Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2009

01-05-2009 | Original Article

Dissociating cognitive and motor interference effects on kinesthetic short-term memory

Auteurs: Waldemar Kirsch, Erwin Hennighausen, Frank Rösler

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2009

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In two experiments, we investigated how short-term memory of kinesthetically defined spatial locations suffers from either motor or cognitive distraction. In Exp. 1, 22 blindfolded participants moved a handle with their right hand towards a mechanical stop and back to the start and then reproduced the encoded stop position by a second movement. The retention interval was adjusted to approximately 0 and 8 s. In half of the trials participants had to provide a verbal judgment of the target distance after encoding (cognitive distractor). Analyses of constant and variable errors indicated that the verbal judgments interfered with the motor reproduction only, when the retention interval was long. In Exp. 2, 22 other participants performed the same task but instead of providing verbal distance estimations they performed an additional movement either with their right or left hand during the retention interval. Constant error was affected by the side of the interpolated movement (right vs. left hand) and by the delay interval. The results show that reproduction of kinesthetically encoded spatial locations is affected differently in long- and short-retention intervals by cognitive and motor interference. This suggests that reproduction behavior is based on distinct codes during immediate vs. delayed recall.
Voetnoten
1
We report uncorrected degrees of freedom and corrected p values in the “Results” section.
 
