Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

07-07-2017 | Original Paper

Differences in Neural Correlates of Speech Perception in 3 Month Olds at High and Low Risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder

Auteurs: Laura A. Edwards, Jennifer B. Wagner, Helen Tager-Flusberg, Charles A. Nelson

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 10/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In this study, we investigated neural precursors of language acquisition as potential endophenotypes of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in 3-month-old infants at high and low familial ASD risk. Infants were imaged using functional near-infrared spectroscopy while they listened to auditory stimuli containing syllable repetitions; their neural responses were analyzed over left and right temporal regions. While female low risk infants showed initial neural activation that decreased over exposure to repetition-based stimuli, potentially indicating a habituation response to repetition in speech, female high risk infants showed no changes in neural activity over exposure. This finding may indicate a potential neural endophenotype of language development or ASD specific to females at risk for the disorder.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
The association between sex and ASD differs with cognitive ability. In children who are cognitively high functioning, the sex ratio may be more than 5.5:1 (M:F), while in children with intellectual disability (ID), the ASD sex ratio is closer to 2:1 (Newschaffer et al. 2007).
 
2
Hair, particularly that which is highly pigmented, interferes with NIRS measurements by absorbing near-infrared (NIR) light from source optodes before it penetrates the skull and arrives at the brain tissue. NIRS headgear is thus designed to keep hair out of the path of NIR light as much as possible. Many adaptations for reducing interference by hair may cause discomfort however, making them inappropriate for studies on infant populations.
 
3
These regions of interest (ROIs) cover roughly identical topographies as those used by Gervain et al. (2008). We include fewer channels in each ROI average herein, in order to distinguish regional brain activity more clearly than was perhaps possible in Gervain’s study of newborns (who have smaller average head sizes than 3 month olds) and to maintain consistency with other studies conducted in our lab, using the same NIRS headgear (i.e., Keehn et al. 2013; Wagner et al. 2011).
 
4
Mauchly’s test of sphericity indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated for the main effect of region, χ2(5) = 27.40, p < 0.001. Therefore, degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.64 for the main effect of region).
 
5
Mauchly’s test of sphericity indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated for the main effect of region, χ2(5) = 18.33, p = 0.003. Therefore, degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.705 for the main effect of region).
 
6
The exposure effects herein are likely not an artifact of the number of trials that participants heard, as the majority (n = 33; 87%) of participants listened to all 28 trials, 2 participants (5.3%) stopped after 25 trials, 1 (2.6%) stopped after 24 trials, 1 (2.6%) stopped after 23 trials, and 1 (2.6%) stopped after 19 trials. Additionally, the number of trials heard did not differ significantly between risk groups (p = 0.18), genders (p = 0.65), or syllabic sequence type (p = 1.00). Finally, the four-way interaction between exposure, syllabic sequence type, risk group and gender was robust to the inclusion of number of trials heard as a covariate in the mixed-design (2 × 4 × 2 × 2x2) ANOVA: exposure × syllabic sequence type × risk group × gender (F(1,32) = 5.46, p = 0.026, partial η2 = 0.146); all interactions with number of trials (p > 0.05).
 
7
At the request of a reviewer, we conducted a 2 (syllabic sequence type) × 4 (region) × 2 (risk group) × 2 (exposure) mixed-design ANOVA, to examine exposure results collapsing across gender. This analysis revealed that the main effect of region was significant when gender was not included as a between-subjects factor (F(2.09, 75.15, corrected using Greenhouse–Geisser estimate of sphericity (ε= 0.696)) = 3.45, p = 0.046, partial η2 = 0.081). Additionally, elimination of HRA females from the sample extinguished the main effect of region over exposure (F(3,24) = 0.591, p = 0.627).
 
8
Preliminary analyses of this nature—based on a subset of the present sample—suggest that the magnitude of the fNIRS neural response to the last four ABB stimuli predicts MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory-III (Fenson et al. 2007) gesture production (p < 0.001, R2 = 0.7005) and sentence complexity (p < 0.001, R2 = 0.4999), and Mullen Scales of Early Learning (Mullen 1995) expressive language scores (p < 0.001, R2 = 0.5052) for both LRC and HRA subjects at 18 months, across gender.
 
