Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2015

01-07-2015 | Original Article

Constrained postures and spatial S–R compatibility as measured by the Simon effect

Auteurs: Magali Kreutzfeldt, Marco Leisten, Jochen Müsseler

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Whereas working under constrained postures is known to influence the worker’s perceived comfort and health, little is known in regard to its influence on performance. Employing an Auditory Simon task while varying posture, we investigated the relationship between constrained postures and cognitive processes in three experiments. In Experiment 1 and 2, participants operated a rocker switch or a control knob with one hand either in front or in the back of their body and while either sitting or kneeling. Perceived musculoskeletal exertion was gathered with a questionnaire. Results of the first two experiments showed differently perceived comfort and a minor effect of constrained posture on cognitive performance. However, results indicated that spatial coding in the back compares to either a virtual turn of the observer towards the control device (front-device coding) or along the observer’s hand (effector coding). To clarify this issue the rocker switch was operated with one or two hands in Experiment 3, showing a comparable coding only in the one-hand condition and indicating evidence for the effector-coding hypothesis in the back.
Voetnoten
1
Note that this research can be related to the field of level II perspective taking. See Kessler and Thomson (2010) and Moll and Meltzoff (2011) for an elaboration on the topic and Janczyk (2013) for evidence of costs of mental self-rotation.
 
