Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2021

Open Access 06-02-2020 | Original Article

Decoding identity from motion: how motor similarities colour our perception of self and others

Auteurs: Alexandre Coste, Benoît G. Bardy, Stefan Janaqi, Piotr Słowiński, Krasimira Tsaneva-Atanasova, Juliette Lozano Goupil, Ludovic Marin

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

For more than 4 decades, it has been shown that humans are particularly sensitive to biological motion and extract socially relevant information from it such as gender, intentions, emotions or a person’s identity. A growing number of findings, however, indicate that identity perception is not always highly accurate, especially due to large inter-individual differences and a fuzzy self-recognition advantage compared to the recognition of others. Here, we investigated the self-other identification performance and sought to relate this performance to the metric properties of perceptual/physical representations of individual motor signatures. We show that identity perception ability varies substantially across individuals and is associated to the perceptual/physical motor similarities between self and other stimuli. Specifically, we found that the perceptual representations of postural signatures are veridical in the sense that closely reflects the physical postural trajectories and those similarities between people’ actions elicit numerous misattributions. While, on average, people can well recognize their self-generated actions, they more frequently attribute to themselves the actions of those acting in a similar way. These findings are consistent with the common coding theory and support that perception and action are tightly linked and may modulate each other by virtue of similarity.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Ashby, F. G., & Perrin, N. A. (1988). Toward a unified theory of similarity and recognition. Psychological Review, 95(1), 124. Ashby, F. G., & Perrin, N. A. (1988). Toward a unified theory of similarity and recognition. Psychological Review, 95(1), 124.
go back to reference Beardsworth, T., & Buckner, T. (1981). The ability to recognize oneself from a video recording of one’s movements without seeing one’s body. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18, 19–22. Beardsworth, T., & Buckner, T. (1981). The ability to recognize oneself from a video recording of one’s movements without seeing one’s body. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18, 19–22.
go back to reference Bläsing, B. E., & Sauzet, O. (2018). My action, my self: Recognition of self-created but visually unfamiliar dance-like actions from point-light displays. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1909.PubMedPubMedCentral Bläsing, B. E., & Sauzet, O. (2018). My action, my self: Recognition of self-created but visually unfamiliar dance-like actions from point-light displays. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1909.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Brainard, D. H., & Vision, S. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436. Brainard, D. H., & Vision, S. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.
go back to reference Burger, B., & Toiviainen, P. (2013). MoCap Toolbox – A Matlab toolbox for computational analysis of movement data. In R. Bresin (Ed.), Proceedings of the 10th Sound and Music Computing Conference (pp. 172–178). Stockholm, Sweden: KTH Royal Institute of Technology. Burger, B., & Toiviainen, P. (2013). MoCap Toolbox – A Matlab toolbox for computational analysis of movement data. In R. Bresin (Ed.), Proceedings of the 10th Sound and Music Computing Conference (pp. 172–178). Stockholm, Sweden: KTH Royal Institute of Technology.
go back to reference Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An FMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8), 1243–1249.PubMed Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An FMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8), 1243–1249.PubMed
go back to reference Casile, A., & Giese, M. A. (2006). Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception. Current Biology, 16(1), 69–74.PubMed Casile, A., & Giese, M. A. (2006). Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception. Current Biology, 16(1), 69–74.PubMed
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Cook, R., Johnston, A., & Heyes, C. (2012). Self-recognition of avatar motion: How do I know it’s me? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1729), 669–674.PubMed Cook, R., Johnston, A., & Heyes, C. (2012). Self-recognition of avatar motion: How do I know it’s me? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1729), 669–674.PubMed
go back to reference Coste, A., Słowiński, P., Tsaneva-Atanasova, K., Bardy, B. G., & Marin, L. (2017). Mapping Individual Postural Signatures. In Studies in perception and action XIV: Nineteenth international conference on perception and action (p. 105). Psychology Press. Coste, A., Słowiński, P., Tsaneva-Atanasova, K., Bardy, B. G., & Marin, L. (2017). Mapping Individual Postural Signatures. In Studies in perception and action XIV: Nineteenth international conference on perception and action (p. 105). Psychology Press.
