In adolescence, engaging in prosocial behavior (i.e., voluntary actions undertaken to benefit others; Eisenberg et al.,
2015) has been linked to increased social acceptance (Layous et al.,
2012), higher friendship quality (Markievicz et al.,
2001), a lower risk for internalizing and externalizing problems (e.g., Memmott-Elison et al.,
2020) and better school performances (e.g., Caprara et al.,
2000). It is therefore essential to identify social conditions that promote prosocial behaviors during adolescence, a period of heightened susceptibility to peer influence (Veenstra & Laninga-Wijnen,
2022). Peer influence ‒ adopting the behavior of other peers ‒ plays an important role in the development of aggression (e.g., Laninga‐Wijnen et al.,
2017). Little is known, however, about the role of peers in prosocial behaviors. The few studies on this topic only considered the role of (best) friends (e.g., Crone et al.,
2022), although adolescents may be influenced by peers other than their friends (Gommans et al.,
2017), such as peers they like and would like to hang out with
1. An important reason why adolescents may try to adopt the behaviors of the peers they would like to hang out with – henceforth referred to as “peers they like” for readability - is that it increases their chance to actually hang out or become friends with this peer (Bowker,
2004). Using a stochastic actor-oriented model (SAOM; Snijders et al.,
2010) with three waves of data, the current study first aims to examine whether adolescents are influenced by the peers they like in their prosocial behavior. As not all peers are equally influential or equally susceptible to influence (Delay et al.,
2022), it was also investigated whether these effects were more likely (1) when the peers whom adolescents liked were also well-liked by classmates in general, and (2) when the adolescents themselves were not well-liked by classmates in general.
Peer Influence and Changes in Prosocial Behavior
Most peer influence studies on prosocial behavior have been based on friendship networks (i.e., friend ties), testing whether adolescents became similar in prosocial behaviors (or changed their behavior in a similar direction) to the ones they nominated as friends. However, the findings have been inconsistent: three social network studies on children and adolescents from three different cultures (i.e., Chilean, American, and Korean) found
no evidence for friendship influence on prosocial behavior in early adolescents (Dijkstra & Berger,
2018; Molano et al.,
2013; Shin,
2017), whereas three other studies detected significant influence effects but in two different directions. In two out of these three studies, they indicated that adolescents were more likely to decrease rather than increase prosocial behavior as a result of friendship influence (Laninga‐Wijnen et al.,
2019; Logis et al.,
2013). This influence of friendship may occur via various mechanisms. Adolescents may become more similar to their friends by spending much time together, observing their behavior, and mimicking each other consciously or unconsciously (perception-behavior paradigm; Chartrand & Bargh,
1999). It is also possible that friends mutually discuss how they should approach social situations and learn through mutual encouragement how they can socially interact with each other as well as with other peers (Laninga-Wijnen & Veenstra,
2021). Since friendship affiliation plays an important role during adolescence by providing a sense of belonging and emotional support (Hartup & Stevens,
1997), becoming more similar to one’s friends may be a way to maintain harmony in the friendship and preserve the friendship itself. Alternatively, they may adopt prosocial behaviors because they see their friends being rewarded (e.g., their friends receiving positive feedback or attention from peers) for their prosocial behavior.
Even though previous social network studies are valuable, they have overlooked the fact that adolescents are surrounded by peers other than their close friends (Gommans et al.,
2017), who may also influence their prosocial behaviors. Some experimental work has examined this question by considering the role of anonymous peers. For instance, one study with Korean college students found that “anonymous confederates” in the immediate social context influenced students’ participation in a campaign by money donation. This suggests that even the prosocial behavior of
anonymous peers - with whom adolescents do not have any relationship - exerted influence on the participants’ prosocial behavior (Park & Shin,
2017).
Adolescents might also be influenced by peers they
like. Peer influence processes in liking networks may operate through mechanisms similar to friendships (e.g., imitation) but also through distinct ones, such as aspiration, where individuals conform themselves to the higher values of peers’ behaviors (Snijders & Lomi,
2019). For instance, adolescents may like peers who are not (yet) their friends but with whom they desire to affiliate or become friends. Indeed, peers who are liked by classmates appear to be highly sought after for friendships (Thomas & Bowker,
2013). This preference might be because liked adolescents tend to be prosocial and trustworthy (Parkhurst & Hopmeyer,
1998). Thus, conforming to these peers’ behaviors may be a way to develop a stronger connection with them, paving the way for friendship formation. As friendships tend to be relatively stable and formed with only a limited number of peers, conforming to the peers they like might extend adolescents’ opportunities for social connections and recreational activities. In addition, adolescents are particularly sensitive to peer reward regarding positive interactions (Cho & Hackel,
2022), and any sign of acceptance, especially from the peers they like, is probably important for their self-esteem and sense of belonging. Even without such reinforcement, aligning with the values and behaviors of peers they like and admire from a distance might help adolescents develop a favorable self-concept (Brechwald & Prinstein,
2011).
