Depression is one of the most prevalent mental disorders worldwide (World Health Organization
2012). Adolescence is a critical period in the development of mood disorders, as the prevalence of depression increases sharply in the course of adolescence (Hankin et al.
1998). Most depression research has focused on negative affect and negative experiences. This seems plausible, as according to the DSM-5 one of the two core symptoms of depression is depressed mood, and several other symptoms reflect negative emotions or cognitions (i.e., feelings of worthlessness, suicidality; American Psychiatric Association
2013). However, the other core symptom, anhedonia (i.e., loss of pleasure), is also important to investigate, because it specifically predicts worse treatment outcome (Spijker et al.
2001) and a longer and more severe course of depression (McMakin et al.
2012). Further, research showed that anhedonia experienced in adolescence is a predictor for Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) during adulthood (Wilcox and Anthony
2004).
Unfortunately, little is known about the relation between depressive symptoms and the actual experience of pleasure in daily life. Hence, it remains unclear which aspects of daily life pleasure are related to depressive symptoms. Research in adults has shown that depressive symptoms are associated with a lower frequency of positive events and lower pleasure levels during these events (i.e., lower intensity of events; Bylsma et al.
2011; Peeters et al.
2003), as well as with lower mean levels of positive affect (PA; Peeters et al.
2003; Thompson et al.
2012). In adolescents, depression has also been associated with lower mean levels of PA (Forbes et al.
2004), and with a lower positive affect/negative affect ratio (Silk et al.
2011). However, affect is not a stable entity, but fluctuates across time. Results on variability in PA in depressed adults are contradictory: depression has been related to higher general variability in PA (Gruber et al.
2013; Höhn et al.
2013; Kuppens et al.
2007), a lack of moment-to-moment variability (i.e., higher inertia) in PA (Kuppens et al.
2010), or was not associated with variability or inertia (Koval et al.
2013; Pe et al.
2014; Thompson et al.
2012). Studies in adolescents have also shown contradictory findings. Whereas one study found that emotional inertia in NA and PA during an interaction task with parents predicted the onset of MDD in adolescents 2 years later (Kuppens et al.
2012), another found that depressive symptoms in early adolescence predicted higher variability in PA, which in turn predicted more depressive symptoms 1 year later (Neumann et al.
2011). Hence, both increased and decreased variability in PA have been found to relate to depressive symptoms in adolescents. These differences in findings may be partly due to how variability was operationalized; some studies examined the standard deviation across measurements (i.e., general variability), some the auto-correlation between PA at
t and PA at
t-1 (i.e., inertia), and some the mean squared successive difference (i.e., instability; Jahng et al.
2008; Koval et al.
2013; Thompson et al.
2012). Hence, there is a need for studies that include multiple measures of variability.
Next to the occurrence of positive events and PA as such, it is also important to investigate their interplay. Positive events are known as important factors that impact PA, but research examining whether depressive symptoms moderate the association between positive events and PA (i.e., reactivity to positive events) is lacking in adolescents. In adults, one study found evidence for a mood brightening effect, in that depressive symptoms were associated with greater increases in PA after positive events (Peeters et al.
2003). Other studies did not find differences in reactivity between depressed and non-depressed adults (Bylsma et al.
2011; Thompson et al.
2012).
The interplay between positive events and PA may also work the other way around, in that PA affects the extent to which positive events are experienced. According to positive psychology theories such as the Broaden-and-Build theory (Fredrickson
2001), positive emotions increase openness to experiences and approach behavior. This suggests that experiencing PA may elicit positive events, but no research has yet investigated this relationship in daily life, let alone the impact of depressive symptoms. As the exposure to more positive events when high in PA could further increase pleasure levels in daily life, it is important to examine whether this process is deficient in individuals suffering from depressive symptoms.
Finally, although it seems likely that the core symptom anhedonia is the driving force in the relation between depressive symptoms and the experience of pleasure in daily life, to our knowledge no studies have examined this hypothesis yet. Therefore, another aim of the present study was to examine whether anhedonia would be stronger related to the experience of pleasure in daily life than the (more heterogeneous) composite measure of depressive symptoms.