2
In similar experimental setups, we found that also the standard deviation, i.e. a measure not relativized to the mean amplitude, did not reflect the typically observed increase in variability with distance. In contrast, the pattern resembled the reported result, i.e. increasing distance was associated with a decrease in response variability (see Kirsch et al., 2008).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 701–725.CrossRef Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 701–725.CrossRef
go back to reference Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., & Starkes, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 6–16.PubMedCrossRef Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., & Starkes, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 6–16.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bradshaw, M. F., & Watt, S. J. (2002). A dissociation of perception and action in normal human observers: the effect of temporal delay. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1766–1778.PubMedCrossRef Bradshaw, M. F., & Watt, S. J. (2002). A dissociation of perception and action in normal human observers: the effect of temporal delay. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1766–1778.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bridgeman, B., Gemmer, A., Forsman, T., & Huemer, V. (2000). Processing spatial information in the sensorimotor branch of the visual system. Vision Research, 40, 3539–3552.PubMedCrossRef Bridgeman, B., Gemmer, A., Forsman, T., & Huemer, V. (2000). Processing spatial information in the sensorimotor branch of the visual system. Vision Research, 40, 3539–3552.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Diewert, G. L. (1975). Retention and coding in motor short-term memory: A comparison of storage codes for distance and location information. Journal of Motor Behavior, 7, 183–190. Diewert, G. L. (1975). Retention and coding in motor short-term memory: A comparison of storage codes for distance and location information. Journal of Motor Behavior, 7, 183–190.
go back to reference Elliott, D., & Madalena, J. (1987). The influence of premovement visual information on manual aiming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39A, 541–559. Elliott, D., & Madalena, J. (1987). The influence of premovement visual information on manual aiming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39A, 541–559.
go back to reference Flanagan, J. R., & Beltzner, M. A. (2000). Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size-weight illusion. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 737–741.PubMedCrossRef Flanagan, J. R., & Beltzner, M. A. (2000). Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size-weight illusion. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 737–741.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of Box’s results on the use of the F distribution in multivariate analysis. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 29, 885–891.CrossRef Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of Box’s results on the use of the F distribution in multivariate analysis. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 29, 885–891.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodale, M. A., Jakobson, L. S., & Keillor, J. M. (1994). Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements. Neuropsychologia, 32, 1159–1178.PubMedCrossRef Goodale, M. A., Jakobson, L. S., & Keillor, J. M. (1994). Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements. Neuropsychologia, 32, 1159–1178.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathway for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 1, 20–25.CrossRef Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathway for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 1, 20–25.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodale, M. A., Westwood, D. A., & Milner, A. (2004). Two distinct modes of control for object-directed action. Progress in Brain Research, 144, 131–144.PubMedCrossRef Goodale, M. A., Westwood, D. A., & Milner, A. (2004). Two distinct modes of control for object-directed action. Progress in Brain Research, 144, 131–144.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grandy, M. S., & Westwood, D. A. (2006). Opposite perceptual and sensorimotor responses to a size-weight illusion. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 3887–3892.PubMedCrossRef Grandy, M. S., & Westwood, D. A. (2006). Opposite perceptual and sensorimotor responses to a size-weight illusion. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 3887–3892.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Granit, R. (1972). Constant errors in the execution and appreciation of movement. Brain, 95, 649–660.CrossRef Granit, R. (1972). Constant errors in the execution and appreciation of movement. Brain, 95, 649–660.CrossRef
go back to reference Hagman, J. D. (1978). Specific-cue effects of interpolated movements on distance and location retention in short-term memory. Memory and Cognition, 6, 432–437. Hagman, J. D. (1978). Specific-cue effects of interpolated movements on distance and location retention in short-term memory. Memory and Cognition, 6, 432–437.
go back to reference Harris, J. A., Harris, I. M., & Diamond, M. E. (2001). Topography of tactile working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 8262–8269.PubMed Harris, J. A., Harris, I. M., & Diamond, M. E. (2001). Topography of tactile working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 8262–8269.PubMed
go back to reference Hu, Y., Eagleson, R., & Goodale, M. A. (1999). The effects of delay on the kinematics of grasping. Experimental Brain Research, 126, 109–116.CrossRef Hu, Y., Eagleson, R., & Goodale, M. A. (1999). The effects of delay on the kinematics of grasping. Experimental Brain Research, 126, 109–116.CrossRef
go back to reference Jeannerod, M., Arbib, M. A., Rizzolatti, G., & Sakata, H. (1995). Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends in Neurosciences, 18, 314–320.PubMedCrossRef Jeannerod, M., Arbib, M. A., Rizzolatti, G., & Sakata, H. (1995). Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends in Neurosciences, 18, 314–320.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jones, L. A. (1986). Perception of force and weight: theory and research. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 29–42.PubMedCrossRef Jones, L. A. (1986). Perception of force and weight: theory and research. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 29–42.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kirsch, W., Hennighausen, E., & Rösler, F. (2008). Electrophysiological markers of encoding and error processing in a motor matching task (in preparation). Kirsch, W., Hennighausen, E., & Rösler, F. (2008). Electrophysiological markers of encoding and error processing in a motor matching task (in preparation).
go back to reference Laabs, G. J. (1973). Retention characteristics of different reproduction cues in motor short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 168–177.PubMedCrossRef Laabs, G. J. (1973). Retention characteristics of different reproduction cues in motor short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 168–177.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Laabs, G. J. (1974). The effect of interpolated motor activity on the short-term retention of movement distance and end-location. Journal of Motor Behavior, 6, 279–288. Laabs, G. J. (1974). The effect of interpolated motor activity on the short-term retention of movement distance and end-location. Journal of Motor Behavior, 6, 279–288.
