–75.CrossRef
go back to reference Jackendoff, R. (1983). Semantics and cognition. Cambridge: MIT Press. Jackendoff, R. (1983). Semantics and cognition. Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Kelley, E., Paul, J. J., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R. (2006). Residual language deficits in optimal outcome children with a history of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 807–828.CrossRefPubMed Kelley, E., Paul, J. J., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. R. (2006). Residual language deficits in optimal outcome children with a history of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 807–828.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Klenberg, L., Korkman, M., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (2001). Differential development of attention and executive functions in 3- to 12-year-old Finnish children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 407–428.CrossRefPubMed Klenberg, L., Korkman, M., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (2001). Differential development of attention and executive functions in 3- to 12-year-old Finnish children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 407–428.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Klinger, L. G., & Dawson, G. (2001). Prototype formation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 111–124.CrossRefPubMed Klinger, L. G., & Dawson, G. (2001). Prototype formation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 111–124.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Landau, B., Smith, L., & Jones, S. (1998). Object shape, object function, and object name. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 1–27.CrossRef Landau, B., Smith, L., & Jones, S. (1998). Object shape, object function, and object name. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 1–27.CrossRef
go back to reference Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3, 299–321.CrossRef Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3, 299–321.CrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., Jr. DiLavore, P. C., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 30, 205–223.CrossRef Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., Jr. DiLavore, P. C., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 30, 205–223.CrossRef
go back to reference Mandler, J. M., & Bauer, P. J. (1988). The cradle of categorization: Is the basic level basic? Cognitive Development, 3, 247–264.CrossRef Mandler, J. M., & Bauer, P. J. (1988). The cradle of categorization: Is the basic level basic? Cognitive Development, 3, 247–264.CrossRef
go back to reference Markman, A. B., & Gentner, D. (1993). Structural alignment during similarity comparisons. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 431–467.CrossRef Markman, A. B., & Gentner, D. (1993). Structural alignment during similarity comparisons. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 431–467.CrossRef
go back to reference Markman, A. B., & Gentner, D. (1996). Commonalities and differences in similarity comparisons. Memory & Cognition, 24, 235–249.CrossRef Markman, A. B., & Gentner, D. (1996). Commonalities and differences in similarity comparisons. Memory & Cognition, 24, 235–249.CrossRef
go back to reference Markson, L., Diesendruck, G., & Bloom, P. (2008). The shape of thought. Developmental Science, 11, 204–208.CrossRefPubMed Markson, L., Diesendruck, G., & Bloom, P. (2008). The shape of thought. Developmental Science, 11, 204–208.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Medin, D. L., Goldstone, R. L., & Gentner, D. (1993). Respects for similarity. Psychological Review, 100, 254–278.CrossRef Medin, D. L., Goldstone, R. L., & Gentner, D. (1993). Respects for similarity. Psychological Review, 100, 254–278.CrossRef
go back to reference Menyuk, P. (1978). Language: What’s wrong and why. In M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A reappraisal of concepts and treatment (pp. 105–116). New York: Plenum.CrossRef Menyuk, P. (1978). Language: What’s wrong and why. In M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A reappraisal of concepts and treatment (pp. 105–116). New York: Plenum.CrossRef
go back to reference Merriman, W. E., Scott, P. D., & Marazita, J. (1993). An appearance function shift in children’s object naming. Journal of Child Language, 20, 101–118.CrossRefPubMed Merriman, W. E., Scott, P. D., & Marazita, J. (1993). An appearance function shift in children’s object naming. Journal of Child Language, 20, 101–118.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Minshew, N. J., Meyer, J., & Goldstein, G. (2002). Abstract reasoning in autism: A dissociation between concept formation and concept identification. Neuropsychology, 16, 327.CrossRefPubMed Minshew, N. J., Meyer, J., & Goldstein, G. (2002). Abstract reasoning in autism: A dissociation between concept formation and concept identification. Neuropsychology, 16, 327.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning: AGS edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service Inc. Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning: AGS edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service Inc.
