In geriatrics, neuropsychological assessments are used to measure cognitive abilities and to detect changes in cognitive functioning (Tuokko & Hadjistavropoulos,
1998). There is a wide range of cognitive assessments testing neurobehavioral disorders in memory, language, emotions, attention, perception, executive functions, or visuospatial skills (Minagar, Finney, & Heimann,
2015). With advancing digitalization, the possibilities for digitalized cognitive assessments using a computer or tablet have expanded. In particular for the early detection of changes in the elderly and in patients with mild cognitive impairment, many digitalized assessments exist (Wild, Howieson, Webbe, Seelye, & Kaye,
2008; Woo,
2008). The advantages of digitalized assessments are described extensively in the literature. These advantages include an increase in reliability, objectivity, and standardization (Sternin, Burns, & Owen,
2019). An automated administration, scoring and interpretation of data, and the possibility of a convenient data storage are further advantages of digitalized assessments (Cernich, Brennana, Barker, & Bleiberg,
2007; Sternin et al.,
2019). Furthermore, tests are able to measure additional data such as response rates on the millisecond level, thereby providing more detailed insight into the patients capabilities. Less is said about the disadvantages of digitalized cognitive assessments. Some literature findings stated that clinicians should use tests on a computer or a tablet with caution and with consideration of potential technical complications (Bracken, Mazur-Mosiewicz, & Glazek,
2019; Cernich et al.,
2007). Cernich et al. focus primarily on the possible technical problems in hardware, software, peripherals, the display, connections, and bandwidth as well as program considerations (Cernich et al.,
2007). However, there are not only the technical challenges; especially for older adults, the computer with mouse, keyboard, and number pad or possibly a touchscreen can be intimidating or unfamiliar in the beginning. Therefore, initial training is necessary (Fortuny & Heaton,
1996). A study by Weber et al. showed that low acceptance of digitalized assessments and patients with a more negative attitude toward computers, correlates with poorer test results in attention tasks (Weber, Fritze, Schneider, Kuhner, & Maurer,
2002). Visual impairment or age-related vision loss as well as cognitive impairment or motor impairment may also cause relevant problems that are not related to the test results (Silverberg et al.,
2011). Bauer et al. also stated that the results can be falsified if the patient has to use their non-dominant hand to manipulate a mouse or a touchscreen, e.g., in hemiparetic patients. They conclude that it cannot be assumed that the results of a paper–pencil test are equal to computerized test results (Bauer et al.,
2012). Since computerized assessments showed only moderate correlations with paper–pencil tests (Silverberg et al.,
2011), new norm values for results classification have to be collected. The generation of new norm values is associated with high effort and extensive costs.
To benefit from the advantages of digitalization in cognitive testing (e.g., automatic soring, additional information) without having to accept the disadvantages (e.g., unfamiliarity), digital tools besides computer, tablet, mouse, and touchscreen can be a solution. A digital pen allows digitalizing all notes written with the pen on normal paper (more information in section
Methodology). The positive effects of digital pen technology were already investigated in learning (Boyle & Joyce,
2019) and as a method to detect conducted homework (Rawson, Stahovich, & Mayer,
2017). Within the study by Rawson et al., a digital pen was used to automatically and reliably record the homework activity to find a connection with academic achievement (Rawson et al.,
2017). Further concepts describe the possibilities of digital pen usage as an intuitive assistance tool for persons with dementia to improve communication, for example, when writing or answering emails (Prange, Sandrala, Weber, & Sonntag,
2015). Within the demo paper of Prange et al., a digital pen was used that streams its data via Bluetooth directly to a server, while the dementia patients write with a seemingly normal pen on paper with an invisible dot pattern. The authors of the demo paper also point out that the cognitive status of a participant can affect pen holding and (fine-) motor movements (Prange et al.,
2015).
H1: The execution time in TMT A and TMT B is not influenced by the type of pen (duration DPT = duration PT).
H2: The electronically measured execution time (ER) correlates significantly with the manually measured execution time (MR).
H3: The additional, electronically measured parameters (number of pen lifts, errors, omitted circles, all circles hit, correct order) correlate significantly with the execution time.