go back to reference Coulson, M. (2004). Attributing emotion to static body postures: Recognition accuracy, confusions, and viewpoint dependence. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(2), 117–139. Coulson, M. (2004). Attributing emotion to static body postures: Recognition accuracy, confusions, and viewpoint dependence. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(2), 117–139.
go back to reference Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9(5), 353–356. Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9(5), 353–356.
go back to reference Dantcheva, A., Velardo, C., D’angelo, A., & Dugelay, J. L. (2011). Bag of soft biometrics for person identification. Multimedia Tools and Applications, 51(2), 739–777. Dantcheva, A., Velardo, C., D’angelo, A., & Dugelay, J. L. (2011). Bag of soft biometrics for person identification. Multimedia Tools and Applications, 51(2), 739–777.
go back to reference Giese, M. A., Thornton, I., & Edelman, S. (2008). Metrics of the perception of body movement. Journal of Vision, 8(9), 13.PubMed Giese, M. A., Thornton, I., & Edelman, S. (2008). Metrics of the perception of body movement. Journal of Vision, 8(9), 13.PubMed
go back to reference Hart, Y., Noy, L., Feniger-Schaal, R., Mayo, A. E., & Alon, U. (2014). Individuality and togetherness in joint improvised motion. PLoS One, 9(2), e87213.PubMedPubMedCentral Hart, Y., Noy, L., Feniger-Schaal, R., Mayo, A. E., & Alon, U. (2014). Individuality and togetherness in joint improvised motion. PLoS One, 9(2), e87213.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Hill, H., & Pollick, F. E. (2000). Exaggerating temporal differences enhances recognition of individuals from point light displays. Psychological Science, 11(3), 223–228.PubMed Hill, H., & Pollick, F. E. (2000). Exaggerating temporal differences enhances recognition of individuals from point light displays. Psychological Science, 11(3), 223–228.PubMed
go back to reference Hohmann, T., Troje, N. F., Olmos, A., & Munzert, J. (2011). The influence of motor expertise and motor experience on action and actor recognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23(4), 403–415. Hohmann, T., Troje, N. F., Olmos, A., & Munzert, J. (2011). The influence of motor expertise and motor experience on action and actor recognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23(4), 403–415.
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937. Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.
go back to reference Jain, A. K., Ross, A. A., & Nandakumar, K. (2011). Introduction to biometrics. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media. Jain, A. K., Ross, A. A., & Nandakumar, K. (2011). Introduction to biometrics. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media.
go back to reference Jeannerod, M. (2003). The mechanism of self-recognition in humans. Behavioural Brain Research, 142(1–2), 1–15.PubMed Jeannerod, M. (2003). The mechanism of self-recognition in humans. Behavioural Brain Research, 142(1–2), 1–15.PubMed
go back to reference Jokisch, D., Daum, I., & Troje, N. F. (2006). Self recognition versus recognition of others by biological motion: Viewpoint-dependent effects. Perception, 35(7), 911–920.PubMed Jokisch, D., Daum, I., & Troje, N. F. (2006). Self recognition versus recognition of others by biological motion: Viewpoint-dependent effects. Perception, 35(7), 911–920.PubMed
go back to reference Knoblich, G., & Flach, R. (2001). Predicting the effects of actions: Interactions of perception and action. Psychological Science, 12(6), 467–472.PubMed Knoblich, G., & Flach, R. (2001). Predicting the effects of actions: Interactions of perception and action. Psychological Science, 12(6), 467–472.PubMed
go back to reference Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2006). The social nature of perception and action. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(3), 99–104. Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2006). The social nature of perception and action. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(3), 99–104.