Who Is More Influential? The Role of High-Status Peers
An important determinant of behavioral influence during adolescence is high status (Field et al.,
2023). Research has highlighted two distinct aspects of social standing in the peer group: popularity and likeability. Popularity is a reputation-based type of peer status associated with more visibility within a peer group (Cillessen & Rose,
2005). Likeability, instead, reflects personal affection and is assessed as the degree to which an adolescent is well-liked and accepted by peers (Laursen et al.,
2023). Popularity and likeability are not mutually exclusive, but they represent different types of status with different implications for behavior (van den Berg et al.,
2020).
Adolescents’ tendency to conform to popular peers is well-established, but little is known about the influential power of highly liked peers. Well-liked peers may be attractive role models because they are well-embedded in the peer group and usually display positive behaviors that make them attractive peers with whom to interact and be affiliated (Thomas & Bowker,
2013). Additionally, well-liked peers may be considered role models particularly for adaptive behaviors because they distinguish themselves from other peers by being well-adjusted and displaying socially desirable behaviors that improve their well-being (de Bruine et al.,
2019). Indeed, well-liked peers have been found to be stronger norm-setters for academic behavior than popular peers (Kwon & Lease,
2014). Moreover, as likeability is positively linked to altruistic prosociality and communal goals (Findley-Van Nostrand & Ojanen,
2018), well-liked students may be powerful role models for prosocial behavior, since the prosocial behaviors they display generally arise from a desire to benefit others without expecting personal gain (Costello & Zozula,
2018), and therefore may be seen as more
genuine (Adler & Adler,
1995). Yet another reason for conforming to well-liked peers is the wish to increase one’s own likeability in the peer group, an argument often made for conforming to popular peers (e.g., basking in reflected glory; Dijkstra et al.,
2010).
Despite the consistent positive association between likeability and prosocial behaviors found in the literature, little is known regarding the influential power of well-liked peers on others’ prosocial behavior. To date, only one experimental study with early adolescents has revealed that the intent to behave prosocially (e.g., volunteering) was stronger when such behavior was previously displayed by highly popular peers or by well-liked peers than when it was displayed by low-status peers (Choukas-Bradley et al.,
2015): watching high-status e-confederates endorsing prosocial responses in hypothetical scenarios encouraged participants to increase their prosocial endorsement more as compared to watching low-status e-confederates endorsing prosocial behavior. Even though experimental designs provide accurate and reliable results by randomizing participants, their external validity is limited. Furthermore, when examining peer influence, it is important to prevent the misattribution of selection effects (i.e., the idea that similarly prosocial students are more likely to like each other) as social influence. This can be done by controlling for selection effects, which requires using a social network approach (Steglich et al.,
2010).
Susceptibility to Prosocial Peer Influence
Susceptibility to peer influence can be defined as the likelihood that peer conformity will occur (Laursen & Faur,
2022). In some cases, peer conformity occurs due to the characteristics of the influencer, but in other occasions, it is a product of the characteristics of the influencer and of the target of influence (Delay et al.,
2022). Adolescence is an important period for peer influence because youth brains are still malleable and oriented toward the social environment, which facilitates behavioral modifications through peer influence (Telzer et al.,
2018). However, not everyone is equally susceptible to peer influence (Laursen & Veenstra,
2023). For instance, adolescents with few alternative opportunities for friendships are more susceptible to the influence of friends (Faur et al.,
2022), and younger adolescents are more susceptible to peer influence regarding prosocial behavior (Foulkes et al.,
2018; Ahmed et al.,
2020).
Because being liked by peers fulfills belonging needs (Baumeister & Leary,
1995), it is plausible that youth with lower levels of likeability are especially susceptible to the influence of highly liked peers. Less-liked adolescents have fewer friends (Stotsky & Bowker,
2018), and their susceptibility may be driven both by the desire to become more accepted by well-liked peers (the source of influence) and by the hope of being rewarded with more connections. Prosocial behavior is crucial for developing harmonious social relationships and may be especially important in adolescence when there is a heightened need for social belonging (Crone & Achterberg,
2022). Additionally, less-liked peers tend to be less prosocial (Eisenberg et al.,
2015) and thus may have more ‘room’ to improve.