Preliminary Analyses
Prior to conducting the main analyses of this study, the empathy measures were subjected to independent samples t-tests, to compare DBD individuals who were using methylphenidate with those who were not. No significant group differences emerged on any of the empathy measure (all p’s > .1). The scores of treated and untreated DBD participants were accordingly collapsed in all statistical analyses.
All participants in the NC group (100%), and most in the DBD group (> 85%) identified sadness as the most prominent emotion of the little bear. Three respondents in the DBD group observed fear instead of sadness. Because they did not experience fear themselves (no affect match), they got an empathy score of 0. Thus, all empathy ratings reflect empathic sadness in the current study.
Zero-order correlations between all empathy measures for the NC and DBD groups separately are shown in Table
3. Observed distress and empathy were significantly positively related in both groups,
rNC = .40,
p = .02;
rDBD = .37,
p = .04, indicating that those who observed more distress in the bear experienced more empathy. Observed distress was also significantly positively related to sympathy in the NC group,
r = .44,
p = .01, but not in the DBD group,
r = −.03,
ns. As can be expected, sympathy and empathy were significantly positively related in both groups,
rNC = .46,
p = .01;
rDBD = .60,
p = .001. Noteworthy are the negative correlations between sympathy and HR responses during the most pathetic (target),
rNC = −.30,
p = .09;
rDBD = −.13,
p > .1 and shocking (stones),
rNC = −.30,
p = .09;
rDBD = −.32,
p = .08, moments in both groups. Although the correlations were not significant, the overall pattern is consistent, and in agreement with previous findings that HR deceleration relates to sympathy (Zhou et al.
2003).
Table 3
Zero-order correlations between empathy-related responses, including HR responses during the food and stones scenes in the NC group (upper right corner) and DBD group (lower left corner)
1. | Observed distress | – | .40* | .44* | .00 | .09 | −.03 |
2. | Empathy | .37* | – | .46** | .00 | .02 | −.04 |
3. | Sympathy | −.03 | .60** | – | −.31 | −.30 | −.30 |
4. | HR_food | .35 | .17 | −.18 | – | .90** | .94** |
5. | HR_stones | .32 | .05 | −.32 | .71** | – | .86** |
6. | HR_target | .38* | .15 | −.13 | .92** | .68** | – |
Associations between Empathy-Related Responses and Psychopathic Traits
Zero-order correlations between predictors and criterium variables within the NC and DBD groups are presented in Table
5. The standardized regression coefficients (
β) for each predictor,
R square change (∆
R2) on each step of the MRA, and total
R square values are presented in Table
6. The skewness and kurtosis statistics of all scores were close to zero, indicating normality, except for observed distress (skewness = −2.6; kurtosis = 6.65). The assumptions of normality, linearity and homoscedasticity of residuals were checked. The overall models of all MRAs were significant, mainly due to the strong contribution of group to the APSD total index and dimensional scores (
p’s < .01).