go back to reference Naigles, L. R., Kelley, E., Troyb, E., & Fein, D. (2013). Residual differences with categorical induction in children with a history of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 2048–2061.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Naigles, L. R., Kelley, E., Troyb, E., & Fein, D. (2013). Residual differences with categorical induction in children with a history of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 2048–2061.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Naigles, L. R., & Tek, S. (2017). ‘Form is easy, meaning is hard’ revisited: (Re) characterizing the strengths and weaknesses of language in children with autism spectrum disorder. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1438.PubMed Naigles, L. R., & Tek, S. (2017). ‘Form is easy, meaning is hard’ revisited: (Re) characterizing the strengths and weaknesses of language in children with autism spectrum disorder. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​wcs.​1438.PubMed
go back to reference Namy, L. L., & Gentner, D. (2002). Making a silk purse out of two sow’s ears: Young children’s use of comparison in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 5–15.CrossRef Namy, L. L., & Gentner, D. (2002). Making a silk purse out of two sow’s ears: Young children’s use of comparison in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 5–15.CrossRef
go back to reference Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 149, 1–138.CrossRef Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 149, 1–138.CrossRef
go back to reference O’Hearn, K., Asato, M., Ordaz, S., & Luna, B. (2008). Neurodevelopment and executive function in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 1103–1132.CrossRefPubMed O’Hearn, K., Asato, M., Ordaz, S., & Luna, B. (2008). Neurodevelopment and executive function in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 1103–1132.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McMahon, W. M., & Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function abilities in autism and tourette syndrome: An information processing approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1015–1032.CrossRefPubMed Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McMahon, W. M., & Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function abilities in autism and tourette syndrome: An information processing approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1015–1032.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pickles, A., Anderson, D. K., & Lord, C. (2014). Heterogeneity and plasticity in the development of language: A 17-year follow-up of children referred early for possible autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 1354–1362.CrossRefPubMed Pickles, A., Anderson, D. K., & Lord, C. (2014). Heterogeneity and plasticity in the development of language: A 17-year follow-up of children referred early for possible autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 1354–1362.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Plaisted, K. C. (2000). Aspects of autism that theory of mind cannot explain. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press. Plaisted, K. C. (2000). Aspects of autism that theory of mind cannot explain. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Potrzeba, E. R., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. (2015). Investigating the shape bias in typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 446.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Potrzeba, E. R., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. (2015). Investigating the shape bias in typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 446.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439.CrossRef Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439.CrossRef
go back to reference Samuelson, L. K., & Smith, L. B. (1999). Early noun vocabularies: do ontology, category organization and syntax correspond? Cognition, 73, 1–33.CrossRefPubMed Samuelson, L. K., & Smith, L. B. (1999). Early noun vocabularies: do ontology, category organization and syntax correspond? Cognition, 73, 1–33.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schuler, A. L., & Bormann, C. (1982). The interrelations between cognitive and communicative development. In C. E. Johnson & C. L. Thew. In Proceedings of the second international congress for the study of child language. Lanham: University Press of America. Schuler, A. L., & Bormann, C. (1982). The interrelations between cognitive and communicative development. In C. E. Johnson & C. L. Thew. In Proceedings of the second international congress for the study of child language. Lanham: University Press of America.
go back to reference Shulman, C., Yirmiya, N., & Greenbaum, C. W. (1995). From categorization to classification: A comparison among individuals with autism, mental retardation, and normal development. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 601–609.CrossRefPubMed Shulman, C., Yirmiya, N., & Greenbaum, C. W. (1995). From categorization to classification: A comparison among individuals with autism, mental retardation, and normal development. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 601–609.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S., & Landau, B. (1996). Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism? Cognition, 60, 143–171.CrossRefPubMed Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S., & Landau, B. (1996). Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism? Cognition, 60, 143–171.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Snape, S., & Krott, A. (2018). The role of inhibition in moving beyond perceptually focused noun extensions. First Language, 38, 95–108.CrossRef Snape, S., & Krott, A. (2018). The role of inhibition in moving beyond perceptually focused noun extensions. First Language, 38, 95–108.CrossRef
go back to reference Soulieres, I., Mottron, L., Giguere, G., & Larochelle, S. (2011). Category induction in autism: Slower, perhaps different, but certainly possible. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 311–327.CrossRefPubMed Soulieres, I., Mottron, L., Giguere, G., & Larochelle, S. (2011). Category induction in autism: Slower, perhaps different, but certainly possible. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 311–327.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Soulieres, I., Mottron, L., Saumier, D., & Larochelle, S. (2007). Atypical categorical perception in autism: Autonomy of discrimination? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 481–490.CrossRefPubMed Soulieres, I., Mottron, L., Saumier, D., & Larochelle, S. (2007). Atypical categorical perception in autism: Autonomy of discrimination? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 481–490.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tager-Flusberg, H. (1985a). Basic level and superordinate level categorization in autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 450–469. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1985a). Basic level and superordinate level categorization in autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 450–469.
go back to reference Tager-Flusberg, H. (1985b). The conceptual basis for referential word meaning in children with autism. Child Development, 56, 1167–1178.CrossRefPubMed Tager-Flusberg, H. (1985b). The conceptual basis for referential word meaning in children with autism. Child Development, 56, 1167–1178.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Strategies for conducting research on language in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 75–80.CrossRefPubMed Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Strategies for conducting research on language in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 75–80.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tek, S., Jaffery, G., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. (2008). Do children with autism spectrum disorders show a shape bias in word learning? Autism Research, 1, 208–222.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Tek, S., Jaffery, G., Fein, D., & Naigles, L. (2008). Do children with autism spectrum disorders show a shape bias in word learning? Autism Research, 1, 208–222.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Ungerer, J., & Sigman, M. (1987). Categorization skills and receptive language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 3–16.CrossRefPubMed Ungerer, J., & Sigman, M. (1987). Categorization skills and receptive language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 3–16.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wittke, K., Mastergeorge, A. M., Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S. J., & Naigles, L. R. (2017). Grammatical language impairment in autism spectrum disorder: Exploring language phenotypes beyond standardized testing. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 532.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wittke, K., Mastergeorge, A. M., Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S. J., & Naigles, L. R. (2017). Grammatical language impairment in autism spectrum disorder: Exploring language phenotypes beyond standardized testing. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 532.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metagegevens
Titel
Do Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Benefit from Structural Alignment When Constructing Categories?
Auteurs
Simon Snape
Andrea Krott
Joseph P. McCleery
Publicatiedatum
03-04-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 9/2018
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3551-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 9/2018

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 9/2018 Naar de uitgave