go back to reference Koul, A., Cavallo, A., Ansuini, C., & Becchio, C. (2016). Doing it your way: How individual movement styles affect action prediction. PLoS One, 11(10), e0165297.PubMedPubMedCentral Koul, A., Cavallo, A., Ansuini, C., & Becchio, C. (2016). Doing it your way: How individual movement styles affect action prediction. PLoS One, 11(10), e0165297.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Latinus, M., McAleer, P., Bestelmeyer, P. E., & Belin, P. (2013). Norm-based coding of voice identity in human auditory cortex. Current Biology, 23(12), 1075–1080.PubMedPubMedCentral Latinus, M., McAleer, P., Bestelmeyer, P. E., & Belin, P. (2013). Norm-based coding of voice identity in human auditory cortex. Current Biology, 23(12), 1075–1080.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Lee, K., Byatt, G., & Rhodes, G. (2000). Caricature effects, distinctiveness, and identification: Testing the face-space framework. Psychological Science, 11(5), 379–385.PubMed Lee, K., Byatt, G., & Rhodes, G. (2000). Caricature effects, distinctiveness, and identification: Testing the face-space framework. Psychological Science, 11(5), 379–385.PubMed
go back to reference Light, L. L., Kayra-Stuart, F., & Hollander, S. (1979). Recognition memory for typical and unusual faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5(3), 212. Light, L. L., Kayra-Stuart, F., & Hollander, S. (1979). Recognition memory for typical and unusual faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5(3), 212.
go back to reference Loula, F., Prasad, S., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Recognizing people from their movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 210–220.PubMed Loula, F., Prasad, S., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Recognizing people from their movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 210–220.PubMed
go back to reference Luce, R. D. (1963). Detection and recognition. In R. D. Luce, R. B. Bush, & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 103–189). New York: Wiley. Luce, R. D. (1963). Detection and recognition. In R. D. Luce, R. B. Bush, & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 103–189). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Noy, L., Dekel, E., & Alon, U. (2011). The mirror game as a paradigm for studying the dynamics of two people improvising motion together. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(52), 20947–20952. Noy, L., Dekel, E., & Alon, U. (2011). The mirror game as a paradigm for studying the dynamics of two people improvising motion together. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(52), 20947–20952.
go back to reference Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.PubMed Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.PubMed
go back to reference Pilz, K. S., & Thornton, I. M. (2017). Idiosyncratic body motion influences person recognition. Visual Cognition, 25(4–6), 539–549. Pilz, K. S., & Thornton, I. M. (2017). Idiosyncratic body motion influences person recognition. Visual Cognition, 25(4–6), 539–549.
go back to reference Pollick, F. E., Fidopiastis, C., & Braden, V. (2001). Recognising the style of spatially exaggerated tennis serves. Perception, 30(3), 323–338.PubMed Pollick, F. E., Fidopiastis, C., & Braden, V. (2001). Recognising the style of spatially exaggerated tennis serves. Perception, 30(3), 323–338.PubMed
go back to reference Pollick, F. E., Paterson, H. M., Bruderlin, A., & Sanford, A. J. (2001). Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition, 82(2), B51–B61.PubMed Pollick, F. E., Paterson, H. M., Bruderlin, A., & Sanford, A. J. (2001). Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition, 82(2), B51–B61.PubMed
go back to reference Pollick, F. E., Lestou, V., Ryu, J., & Cho, S. B. (2002). Estimating the efficiency of recognizing gender and affect from biological motion. Vision Research, 42(20), 2345–2355.PubMed Pollick, F. E., Lestou, V., Ryu, J., & Cho, S. B. (2002). Estimating the efficiency of recognizing gender and affect from biological motion. Vision Research, 42(20), 2345–2355.PubMed
go back to reference Prasad, S., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). Viewpoint and the recognition of people from their movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(1), 39.PubMed Prasad, S., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). Viewpoint and the recognition of people from their movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(1), 39.PubMed
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.