Table 5
Zero-order correlations between the APSD scales, empathy-related responses and HR responses during the food and stones scenes
Self-reports |
Observed distress | −.05 | .10 | −.16 | −.01 | −.03 | −.10 | −.20 | .17 |
Empathy | −.22 | −.25 | −.02 | −.24 | .13 | .03 | .05 | .27 |
Sympathy | −.38* | −.16 | −.30 | −.31 | .27 | .21 | .06 | .46** |
HR reactivity |
Food | .25 | .35 | .04 | .16 | −.26 | −.21 | −.27 | −.11 |
Stones | .35* | .39* | .10 | .28 | −.48** | −.32 | −.44* | −.41* |
Target | .25 | .36* | .04 | .15 | −.26 | −.27 | −.25 | −.05 |
Table 6
Hierarchical moderator regression analyses predicting psychopathic tendencies from empathy-related responses (N = 63)
Predictor | ∆R2 | β | ∆R2 | β | ∆R2 | β | ∆R2 | β |
Step 1 | .65*** | | .65** | | .23*** | | .57** | |
Observed distress | | −.01 | | −.03 | | −.14 | | .07 |
Group | | .80*** | | .80** | | .43*** | | .76** |
Step 2 | .00 | | .00 | | .01 | | .00 | |
Observed distress | | −.05 | | .10 | | −.13 | | −.02 |
Group | | .80*** | | .81** | | .42** | | .76** |
Observed distress x Group | | .03 | | −.14 | | .19 | | .09 |
Total R2 | .65*** | | .81*** | | .48*** | | .75*** | |
Step 1 | .65*** | | .81*** | | .21** | | .85*** | |
Empathy | | −.01 | | −.05 | | .01 | | .02 |
Group | | .80*** | | .80*** | | .46*** | | .75*** |
Step 2 | .01 | | .01 | | .00 | | .03 | |
Empathy | | −.14 | | −.14 | | −.03 | | −.18 |
Group | | .80*** | | .80*** | | .46*** | | .75*** |
Empathy x Group | | .16 | | .12 | | .05 | | .26† |
Total R2 | .66*** | | .81*** | | .21** | | .77*** | |
Step 1 | .65*** | | .81*** | | .22** | | .75*** | |
Sympathy | | .02 | | .05 | | −.11 | | .09 |
Group | | .81*** | | .81*** | | .44*** | | .76*** |
Step 2 | .04* | | .01 | | .04 | | .06* | |
Sympathy | | −.26† | | −.10 | | −.40† | | −.26 |
Group | | .80*** | | .80*** | | .43*** | | .75*** |
Sympathy x Group | | .34* | | .18 | | .36† | | .43* |
Total R2 | .68*** | | .81*** | | .26*** | | .79*** | |
Step 1 | .65*** | | .65*** | | .21** | | .56*** | |
HR_target | | .00 | | .02 | | −.05 | | .04 |
Group | | .80*** | | .81*** | | .45*** | | .75*** |
Step 2 | .02† | | .03* | | .01 | | .00 | |
HR_target | | .12 | | .16 | | .04 | | .09 |
Group | | .80*** | | .80*** | | .41** | | .75*** |
HR_target x Group | | −.19† | | −.23* | | −.14 | | −.08 |
Total R2 | .82*** | | .68*** | | .23** | | .75*** | |
At the global construct level, MRAs showed that group significantly moderated the relationship between sympathy and the APSD total index score. As can be seen in Table
5, the relationships were opposite to each other in the NC and DBD groups. In agreement with expectations, sympathy was significantly negatively related to the APSD total index score in the NC group, indicating that those with higher APSD total index scores reported less sympathy for the bear. In contrast, the relationship was positive, though not significant, in the DBD group. Furthermore, as seen in Table
5, significant relationships were found between the APSD total index score and the HR response during the stones scene (preceding the target scene) in both groups, though in opposite directions: Normal controls with higher APSD scores showed stronger HR acceleration witnessing the stones roll down, while DBD individuals with higher APSD scores showed stronger HR deceleration.
At the dimensional level, the MRAs showed that group significantly moderated the relationship between sympathy and IMP, as well as the relationship between the HR response (target) and NAR. Again, all relationships were reversed in both the NC and DBD groups. In agreement with expectations, sympathy was insignificantly negative associated with IMP in the NC group, but significantly positive with IMP in the DBD group. Moreover, the HR response (target) was significantly positively related to NAR in the NC group, indicating that normal controls with higher levels of narcissism showed more HR acceleration during the target scene. The relationship was reversed, though not significant, in the DBD group, suggesting that DBD individuals with higher levels of narcissism tend to show more HR deceleration during the target scene. Different from expectations, none of the empathy indexes were significantly related to CU.
Discussion
The current study examined empathy towards animal distress in male adolescents with DBD and high or low levels of psychopathic traits and normal controls. Empathy-related responses were examined within the context of a film clip portraying a baby bear in distress. A first goal was to examine whether male adolescents with DBD, especially those with high levels of psychopathic traits, show subnormal levels of empathy towards animals. Contrary to expectations, no significant group differences emerged in any of the empathy related responses. Both DBD groups and normal controls observed equal levels of distress in the baby bear, experienced as much empathy and sympathy and showed similar reductions in HR during the most dramatic final scene. Classifications based on the broad construct of psychopathy as well as its dimensions yielded exactly the same results. In earlier work with the same sample (de Wied et al.