go back to reference Laabs, G. J. (1976). A note concerning the effect of a kinesthetic memory load on the retention of movement end-location. Journal of Motor Behavior, 8, 313–316. Laabs, G. J. (1976). A note concerning the effect of a kinesthetic memory load on the retention of movement end-location. Journal of Motor Behavior, 8, 313–316.
go back to reference Laabs, G. J., & Simmons, R. W. (1981). Motor memory. In D. H. Holding (Ed.), Human skills (pp. 119–152). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. Laabs, G. J., & Simmons, R. W. (1981). Motor memory. In D. H. Holding (Ed.), Human skills (pp. 119–152). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
go back to reference Laszlo, J. I. (1992). Motor control and learning: how far do the experimental tasks restrict our theoretical insight? In J. J. Summers (Ed.), Approaches to the study of motor control and learning (pp. 47–79). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef Laszlo, J. I. (1992). Motor control and learning: how far do the experimental tasks restrict our theoretical insight? In J. J. Summers (Ed.), Approaches to the study of motor control and learning (pp. 47–79). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef
go back to reference Murray, D. J., Ellis, R. R., Bandomir, C. A., & Ross, H. E. (1999). Charpentier (1891) on the size-weight illusion. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1681–1685. Murray, D. J., Ellis, R. R., Bandomir, C. A., & Ross, H. E. (1999). Charpentier (1891) on the size-weight illusion. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1681–1685.
go back to reference Navon, D., & Gopher, D. (1979). On the economy of the human-processing system. Psychological Review, 86, 214–255.CrossRef Navon, D., & Gopher, D. (1979). On the economy of the human-processing system. Psychological Review, 86, 214–255.CrossRef
go back to reference Norman, J. (2002). Two visual systems and two theories of perception: An attempt to reconcile the constructivist and ecological approaches. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 73–144.PubMed Norman, J. (2002). Two visual systems and two theories of perception: An attempt to reconcile the constructivist and ecological approaches. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 73–144.PubMed
go back to reference Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 44–64.CrossRef Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 44–64.CrossRef
go back to reference Paillard, J. (1987). Cognitive versus sensori-motor encoding of spatial information. In P. Ellen & C. Thinus-Blanc (Eds.), Cognitive processes and spatial orientation in animal and man (pp. 43–77). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff. Paillard, J. (1987). Cognitive versus sensori-motor encoding of spatial information. In P. Ellen & C. Thinus-Blanc (Eds.), Cognitive processes and spatial orientation in animal and man (pp. 43–77). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff.
go back to reference Posner, M. I. (1967). Characteristics of visual and kinesthetic memory codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 103–107.PubMedCrossRef Posner, M. I. (1967). Characteristics of visual and kinesthetic memory codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 103–107.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rossetti, Y. (1998). Implicit short-lived motor representation of space in brain-damaged and healthy subjects. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 520–558.PubMedCrossRef Rossetti, Y. (1998). Implicit short-lived motor representation of space in brain-damaged and healthy subjects. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 520–558.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rossetti, Y., & Pisella, L. (2002). Several vision for action systems: a guide to dissociating and integrating dorsal and ventral functions. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 62–119). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Rossetti, Y., & Pisella, L. (2002). Several vision for action systems: a guide to dissociating and integrating dorsal and ventral functions. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 62–119). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Rossetti, Y., Rode, G., & Boisson, D. (1995). Implicit processing of somaesthetic information: a dissociation between where and how? Neuroreport, 6, 506–510.PubMedCrossRef Rossetti, Y., Rode, G., & Boisson, D. (1995). Implicit processing of somaesthetic information: a dissociation between where and how? Neuroreport, 6, 506–510.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smyth, M. M. (1984). Memory for movements. In M. M. Smyth & A. Wing (Eds.), The psychology of human movement (pp. 83–117). London: Academic Press. Smyth, M. M. (1984). Memory for movements. In M. M. Smyth & A. Wing (Eds.), The psychology of human movement (pp. 83–117). London: Academic Press.
go back to reference Soechting, J. F., & Flanders, M. (1992). Moving in three-dimensional space: Frames of reference, vectors, and coordinate systems. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 167–191.PubMedCrossRef Soechting, J. F., & Flanders, M. (1992). Moving in three-dimensional space: Frames of reference, vectors, and coordinate systems. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 167–191.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tillery, S. I., Flanders, M., & Soechting, J. F. (1994). Errors in kinesthetic transformations for hand apposition. Neuroreport, 6, 177–181.PubMedCrossRef Tillery, S. I., Flanders, M., & Soechting, J. F. (1994). Errors in kinesthetic transformations for hand apposition. Neuroreport, 6, 177–181.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Trumbo, D., Milone, F., & Noble, M. (1972). Interpolated activity and response mechanisms in motor short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 205–212.PubMedCrossRef Trumbo, D., Milone, F., & Noble, M. (1972). Interpolated activity and response mechanisms in motor short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 205–212.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vaillancourt, D. E., & Russell, D. M. (2002). Temporal capacity of short-term visuomotor memory in continuous force production. Experimental Brain Research, 145, 275–285.CrossRef Vaillancourt, D. E., & Russell, D. M. (2002). Temporal capacity of short-term visuomotor memory in continuous force production. Experimental Brain Research, 145, 275–285.CrossRef
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269, 1880–1882.PubMedCrossRef Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269, 1880–1882.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wulf, G., McNevin, N., & Shea, C. H. (2001). The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, A, 54, 1143–1154.CrossRef Wulf, G., McNevin, N., & Shea, C. H. (2001). The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, A, 54, 1143–1154.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Dissociating cognitive and motor interference effects on kinesthetic short-term memory
Auteurs
Waldemar Kirsch
Erwin Hennighausen
Frank Rösler
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2009
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2009
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0159-1

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2009

Psychological Research 3/2009 Naar de uitgave