In sum, the aim of the present study was to examine relations between depressive symptoms (measured on a macro-level) and the experience of pleasure in adolescents’ daily life (measured on a micro-level). Knowledge about which of these aspects of pleasure in daily life are associated with depressive symptoms may provide important starting points for interventions. We investigated four main research questions, each representing a different aspect of daily life experiences of pleasure. First, we examined whether depressive symptoms were related to the proportion and intensity of positive events experienced in daily life (i.e., positive event characteristics). Second, the relations between depressive symptoms and mean PA and the three different types of PA variability (i.e., general variability, instability, and inertia in PA) were investigated (i.e., PA characteristics). Third, we examined whether depressive symptoms were associated with PA reactivity to positive events, and fourth, whether depressive symptoms were related to the extent to which PA elicited positive events. We hypothesized that depressive symptoms would be associated with a lower proportion and lower intensity of positive events (research question 1), and lower mean levels of PA (research question 2). With regard to the variability in PA (research question 2), we did not have a specific hypothesis, as previous research both found increased and decreased variability to be associated with depressive symptoms. Further, we expected that depressive symptoms would be associated with decreased PA reactivity to positive events (research question 3) and decreased positive event generation by PA (research question 4).
As the reward system is in full development during adolescence (Davey et al.
2008), it is possible that the association between depressive symptoms and the experience of pleasure alters during this period. Therefore, we compared an early adolescent female sample (from now on referred to as EA girls) and a late adolescent female sample (from now on referred to as LA girls) to examine age differences. Further, levels of PA and depressive symptoms differ between boys and girls (Hankin and Abramson
2001; Larson et al.
2002), which is why we compared the early adolescent female sample to an early adolescent male sample (from now on referred to as EA boys) to examine sex differences. In all three subsamples, the Experience Sampling Method (ESM) was used to measure daily life positive events and PA. The main advantages of this method are the high ecological validity and low recall bias, as participants report on their emotions and events when they experience them, in their natural environment (Myin-Germeys et al.
2009). Finally, as it seems likely that specifically the core symptom anhedonia would be associated with daily life experiences of pleasure, we examined whether the relations between depressive symptoms and the experience of pleasure in daily life are particularly driven by this symptom.
Methods
Participants
We used data from two different samples in the present study. Because these samples were collected for different research purposes, somewhat different inclusion and exclusion criteria were used, and some of the study procedures differed. Both samples are described in detail below.
Strategy of Analyses
Descriptive statistics were calculated, and differences between subsamples in mean levels of depressive feelings, anhedonia, PA, moment-to-moment variability (i.e., RMSSD), general variability (mean SD), and the proportion of positive events were tested by conducting ANOVAs.
Subsequently, associations between depressive symptoms and positive event characteristics were examined. Associations between depressive symptoms and the proportion of positive events were explored by using regression analyses in SPSS, whereas the association between depressive symptoms and the intensity of positive events was examined by using multilevel analyses in Mplus, as the multiple assessments are nested within individuals (i.e., Level 1 = assessment level, Level 2 = individual level). Multilevel analyses enable the examination of relations between Level 1 (e.g., positive events; dependent variable) and Level 2 variables (e.g., depressive symptoms; independent variable).
Next, the associations between depressive symptoms and PA characteristics were investigated. The relation between depressive symptoms and the mean level of PA was examined in a multilevel analysis with PA as dependent variable and depressive symptoms as independent variable. Inertia (i.e., instability in PA) was examined in a multilevel model in which PA
T-1
and PA
T
(i.e., the autocorrelation of PA) were included as independent and dependent variable, respectively. We examined whether depressive symptoms affected inertia by including the cross-level interaction between depressive symptoms and PA
T-1
. The relation between depressive symptoms and general variability of positive affect and general instability was examined by two regression analyses with the SD and the RMSSD as dependent variables, respectively, and depressive symptoms as independent variables.
The relation between positive events and PA (i.e., PA reactivity to positive events) was examined in a multilevel model in which positive events was included as independent variable and PA
T
as dependent variable, while controlling for PA
T-1
. Moderation of depressive symptoms was examined by adding the cross-level interaction with positive events.
We investigated the relation between PA
T-1
and positive events at T (i.e., positive event generation by PA), and whether depressive symptoms moderated this relation, again in a multilevel model, with PA
T-1
and the cross-level interaction between PA
T-1
and depressive symptoms as independent variables and positive events at T as dependent variable, while controlling for positive events at T-1. For all analyses described above, we replaced depressive symptoms with anhedonia to examine whether the found associations were driven by anhedonia.