9
It is also possible that the gender effects found herein are confounding results that are instead attributable to participants’ eventual ASD outcomes (since more males than females will go on to develop ASD). Unfortunately, we did not have the statistical power to test this hypothesis directly, as only 19 of the 38 infants (11 LRC, 8 HRA) analyzed herein returned for clinical evaluation and diagnosis confirmation (or lack thereof) at 36 months of age.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association.CrossRef American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association.CrossRef
go back to reference Baio, J., 010 Principal, et al. (2014). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years-autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. Surveillance Summaries (Washington, DC: 2002), 63(2), 1. Baio, J., 010 Principal, et al. (2014). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years-autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. Surveillance Summaries (Washington, DC: 2002), 63(2), 1.
go back to reference Bauer, D. J., Goldfield, B. A., & Reznick, J. S. (2002). Alternative approaches to analyzing individual differences in the rate of early vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics, doi:10.1017/S0142716402003016. Bauer, D. J., Goldfield, B. A., & Reznick, J. S. (2002). Alternative approaches to analyzing individual differences in the rate of early vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics, doi:10.​1017/​S014271640200301​6.
go back to reference Boddaert, N., Belin, P., Chabane, N., Poline, J.-B., Barthélémy, C., Mouren-Simeoni, M.-C., … Zilbovicius, M. (2003). Perception of complex sounds: abnormal pattern of cortical activation in autism. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(11), 2057–2060.CrossRefPubMed Boddaert, N., Belin, P., Chabane, N., Poline, J.-B., Barthélémy, C., Mouren-Simeoni, M.-C., … Zilbovicius, M. (2003). Perception of complex sounds: abnormal pattern of cortical activation in autism. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(11), 2057–2060.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., & Haynes, O. M. (2004). Specific and general language performance across early childhood: Stability and gender considerations. First Language, 24(3), 267–304. doi:10.1177/0142723704045681.CrossRef Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., & Haynes, O. M. (2004). Specific and general language performance across early childhood: Stability and gender considerations. First Language, 24(3), 267–304. doi:10.​1177/​0142723704045681​.CrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Academic Press. Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Academic Press.
go back to reference De Giacomo, A., & Fombonne, E. (1998). Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 7(3), 131–136.CrossRef De Giacomo, A., & Fombonne, E. (1998). Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 7(3), 131–136.CrossRef
go back to reference Devor, A., Ulbert, I., Dunn, A. K., Narayanan, S. N., Jones, S. R., Andermann, M. L., … Dale, A. M. (2005). Coupling of the cortical hemodynamic response to cortical and thalamic neuronal activity. In Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(10), 3822–3827.CrossRef Devor, A., Ulbert, I., Dunn, A. K., Narayanan, S. N., Jones, S. R., Andermann, M. L., … Dale, A. M. (2005). Coupling of the cortical hemodynamic response to cortical and thalamic neuronal activity. In Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(10), 3822–3827.CrossRef
go back to reference Emberson, L. L., Cannon, G., Palmeri, H., Richards, J. E., & Aslin, R. N. (2017). Using fNIRS to examine occipital and temporal responses to stimulus repetition in young infants: Evidence of selective frontal cortex involvement. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 26–38. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2016.11.002.CrossRefPubMed Emberson, L. L., Cannon, G., Palmeri, H., Richards, J. E., & Aslin, R. N. (2017). Using fNIRS to examine occipital and temporal responses to stimulus repetition in young infants: Evidence of selective frontal cortex involvement. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 26–38. doi:10.​1016/​j.​dcn.​2016.​11.​002.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pérez Pereira, M., Wehberg, S., … Gallego, C. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(Pt 2), 326–343. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02042.x.CrossRefPubMed Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pérez Pereira, M., Wehberg, S., … Gallego, C. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(Pt 2), 326–343. doi:10.​1111/​j.​2044-835X.​2011.​02042.​x.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Eyler, L. T., Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. (2012). A failure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging and fundamental property of autism. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 135(3), 949–960. doi:10.1093/brain/awr364.CrossRef Eyler, L. T., Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. (2012). A failure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging and fundamental property of autism. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 135(3), 949–960. doi:10.​1093/​brain/​awr364.CrossRef