go back to reference Repp, B. H., & Knoblich, G. (2004). Perceiving action identity: How pianists recognize their own performances. Psychological Science, 15, 604–609.PubMed Repp, B. H., & Knoblich, G. (2004). Perceiving action identity: How pianists recognize their own performances. Psychological Science, 15, 604–609.PubMed
go back to reference Rice, A., Phillips, P. J., Natu, V., An, X., & O’Toole, A. J. (2013). Unaware person recognition from the body when face identification fails. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2235–2243.PubMed Rice, A., Phillips, P. J., Natu, V., An, X., & O’Toole, A. J. (2013). Unaware person recognition from the body when face identification fails. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2235–2243.PubMed
go back to reference Rips, L. J. (1989). Similarity, typicality, and categorization. In A. O. Stella Vosniadou (Ed.), Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp. 21–59). New York: Cambridge Univ. Press. Rips, L. J. (1989). Similarity, typicality, and categorization. In A. O. Stella Vosniadou (Ed.), Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp. 21–59). New York: Cambridge Univ. Press.
go back to reference Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Prinz, W. (2007). Perceptual resonance: Action-induced modulation of perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 349–355.PubMed Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Prinz, W. (2007). Perceptual resonance: Action-induced modulation of perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 349–355.PubMed
go back to reference Słowiński, P., Zhai, C., Alderisio, F., Salesse, R., Gueugnon, M., Marin, L., … Tsaneva-Atanasova, K. (2016). Dynamic similarity promotes interpersonal coordination in joint action. Journal of the Royal Society, Interface, 13(116), 20151093.PubMedPubMedCentral Słowiński, P., Zhai, C., Alderisio, F., Salesse, R., Gueugnon, M., Marin, L., … Tsaneva-Atanasova, K. (2016). Dynamic similarity promotes interpersonal coordination in joint action. Journal of the Royal Society, Interface, 13(116), 20151093.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Steel, K., Ellem, E., & Baxter, D. (2015). The application of biological motion research: Biometrics, sport, and the military. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(1), 78–87. Steel, K., Ellem, E., & Baxter, D. (2015). The application of biological motion research: Biometrics, sport, and the military. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(1), 78–87.
go back to reference Su, Y. H., & Keller, P. E. (2018). Your move or mine? Music training and kinematic compatibility modulate synchronization with self-versus other-generated dance movement. Psychological Research, 82(1), 1–19. Su, Y. H., & Keller, P. E. (2018). Your move or mine? Music training and kinematic compatibility modulate synchronization with self-versus other-generated dance movement. Psychological Research, 82(1), 1–19.
go back to reference Sumi, S. (1984). Upside-down presentation of the Johansson moving light-spot pattern. Perception, 13(3), 283–286.PubMed Sumi, S. (1984). Upside-down presentation of the Johansson moving light-spot pattern. Perception, 13(3), 283–286.PubMed
go back to reference Tversky, A., & Gati, I. (1982). Similarity, separability, and the triangle inequality. Psychological review, 89(2), 123.PubMed Tversky, A., & Gati, I. (1982). Similarity, separability, and the triangle inequality. Psychological review, 89(2), 123.PubMed
go back to reference Valentine, T. (1991). A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion, and race in face recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 43(2), 161–204. Valentine, T. (1991). A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion, and race in face recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 43(2), 161–204.
go back to reference Wolff, W. (1931). Zuordnung individueller Gangmerkmale zur Individual- Charakteristik. Beihefte zur Zeitschrift für Angewandte Psychologie, 58, 108–122. Wolff, W. (1931). Zuordnung individueller Gangmerkmale zur Individual- Charakteristik. Beihefte zur Zeitschrift für Angewandte Psychologie, 58, 108–122.
go back to reference Yovel, G., & Belin, P. (2013). A unified coding strategy for processing faces and voices. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(6), 263–271.PubMedPubMedCentral Yovel, G., & Belin, P. (2013). A unified coding strategy for processing faces and voices. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(6), 263–271.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Yovel, G., & O’Toole, A. J. (2016). Recognizing people in motion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(5), 383–395.PubMed Yovel, G., & O’Toole, A. J. (2016). Recognizing people in motion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(5), 383–395.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Decoding identity from motion: how motor similarities colour our perception of self and others
Auteurs
Alexandre Coste
Benoît G. Bardy
Stefan Janaqi
Piotr Słowiński
Krasimira Tsaneva-Atanasova
Juliette Lozano Goupil
Ludovic Marin
Publicatiedatum
06-02-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01290-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2021

Psychological Research 2/2021 Naar de uitgave