For all models, we examined whether the relations differed between EA girls versus EA boys (i.e., sex differences), and between EA girls versus LA girls (i.e., age differences). This was done by including dummy variables for subsample and subsample by depressive symptoms interactions in the regression analyses, and by conducting multi-group analyses in the multilevel models. In the multi-group analyses we examined whether the model fit (∆χ
2) of the multilevel model in which the paths of interest are allowed to differ between subsamples was significantly better than the model fit of the model in which all paths were constrained to be equal across subsamples.
Predictors at the individual level (i.e., depressive symptoms and anhedonia) were standardized within subsamples and predictors at the assessment level (i.e., PA, positive events) were centered within individuals (i.e., person-specific mean centering). Significance levels (two-tailed) were set at
p < 0.05 for all models. Missing data were not imputed; the multilevel models used all data that were available for each adolescent. That is, for some adolescents 20 assessments were included in the analyses, whereas for others 54 assessments were included. For multilevel analyses, the recommended sample size is a minimum of 20 observations (momentary assessments) for at least 50 groups (individuals) (Maas and Hox
2005). Our smallest subgroup consisted of 74 participants, showing that we had enough power for our analyses.
Results
Descriptive Statistics
Table
1 shows the mean levels of the model variables in the different samples. EA girls reported significantly more depressive symptoms and anhedonia than LA girls. Scores on depressive symptoms (means =1.36–1.60; 75th percentile EA girls =1.83, EA boys =1.67, LA girls =1.50) and anhedonia (means =1.12–1.92; 75th percentile EA girls =2.00, EA boys = 2.00, LA girls =1.00) were relatively low, given the range (range = 1–5), whereas mean levels of PA (means =4.57–5.05) were relatively high (range = 1–7). Mean levels of PA were higher in EA girls, compared to LA girls, whereas the proportion of positive events was higher in LA girls compared to EA girls. General variability and general instability were higher in EA girls than in LA girls. EA boys had similar levels of mean PA, general instability, and a similar proportion of positive events as EA girls. General variability and depressive symptoms were higher in EA girls than in EA boys. As can be seen in Table
2, the variability measures were correlated in all samples. Further, higher mean levels of PA were related to lower variability in PA.
Table 1
Descriptive statistics for each sample
Age | 14.25a (0.54) | 13–16 | 116 | 14.15a (0.54) | 13–16 | 168 | 20.91b (1.81) | 18–25 | 74 | 1416.87*** | 357 |
Depressive symptoms | 1.43a (0.44) | 1.00–3.33 | 116 | 1.60b (0.62) | 1.00–4.67 | 163 | 1.36a (0.46) | 1.00–3.50 | 74 | 6.29** | 352 |
Anhedonia | 1.92a (0.79) | 1.00–5.00 | 116 | 1.74a (0.87) | 1.00–5.00 | 163 | 1.12b (0.50) | 1.00–4.00 | 74 | 25.01*** | 352 |
Mean PA | 5.05a (0.76) | 2.87–6.70 | 116 | 4.97a (0.72) | 2.98–6.41 | 168 | 4.57b (0.67) | 2.59–5.96 | 74 | 10.72*** | 357 |
RMSSD | 0.99ab (0.39) | 0.25–2.21 | 116 | 1.08a (0.36) | 0.41–2.15 | 168 | 0.90b (0.25) | 0.42–1.67 | 74 | 7.57** | 357 |
Mean SD PA | 0.82a (0.29) | 0.24–1.71 | 116 | 0.91b (0.28) | 0.29–1.95 | 168 | 0.77a (0.21) | 0.31–1.47 | 74 | 7.05*** | 357 |
Proportion PE | 0.56a (0.19) | 0.05–0.97 | 116 | 0.56a (0.17) | 0.07–0.95 | 168 | 0.62b (0.20) | 0.00–0.96 | 74 | 3.31* | 357 |
Intensity PE | 1.33a (0.52) | 0.10–2.45 | 116 | 1.33a (0.46) | 0.21–2.53 | 168 | 1.01b (0.38) | 0.00–2.09 | 74 | 13.57*** | 357 |
Table 2
Correlations between model variables, split per sample
1. Depressive symptoms | – | | | | | | – | | | | | | – | | | | | |
2. Anhedonia | 0.43*** | – | | | | | 0.57*** | – | | | | | 0.73*** | – | | | | |
3. Mean PA | −0.36*** | −0.12 | – | | | | −0.22** | 0.01 | – | | | | −0.39** | −0.30* | – | | | |