2
Note that Simon (1968, as cited in Simon, 1990) as well as Heister, Ehrenstein and Schroeder-Heister (1987) demonstrated the existence of the Simon effect with two fingers of one hand. More recent evidence comes from a study by Cho and Proctor (2010).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Beermann, B., Brenscheidt, F., & Siefer, A. (2005). Arbeitsbedingungen in Deutschland—Belastungen, Anforderungen und Gesundheit. Dortmund: Bundesanstalt für Arbeitsschutz und Arbeitsmedizin. Beermann, B., Brenscheidt, F., & Siefer, A. (2005). Arbeitsbedingungen in Deutschland—Belastungen, Anforderungen und Gesundheit. Dortmund: Bundesanstalt für Arbeitsschutz und Arbeitsmedizin.
go back to reference Bhatnager, V., Drury, C. G., & Schiro, S. G. (1985). Posture, postural discomfort, and performance. Human Factors, 27, 189–199.PubMed Bhatnager, V., Drury, C. G., & Schiro, S. G. (1985). Posture, postural discomfort, and performance. Human Factors, 27, 189–199.PubMed
go back to reference Borg, G. (1982). Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 14, 377–381.PubMed Borg, G. (1982). Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 14, 377–381.PubMed
go back to reference Bundesministerium für Arbeit und Soziales (BMAS), & Bundesanstalt für Arbeitsschutz und Arbeitsmedizin (BAuA). (2009). Sicherheit und Gesundheit bei der Arbeit 2009, Unfallverhütungsbericht Arbeit. Berlin: Bundesanstalt für Arbeitsschutz und Arbeitsmedizin. Bundesministerium für Arbeit und Soziales (BMAS), & Bundesanstalt für Arbeitsschutz und Arbeitsmedizin (BAuA). (2009). Sicherheit und Gesundheit bei der Arbeit 2009, Unfallverhütungsbericht Arbeit. Berlin: Bundesanstalt für Arbeitsschutz und Arbeitsmedizin.
go back to reference Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon effect: grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 853–861.PubMed Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon effect: grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 853–861.PubMed
go back to reference DIN EN ISO 9241-9 (2000). Ergonomic requirements for office work with visual display terminals (VDTs)—Part 9: Requirements for non-keyboard input devices. DIN EN ISO 9241-9 (2000). Ergonomic requirements for office work with visual display terminals (VDTs)Part 9: Requirements for non-keyboard input devices.
go back to reference Fabbri, M., Martoni, M., Esposito, M. J., Brighetti, G., & Natale, V. (2006). Postural control after a night without sleep. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2520–2525.PubMedCrossRef Fabbri, M., Martoni, M., Esposito, M. J., Brighetti, G., & Natale, V. (2006). Postural control after a night without sleep. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2520–2525.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Guiard, Y. (1983). The lateral coding of rotations: a study of the Simon effect with wheel-rotation responses. Journal of Motor Behavior, 15, 331–342.PubMedCrossRef Guiard, Y. (1983). The lateral coding of rotations: a study of the Simon effect with wheel-rotation responses. Journal of Motor Behavior, 15, 331–342.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Heister, G., Ehrenstein, W. H., & Schroeder-Heister, P. (1987). Spatial S-R compatibility with unimanual two-finger choice reactions: effects of irrelevant stimulus location. Perception and Psychophysics, 42, 195–201.PubMedCrossRef Heister, G., Ehrenstein, W. H., & Schroeder-Heister, P. (1987). Spatial S-R compatibility with unimanual two-finger choice reactions: effects of irrelevant stimulus location. Perception and Psychophysics, 42, 195–201.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research, 56, 261–268.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research, 56, 261–268.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1996). S-R compatibility effects without response uncertainty. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 546–571.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1996). S-R compatibility effects without response uncertainty. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 546–571.CrossRef
go back to reference Hünting, W., Läubli, T. H., & Grandjean, E. (1981). Postural and visual loads at VDT workplaces I. Constrained postures. Ergonomics, 24, 917–931.PubMedCrossRef Hünting, W., Läubli, T. H., & Grandjean, E. (1981). Postural and visual loads at VDT workplaces I. Constrained postures. Ergonomics, 24, 917–931.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Janczyk, M. (2013). Level 2 perspective taking entails two processes: evidence from PRP experiments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1878–1887.PubMed Janczyk, M. (2013). Level 2 perspective taking entails two processes: evidence from PRP experiments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1878–1887.PubMed
go back to reference Kessler, K., & Thomson, L. A. (2010). The embodied nature of spatial perspective taking: embodied transformation versus sensorimotor interference. Cognition, 114, 72–88.PubMedCrossRef Kessler, K., & Thomson, L. A. (2010). The embodied nature of spatial perspective taking: embodied transformation versus sensorimotor interference. Cognition, 114, 72–88.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kimura, K., Imanaka, K., & Kita, I. (2002). The effects of different instructions for preparatory muscle tension on simple reaction time. Human Movement Science, 21, 947–960.PubMedCrossRef Kimura, K., Imanaka, K., & Kita, I. (2002). The effects of different instructions for preparatory muscle tension on simple reaction time. Human Movement Science, 21, 947–960.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Liao, M.-H., & Drury, C. G. (2000). Posture, discomfort and performance in a VDT task. Ergonomics, 43, 345–359.PubMedCrossRef Liao, M.-H., & Drury, C. G. (2000). Posture, discomfort and performance in a VDT task. Ergonomics, 43, 345–359.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 476–490.CrossRef Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 476–490.CrossRef