2012), DBD adolescents with high CU traits showed significantly less empathy towards human peers in distress. This could mean that empathy for humans does not generalize to animals, though we should be careful when drawing conclusions since the human and animal clips differed in many ways.
The lack of support for group differences in animal-directed empathy may have several reasons. First, stimulus characteristics may have evoked stronger empathy-related responses in all respondents, including those with psychopathic traits. We know from studies with undifferentiated groups of school-aged children with DBD (de Wied et al.
2005), and DBD children with and without comorbid anxiety disorder (Pijper et al.
2018) that stimulus characteristics play a role in DBD children’s reduced responsiveness to another’s distress. In both studies, all children reported more empathy and sympathy when watching a baby bear in distress than when watching human peers in distress. It is quite possible that the theme of the bear clip (loss of the mother), together with characteristics of the baby bear (cuteness and vulnerability) enhanced empathy in all groups such that group differences did not emerge in the current study.
Second, because the bear clip is a strong emotional stimulus, it may have attracted attention to the bear’s fate, which is a precondition for eliciting empathy. We know from previous studies that reduced attention to the emotions of others can be the cause of reduced empathy in individuals with psychopathic traits, and that explicitly directing attention to the emotions of the target may reduce group differences in empathy (van Baardewijk et al.
2009; Dadds et al.
2006b; Dadds et al.
2008; Meffert et al.
2013). In healthy persons, attending to affective film clips is generally associated with cardiac deceleration (e.g., Gomez et al.
2005; Kreibig et al.
2007), which may reflect stimulus intake or an orienting/attention response (Bradley et al.
2001; Cook and Turpin
1997). In the current study, cardiac deceleration was seen in all groups during the target scene, so it is possible that the bear clip generated equal levels of empathy across groups because salient cues attracted reflexive/involuntary attention. Increased attention to the sad situation of the baby bear may have triggered empathy and sympathy in all respondents, including those with psychopathic traits. This raises the question whether HR patterns may be considered to reflect focused attention, empathy related responses, or both.
Attention is an essential first step in the empathy process. However, the relationship between the empathizer and the person with whom one empathizes, personal characteristics of both parties and other contextual elements further determine the strength of the response (Main et al.
2017). The current findings demonstrate that under certain (perhaps most favorable) conditions, male adolescents with psychopathic traits show normal levels of empathy towards animals in distress. Nevertheless, they may well react differently to the suffering of other, perhaps less loveable animals in normal life. Empathy is a dynamic, mutual process that develops over time (Main et al.
2017). As yet, the context and relational dynamics of empathy have been largely neglected in research on empathy dysfunction in DBD individuals. Focusing on the dynamics of empathy is important because it can tell us more about the empathic capacity of individuals and the readiness to show empathy under different circumstances.
In the scope of previous results with the same sample, revealing subnormal levels of empathy for human peers among DBD adolescents with high CU traits (de Wied et al.
2012), the current results suggest that stimulus characteristics play a role in empathy problems seen in these individuals. This finding is compatible with the hypothesis that psychopaths have the
ability to empathize but not always the
propensity to empathize with others, as put forward by Keysers and Gazzola (
2014). Alternatively, the results could be taken to mean that empathy does not generalize from animals to humans and that inter-human and animal-directed empathy represent different psychological concepts (McPhedran
2009). More systematic research is needed to uncover the mechanisms involved, to determine whether empathy for humans extends to animals (and vice versa), and whether animal-directed empathy plays a role in animal cruelty. This knowledge is important for combating animal cruelty, and to improve training programs in which animals are used to enhance empathy towards humans (e.g., Grommon et al.
2020).