4. RMSSD | 0.20* | 0.12 | −0.44*** | – | | | 0.10 | 0.03 | −0.37*** | – | | | 0.10 | −0.05 | −0.44*** | – | | |
5. Mean SD | 0.24** | 0.15 | −0.42*** | 0.86*** | – | | 0.11 | 0.08 | −0.41*** | 0.85*** | – | | 0.14 | −0.05 | −0.47*** | 0.89*** | – | |
6. Proportion positive events | −0.23* | −0.21* | 0.22* | −0.14 | −0.11 | – | −0.13 | 0.05 | 0.28** | −0.18* | −0.24** | | −0.31** | −0.31** | 0.49*** | −0.11 | −0.17 | – |
7. Intensity positive events | −0.28** | −0.16 | 0.29** | 0.03 | 0.06 | 0.88*** | −0.09 | 0.10 | 0.36*** | −0.06 | −0.12 | 0.88*** | −0.26* | −0.28* | 0.51*** | −0.07 | −0.09 | 0.86*** |
Depressive Symptoms and Positive Event Characteristics
Results showed that depressive symptoms were related to a lower proportion of positive events (B = −0.04, SE = 0.01, p < 0.001). Depressive symptoms were also related to less pleasure during positive events (B = −0.09, SE = 0.03, p < 0.01). Anhedonia was negatively related to the proportion of positive events (B = −0.02, SE = 0.01, p < 0.05), but not related to the level of pleasure during events (B = −0.03, SE = 0.03, p = 0.30).
Depressive Symptoms and PA Characteristics
Depressive symptoms were related to lower levels of PA (B = −0.22, SE = 0.04, p < 0.001) Depressive symptoms were related to higher levels of general variability (i.e., SD; B = 0.04, SE = 0.01, p < 0.01) and higher levels of instability (i.e., RMSSD; B = 0.05, SE = 0.03, p < 0.01). The auto-correlation between PA
T-1
and PA
T
was significant (B = 0.27, SE = 0.02, p < 0.001). Depressive symptoms did not affect the autocorrelation between PA
T-1
and PA
T
(B = −0.00, SE = 0.01, p = 0.80), indicating no association between depressive symptoms and inertia in PA.
Anhedonia was not related to PA (B = −0.07, SE = 0.05, p = 0.13), nor general variability in PA (B
SD
= 0.02, SE = 0.02, p = 0.16) nor instability (B
RMSSD
= 0.02, SE = 0.02, p = 0.38), and did not affect the autocorrelation between PA
T-1
and PA
T
(B = 0.00, SE = 0.01, p = 0.81).
Depressive Symptoms and PA Reactivity to Positive Events
Positive events were related to PA (B = 0.15, SE = 0.01, p < 0.001). The cross-level interaction between depressive symptoms and positive events on PA was borderline significant (B = 0.02, SE = 0.01, p = 0.05). This finding tentatively indicates that adolescents with many depressive symptoms increased more in positive affect when they experienced more positive events. Anhedonia did not moderate the relation between positive events and PA (B = 0.01, SE = 0.01, p = 0.34).
Depressive Symptoms and Positive Event Generation by PA
PAT-1 was positively related to positive events at T (B = 0.04, SE = 0.01, p < 0.01). Depressive symptoms did not moderate the relation between PAT-1 and positive events (B = 0.01, SE = 0.01, p = 0.48). Anhedonia did not moderate the relation between PA
T-1
and positive events at T (B = 0.00, SE = 0.01, p = 0.89).
Discussion
This study aimed to examine relations between depressive symptoms and experience of pleasure in daily life in early and late adolescents. Results showed that depressive symptoms were related to a lower mean PA, lower proportion of positive events, lower intensity of positive events, and higher general variability in PA regardless of sex and stage of adolescence. We found evidence for reactivity to positive events and to PA (i.e. whether PA elicits positive events), which were both strongest in LA girls. However, no clear evidence was found for moderation of these associations by depressive symptoms. Anhedonia did not drive the associations between depressive symptoms and momentary experiences of pleasure, as it was only related to the proportion of positive events.
Our findings indicated that individuals with many depressive symptoms experienced fewer positive events and less pleasure during these events, which is in line with previous research in adults (Bylsma et al.
2011; Peeters et al.
2003; Thompson et al.