go back to reference Fantz, R. L. (1964). Visual experience in infants: Decreased attention to familiar patterns relative to novel ones. Science, 146(3644), 668–670.CrossRefPubMed Fantz, R. L. (1964). Visual experience in infants: Decreased attention to familiar patterns relative to novel ones. Science, 146(3644), 668–670.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
go back to reference Gervain, J., Macagno, F., Cogoi, S., Peña, M., & Mehler, J. (2008). The neonate brain detects speech structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(37), 14222–14227.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gervain, J., Macagno, F., Cogoi, S., Peña, M., & Mehler, J. (2008). The neonate brain detects speech structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(37), 14222–14227.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Gleason, J. B., & Ely, R. (2002). Gender differences in language development. Biology, Society, and Behavior: The Development of Sex Differences in Cognition, 21, 127–154. Gleason, J. B., & Ely, R. (2002). Gender differences in language development. Biology, Society, and Behavior: The Development of Sex Differences in Cognition, 21, 127–154.
go back to reference Gottesman, I. I., & Gould, T. D. (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(4), 636–645.CrossRefPubMed Gottesman, I. I., & Gould, T. D. (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(4), 636–645.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Guiraud, J. A., Kushnerenko, E., Tomalski, P., Davies, K., Ribeiro, H., Johnson, M. H., & BASIS Team. (2011). Differential habituation to repeated sounds in infants at high risk for autism. Neuroreport, 22(16), 845–849. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834c0bec.PubMed Guiraud, J. A., Kushnerenko, E., Tomalski, P., Davies, K., Ribeiro, H., Johnson, M. H., & BASIS Team. (2011). Differential habituation to repeated sounds in infants at high risk for autism. Neuroreport, 22(16), 845–849. doi:10.​1097/​WNR.​0b013e32834c0bec​.PubMed
go back to reference Halladay, A. K., Bishop, S., Constantino, J. N., Daniels, A. M., Koenig, K., Palmer, K., … Szatmari, P. (2015). Sex and gender differences in autism spectrum disorder: summarizing evidence gaps and identifying emerging areas of priority. Molecular Autism. doi:10.1186/s13229-015-0019-y.PubMedPubMedCentral Halladay, A. K., Bishop, S., Constantino, J. N., Daniels, A. M., Koenig, K., Palmer, K., … Szatmari, P. (2015). Sex and gender differences in autism spectrum disorder: summarizing evidence gaps and identifying emerging areas of priority. Molecular Autism. doi:10.​1186/​s13229-015-0019-y.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Huppert, T. J., Diamond, S. G., Franceschini, M. A., & Boas, D. A. (2009). HomER: A review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Applied Optics, 48(10), D280–D298.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Huppert, T. J., Diamond, S. G., Franceschini, M. A., & Boas, D. A. (2009). HomER: A review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Applied Optics, 48(10), D280–D298.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Kameyama, M., Fukuda, M., Uehara, T., & Mikuni, M. (2004). Sex and age dependencies of cerebral blood volume changes during cognitive activation: a multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy study. NeuroImage, 22(4), 1715–1721.CrossRefPubMed Kameyama, M., Fukuda, M., Uehara, T., & Mikuni, M. (2004). Sex and age dependencies of cerebral blood volume changes during cognitive activation: a multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy study. NeuroImage, 22(4), 1715–1721.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Keehn, B., Vogel-Farley, V., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2015). Atypical hemispheric specialization for faces in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 8(2), 187–198.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Keehn, B., Vogel-Farley, V., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2015). Atypical hemispheric specialization for faces in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 8(2), 187–198.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Key, A. P. F., & Stone, W. L. (2012). Same but different: 9-month-old infants at average and high risk for autism look at the same facial features but process them using different brain mechanisms. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 5(4), 253–266. doi:10.1002/aur.1231.CrossRef Key, A. P. F., & Stone, W. L. (2012). Same but different: 9-month-old infants at average and high risk for autism look at the same facial features but process them using different brain mechanisms. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 5(4), 253–266. doi:10.​1002/​aur.​1231.CrossRef