A second goal of the current study was to examine mutual relationships between empathy-related responses and different components of psychopathy. There are two important findings. First, within both the NC and DBD groups, none of the empathy indexes were significantly related to the CU dimension. Self-reported sympathy was significantly related to the APSD total index in the NC group and the impulsivity dimension in the DBD group. The HR response during the target was only significantly related to the narcissism dimension in the NC group. Because the empathy-related responses were differentially related to the dimensions of psychopathy, results suggest that the broader construct of psychopathy might add more to the diagnostic picture than the CU dimension alone. Second, the relationships were different and for the most part reversed in both groups. Because the samples were small, most correlations did not reach significance. Nevertheless, group was found to significantly moderate the relationships between self-reported sympathy and both the global construct of psychopathy and the impulsivity dimension. Also, group significantly moderated the relationship between the HR response and the narcissism dimension. The relationships between empathy-related responses and psychopathic traits were in agreement with expectations in the NC group: Higher levels of psychopathic traits were associated with lower levels of self-reported sympathy and stronger HR acceleration (or less HR deceleration) during the target scene. In the DBD group the relationships were reversed and counter-intuitive, with higher levels of psychopathy being associated with higher levels of self-reported sympathy and stronger HR deceleration. Except for the relationship between the APSD total index score and sympathy in the NC group, none of the relationships were significant, however, so we have to be careful interpreting the results.
Interestingly, significant relationships between the broader construct of psychopathy and HR responses were established in both the NC and DBD groups during the scene preceding the target, that is, the scene in which the stones roll down and hit the mother. Again, the relationships were reversed in both groups. In the NC group those with higher levels of psychopathic traits showed stronger HR acceleration (indicating personal distress) while in the DBD group they showed stronger HR deceleration (indicating focused attention, sympathy, or both). We did not ask the respondents what they felt during this particular scene, but the scene may have had a startling effect with various consequences for attention processes (Bradley et al.
2001).The opposing patterns are noteworthy and, if replicated, require more in-depth research into the underlying mechanisms.
The current study has several limitations and strengths. First, because the sample size in the current study was relatively small, results need replication with a larger number of respondents to draw firm conclusions. Second, the current study included only male adolescents and thus requires replication with female adolescents. Third, as in previous studies on human-directed empathy (e.g., de Wied et al.
2012) baseline HR was measured during the first 10-s period of the film clip. This may detract from the reliability of this measure since (1) such a period may be rather short to reliably measure resting HR, and (2) the current baseline period occurs at the early onset of the clip whereas HR may need a longer period to stabilize at the real resting baseline level (Hastrup
1986). The ANOVAs were therefore also performed with baseline HR measured during the first 100-s, but this yielded the same result. Fourth, a null result cannot provide evidence of absence. However, we believe the current null results are meaningful because (a) previous work with the same sample has demonstrated significant differences in human-directed empathy between DBD individuals and controls with partly similar indexes of empathy within a similar research setting (de Wied et al.
2012), and (b) the bear clip can be considered a valid instrument to arouse empathy in the laboratory as research has shown that this clip evokes stronger empathy-related responses in children with DBD than film clips portraying human peers in distress (de Wied et al.
2005; Pijper et al.
2018). Finally, it is important to note that the APSD often shows weak reliability, especially on the CU dimension (Ribeiro da Silva et al.
2020; Frick and Hare
2001). In the current study, however, the APSD showed sufficient agreement across teachers and parents and acceptable to good reliability on all dimensions.
Strengths of the study can be seen in the inclusion of a well-defined sample of adolescents with ODD/CD and psychopathic scores in the (sub-) clinical range. A further strength is the inclusion of multiple indexes of empathy that allowed for the mapping of empathy-related responses at different levels of information processing. Furthermore, our results confirm that attending to an empathy-inducing film clip portraying sadness is associated with HR deceleration (e.g., Kreibig et al.
2007), which strengthens the internal validity of the study.
In conclusion, the current study examined animal-directed empathy in DBD subgroups and normal controls, as well as the relationships between empathy-related responses and psychopathic traits. Different from expectations, no group differences emerged: Male adolescents with DBD and high levels of psychopathic traits showed as much empathy towards a baby bear in distress as those with lower levels of psychopathic traits and normal controls. Because previous research with the same sample did find group differences in human-directed empathy (de Wied et al.
2012), the current results suggest that empathy for humans does not generalize to animals. Furthermore, empathy-related responses were differently associated with psychopathic traits within and between the DBD and NC groups, indicating that results obtained within a sample of healthy adolescents do not simply generalize to adolescents with DBD, and vice versa.