2012). However, we did not find robust evidence that individuals with many depressive symptoms differed in PA reactivity to positive events. Hence, even though they experienced less pleasure and PA, their PA levels increased after positive events to the same extent as individuals with fewer depressive symptoms. We even found a slight indication for the mood brightening effect, which has been previously reported in adults (Bylsma et al.
2011; Peeters et al.
2003; Thompson et al.
2012). These findings indicate that the ability to increase PA after positive events is not affected by depressive symptoms.
With regard to the variability in PA, we found that depressive symptoms were associated with higher general variability and instability (i.e., greater SD and MSSD of PA), but not with lower inertia of PA. It may seem contradictive that we did find an association with the MSSD but not with the autocorrelation of PA, as they both relate to the frequency of shifts. The main difference between these two measures is that the autocorrelation takes the mean level of PA into account, and the MSSD does not. Hence, a strong negative autocorrelation merely reflects frequent shifts around the mean level of PA and a high MSSD indicates frequent and large shifts in PA, irrespective of the mean level of PA. The finding that inertia in PA was not associated with depressive symptoms thus indicates that depressive symptoms are not associated with more fluctuations around the mean level of PA, which is measured by the autocorrelation. Rather, depressive symptoms are related to a greater range in PA levels. These diverging findings illustrate the importance of taking into account multiple measures of variability in one study.
As we found little evidence for differential reactivity to positive events in individuals with many depressive symptoms, it seems implausible that the increased variability in PA in individuals with many depressive symptoms is caused by different responses to positive events. Since it has been shown that adolescents at risk for psychopathology respond more negatively to negative events (Schneiders et al.
2006) and that adolescents with many depressive symptoms benefit more from being with close others (Brown et al.
2011), depression-related shifts in PA are more likely due to differential reactivity to negative events or to enduring characteristics of the social context.
Our daily life measures of PA and positive events may reflect two different types of anhedonic processes. The level of PA during and after positive events may reflect consummatory anhedonia, that is, the ability to enjoy a positive event when it happens (Treadway and Zald
2011). The proportion of positive events may be more strongly related to motivational anhedonia, which is a lack of motivation or desire to engage in pleasurable activities. From this perspective, our findings are inconsistent regarding consummatory anhedonia: Depressive symptoms were related to a lower level of pleasure during positive events (i.e., intensity of positive events), but not associated with the experience of PA in response to positive events. Hence, although adolescents with many depressive symptoms experienced lower levels of pleasure during positive events, they responded similarly to these events as their levels of PA increased to the same extent as in adolescents with fewer depressive symptoms. A possible explanation for these differences in findings with regard to consummatory anhedonia may lie in how we measured these two concepts. The intensity of events was asked directly, as participants rated how pleasurable the most important event was. Therefore, the negative bias that is associated with depressive symptoms may be activated here. For the reactivity to events, we created a link between events and PA in the analyses, hence participants themselves did not consciously make a link between the events and how they felt. The negative bias may thus play less of a role in the assessment of reactivity to events. Further research could try to overcome these issues by including a more objective measure of events (i.e., by having participants describe the most important event, and have these answers rated on level of pleasure by independent coders).
Motivational anhedonia may explain why we found that individuals with many depressive symptoms did not experience as many positive events as individuals with fewer depressive symptoms, as they may not go out and participate in pleasurable activities. To investigate the role of motivational anhedonia, further research should include questions about momentary motivation or drive.
Both age and sex differences were found in the experience of pleasure in daily life. First, EA girls showed greater variability in PA than LA girls and EA boys, which may indicate that there are sex differences in PA variability in early adolescents, and that affect stabilizes in the course of adolescence. Other studies have also found that girls are more variable in PA than boys (Larson and Csikszentmihalyi
1980) and that affect stabilizes throughout adolescence, in both boys and girls (Larson et al.
2002).
Second, the relation between positive events and PA was stronger in LA girls than in EA girls. A possible explanation for this finding may be that the timeframe between two assessments was different between the two samples. Although the amount of time between assessments did not affect the results we found when we controlled for it, it could be that the events reported by the LA sample may have been more important, because the timeframe was larger and hence there were more opportunities for greater events to happen.