go back to reference Knaus, T. A., Silver, A. M., Kennedy, M., Lindgren, K. A., Dominick, K. C., Siegel, J., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2010). Language laterality in autism spectrum disorder and typical controls: A functional, volumetric, and diffusion tensor MRI study. Brain and Language, 112(2), 113–120. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2009.11.005.CrossRefPubMed Knaus, T. A., Silver, A. M., Kennedy, M., Lindgren, K. A., Dominick, K. C., Siegel, J., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2010). Language laterality in autism spectrum disorder and typical controls: A functional, volumetric, and diffusion tensor MRI study. Brain and Language, 112(2), 113–120. doi:10.​1016/​j.​bandl.​2009.​11.​005.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lai, M.-C., Lombardo, M. V., Pasco, G., Ruigrok, A. N. V., Wheelwright, S. J., Sadek, S. A., … Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). A behavioral comparison of male and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020835. Lai, M.-C., Lombardo, M. V., Pasco, G., Ruigrok, A. N. V., Wheelwright, S. J., Sadek, S. A., … Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). A behavioral comparison of male and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE. doi:10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0020835.
go back to reference Little, V. M., Thomas, D. G., & Letterman, M. R. (1999). Single-trial analyses of developmental trends in infant auditory event-related potentials. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16(3), 455–478.CrossRef Little, V. M., Thomas, D. G., & Letterman, M. R. (1999). Single-trial analyses of developmental trends in infant auditory event-related potentials. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16(3), 455–478.CrossRef
go back to reference Lloyd-Fox, S., Blasi, A., Elwell, C. E., Charman, T., Murphy, D., & Johnson, M. H. (2013). Reduced neural sensitivity to social stimuli in infants at risk for autism. Proceedings of Royal Society B, 280(1758), 20123026. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.3026. Lloyd-Fox, S., Blasi, A., Elwell, C. E., Charman, T., Murphy, D., & Johnson, M. H. (2013). Reduced neural sensitivity to social stimuli in infants at risk for autism. Proceedings of Royal Society B, 280(1758), 20123026. doi:10.​1098/​rspb.​2012.​3026.
go back to reference Luyster, R. J., Powell, C., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2014). Neural measures of social attention across the first years of life: characterizing typical development and markers of autism risk. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 131–143. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2013.09.006.CrossRefPubMed Luyster, R. J., Powell, C., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2014). Neural measures of social attention across the first years of life: characterizing typical development and markers of autism risk. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 131–143. doi:10.​1016/​j.​dcn.​2013.​09.​006.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Messinger, D., Young, G. S., Ozonoff, S., Dobkins, K., Carter, A., Zwaigenbaum, L., … Sigman, M. (2013). Beyond autism: A baby siblings research consortium study of high-risk children at three years of age. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(3), 300–308.e1. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.12.011.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Messinger, D., Young, G. S., Ozonoff, S., Dobkins, K., Carter, A., Zwaigenbaum, L., … Sigman, M. (2013). Beyond autism: A baby siblings research consortium study of high-risk children at three years of age. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(3), 300–308.e1. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jaac.​2012.​12.​011.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning (AGS ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service Inc. Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning (AGS ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service Inc.
go back to reference Newschaffer, C. J., Croen, L. A., Daniels, J., Giarelli, E., Grether, J. K., Levy, S. E., et al. (2007). The epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders*. Annual Review of Public Health, 28, 235–258.CrossRefPubMed Newschaffer, C. J., Croen, L. A., Daniels, J., Giarelli, E., Grether, J. K., Levy, S. E., et al. (2007). The epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders*. Annual Review of Public Health, 28, 235–258.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Okamoto, M., Dan, H., Sakamoto, K., Takeo, K., Shimizu, K., Kohno, S., … Dan, I. (2004). Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10–20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping. NeuroImage, 21(1), 99–111.CrossRefPubMed Okamoto, M., Dan, H., Sakamoto, K., Takeo, K., Shimizu, K., Kohno, S., … Dan, I. (2004). Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10–20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping. NeuroImage, 21(1), 99–111.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Carter, A., Messinger, D., Yirmiya, N., Zwaigenbaum, L., … Stone, W. L. (2011). Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics, 128(3), e488–e495. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-2825.PubMedPubMedCentral Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Carter, A., Messinger, D., Yirmiya, N., Zwaigenbaum, L., … Stone, W. L. (2011). Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics, 128(3), e488–e495. doi:10.​1542/​peds.​2010-2825.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Parke, R., & Gauvain, M. (2008). Child psychology: a contemporary view point (7 ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill Education. Parke, R., & Gauvain, M. (2008). Child psychology: a contemporary view point (7 ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill Education.
go back to reference Peña, M., Maki, A., Kovacić, D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Koizumi, H., Bouquet, F., & Mehler, J. (2003). Sounds and silence: An optical topography study of language recognition at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(20), 11702–11705. doi:10.1073/pnas.1934290100.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Peña, M., Maki, A., Kovacić, D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Koizumi, H., Bouquet, F., & Mehler, J. (2003). Sounds and silence: An optical topography study of language recognition at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(20), 11702–11705. doi:10.​1073/​pnas.​1934290100.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Seery, A. M., Vogel-Farley, V., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2013). Atypical lateralization of ERP response to native and non-native speech in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 10–24. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.007.CrossRefPubMed Seery, A. M., Vogel-Farley, V., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2013). Atypical lateralization of ERP response to native and non-native speech in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 10–24. doi:10.​1016/​j.​dcn.​2012.​11.​007.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sinha, P., Kjelgaard, M. M., Gandhi, T. K., Tsourides, K., Cardinaux, A. L., Pantazis, D., … Held, R. M. (2014). Autism as a disorder of prediction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 111(42), 15220–15225. doi:10.1073/pnas.1416797111.CrossRef Sinha, P., Kjelgaard, M. M., Gandhi, T. K., Tsourides, K., Cardinaux, A. L., Pantazis, D., … Held, R. M. (2014). Autism as a disorder of prediction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 111(42), 15220–15225. doi:10.​1073/​pnas.​1416797111.CrossRef
go back to reference Thomas, D. G., & Lykins, M. S. (1995). Event-related potential measures of 24-hour retention in 5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 31(6), 946.CrossRef Thomas, D. G., & Lykins, M. S. (1995). Event-related potential measures of 24-hour retention in 5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 31(6), 946.CrossRef
go back to reference Watanabe, A., & Kato, T. (2004). Cerebrovascular response to cognitive tasks in patients with schizophrenia measured by near-infrared spectroscopy. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30(2), 435–444.CrossRefPubMed Watanabe, A., & Kato, T. (2004). Cerebrovascular response to cognitive tasks in patients with schizophrenia measured by near-infrared spectroscopy. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30(2), 435–444.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wetherby, A., Woods, J., Allen, L., Cleary, J., Dickinson, H., & Lord, C. (2004). Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 473–493.CrossRef Wetherby, A., Woods, J., Allen, L., Cleary, J., Dickinson, H., & Lord, C. (2004). Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 473–493.CrossRef
go back to reference Wolff, J. J., Gerig, G., Lewis, J. D., Soda, T., Styner, M. A., Vachet, C., … IBIS Network. (2015). Altered corpus callosum morphology associated with autism over the first 2 years of life. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 138(Pt 7), 2046–2058. doi:10.1093/brain/awv118.CrossRef Wolff, J. J., Gerig, G., Lewis, J. D., Soda, T., Styner, M. A., Vachet, C., … IBIS Network. (2015). Altered corpus callosum morphology associated with autism over the first 2 years of life. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 138(Pt 7), 2046–2058. doi:10.​1093/​brain/​awv118.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Differences in Neural Correlates of Speech Perception in 3 Month Olds at High and Low Risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder
Auteurs
Laura A. Edwards
Jennifer B. Wagner
Helen Tager-Flusberg
Charles A. Nelson
Publicatiedatum
07-07-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 10/2017
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3222-1