Third, PA at a certain time point elicited more positive events at the next time point in LA girls, but not in EA girls and EA boys. A developmental explanation for this age difference could be that young adolescents do not yet have the capacity to elicit more positive events when they are high in PA. To our knowledge, no studies have yet examined age differences in how high levels of PA affect individuals. Further, the EA samples could have had less freedom to choose the activities they wanted to do, because they spent a great amount of time at school and at home with their parents. The older sample consisted mainly of college students who lived on their own (i.e., > 95 %) and did not spend as much time at school as the younger subsamples. Hence, the younger samples may have had the same capacity to experience positive events in case of high PA as the older one, but not be able to materialize this capacity due to their more rigid schedule.
Finally, our findings seem to indicate that anhedonia is more important for the experience of pleasure in daily life later than early in adolescence, as we found that anhedonia was related to the proportion of positive events or pleasure levels during events in LA girls only. Further research is needed to examine the development of anhedonia throughout adolescence.
Limitations
Our study has several limitations. First, although we could examine sex and age differences, we did not have a sample of older boys, and hence could not investigate sex differences in late adolescence. Further research is needed to examine whether some of the differences between EA girls and LA girls are similar for boys. Related to this, we did not measure pubertal development in the early adolescent sample. Previous research has shown that pubertal phase is associated with altered brain reactivity to rewards, with adolescents in mid-late puberty showing increased striatal reactivity to rewards (Forbes et al.
2010). Hence, puberty may affect daily life pleasure within the early adolescent sample. Although it is likely that most adolescents in our sample were in the mid-late pubertal phase, given their age (mean age = 14.20), it would have been interesting to examine whether pubertal stage could explain some of the sex and age differences we found. Second, our samples consisted of healthy individuals and therefore, the mean scores of depressive symptoms were relatively low. To assess potential treatment implications, it is important to examine to what extent the associations found in our samples can be generalized to adolescent patient samples.
A third limitation concerns the depression and anhedonia measures used in this study. Our depression measure only included cognitive-affective symptoms and no somatic symptoms (e.g., fatigue, appetite, sleep abnormalities). Cognitive-affective symptoms are expected to be more strongly related to daily life experiences of pleasure than somatic symptoms, but this is an untested assumption. Moreover, a more extensive measure of anhedonia is warranted. Our 1-item measure that focused on loss of interest (i.e., motivational aspect of pleasure) was related only to the proportion of positive events, which also reflects motivational aspects of anhedonia rather than consummatory aspects. Possibly, an anhedonia measure that differentiates between aspects such as consummatory and motivational anhedonia will reveal more specific associations with daily life experiences of pleasure. The difference in findings between anhedonia and depressive symptoms may be explained by the fact that the anhedonia measure included only one item, whereas the depressive symptoms measure consisted of 6 items. Therefore, the latter measure may be better in explaining daily life experiences, as it is likely a more reliable measure with a greater range.
Fourth, although we used very similar measures in both samples, there were some differences in design that may have affected the results. The main difference was that the time between assessments was larger in the LA subsample. Controlling for the time between assessments did not affect our findings directly, yet it may have impacted the importance of the reported events. Related to this, the amount of time between the assessment of depressive symptoms and the ESM procedure varied between samples. In LA girls, the baseline was consistently administered 1 day prior to the ESM. In EA boys and girls, the baseline questionnaire was administered 2 to 8 weeks prior to the ESM and so may have changed in the period in between. However, our results do not consistently show stronger relations between depressive symptoms and ESM measures in LA girls, which suggests that this time difference has not affected our results to a great extent.
Finally, there are many other factors that could influence daily life pleasure that were not included in the present study. For example, previous research using fMRI has shown that sleep deprivation (Holm et al.
2009; Mullin et al.
2013), peer victimization, and low parental warmth (Casement et al.
2014) are associated with reduced reward reactivity. Hence, further research could examine whether these factors affect or explain the association between depressive symptoms and daily life experiences of pleasure.
Conclusion
In conclusion, this exploratory study showed that depressive symptoms were associated with a lower proportion of positive events and less experienced pleasure during these events in both early and late adolescents. Nevertheless, individuals with many depressive symptoms equally benefited from positive events as adolescents with fewer depressive symptoms, showing that the capacity to enjoy pleasurable events is not affected. These associations were not primarily driven by anhedonia, as anhedonia had little effect on the experience of pleasure in daily life. Pending future research on this topic, interventions might focus on stimulating positive events in individuals with many depressive symptoms, possibly by increasing the motivation to engage in pleasurable activities.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.