go back to reference Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). Perspective-taking and its foundation in joint attention. In N. Eilan, H. Lerman, & J. Roessler (Eds.), Perception, causation, and objectivity. Issues in philosophy and psychology (pp. 286–304). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). Perspective-taking and its foundation in joint attention. In N. Eilan, H. Lerman, & J. Roessler (Eds.), Perception, causation, and objectivity. Issues in philosophy and psychology (pp. 286–304). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Murchison, N. M., & Proctor, R. W. (2013). Spatial compatibility effects with unimanual and bimanual wheel-rotation responses: an homage to Guiard (1983). Journal of Motor Behavior, 45, 441–454.PubMedCrossRef Murchison, N. M., & Proctor, R. W. (2013). Spatial compatibility effects with unimanual and bimanual wheel-rotation responses: an homage to Guiard (1983). Journal of Motor Behavior, 45, 441–454.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Müsseler, J., Koch, I., & Wühr, P. (2005). Testing the boundary conditions for processing irrelevant location information: the cross-task Simon effect. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 708–726. doi:10.1080/09541440540000068.CrossRef Müsseler, J., Koch, I., & Wühr, P. (2005). Testing the boundary conditions for processing irrelevant location information: the cross-task Simon effect. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 708–726. doi:10.​1080/​0954144054000006​8.CrossRef
go back to reference Nag, A., Vyas, H., Shah, P., & Nag, P. K. (2012). Risk factors and musculoskeletal disorders among women workers performing fish processing. American Journal of Industrial Medicine, 55, 833–843.PubMedCrossRef Nag, A., Vyas, H., Shah, P., & Nag, P. K. (2012). Risk factors and musculoskeletal disorders among women workers performing fish processing. American Journal of Industrial Medicine, 55, 833–843.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.PubMedCrossRef Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2013). Confidence intervals for two sample means: calculation, interpretation, and a few simple rules. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 9, 74–80.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2013). Confidence intervals for two sample means: calculation, interpretation, and a few simple rules. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 9, 74–80.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Miles, J. D., & Baroni, G. (2011). Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 242–266.CrossRef Proctor, R. W., Miles, J. D., & Baroni, G. (2011). Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 242–266.CrossRef
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2006). Stimulus-response compatibility principles: data, theory, and application. Boca Raton: Taylor & Francis Group. Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2006). Stimulus-response compatibility principles: data, theory, and application. Boca Raton: Taylor & Francis Group.
go back to reference Roswarski, T. E., & Proctor, R. W. (2000). Auditory stimulus-response compatibility: is there a contribution of stimulus-hand correspondence? Psychological Research, 63, 148–158.PubMedCrossRef Roswarski, T. E., & Proctor, R. W. (2000). Auditory stimulus-response compatibility: is there a contribution of stimulus-hand correspondence? Psychological Research, 63, 148–158.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sanders, A. F. (1980). Some effects of instructed muscle tension on choice reaction time and movement time. In R. S. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 59–74). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Sanders, A. F. (1980). Some effects of instructed muscle tension on choice reaction time and movement time. In R. S. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 59–74). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Sanders, A. F. (1998). Elements of human performance: reaction processes and attention in human skill. Mahwah: Erlbaum. Sanders, A. F. (1998). Elements of human performance: reaction processes and attention in human skill. Mahwah: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Shiu, L. P., & Kornblum, S. (1999). Stimulus-response compatibility effects in go-no-go tasks: a dimensional overlap account. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1613–1623.CrossRef Shiu, L. P., & Kornblum, S. (1999). Stimulus-response compatibility effects in go-no-go tasks: a dimensional overlap account. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1613–1623.CrossRef
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: an integrated perspective (pp. 31–86). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: an integrated perspective (pp. 31–86). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304.PubMedCrossRef Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Simon, J. R., & Small, A. M. (1969). Processing auditory information: interference from an irrelevant cue. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 433–435.PubMedCrossRef Simon, J. R., & Small, A. M. (1969). Processing auditory information: interference from an irrelevant cue. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 433–435.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Spijkers, W. A. (1990). The relation between response-specificity, S-R compatibility, foreperiod duration and muscle-tension in a target aiming task. Acta Psychologica, 75, 262–277.CrossRef Spijkers, W. A. (1990). The relation between response-specificity, S-R compatibility, foreperiod duration and muscle-tension in a target aiming task. Acta Psychologica, 75, 262–277.CrossRef
go back to reference Spilling, S., Eitrheim, J., & Åaras, A. (1986). Cost-benefit analysis of work environment investment at STK’s telephone plant at Kongsvinger. In N. Corlett, J. Wilson, & I. Manenica (Eds.), The ergonomics of working postures (pp. 380–397). London: Taylor & Francis. Spilling, S., Eitrheim, J., & Åaras, A. (1986). Cost-benefit analysis of work environment investment at STK’s telephone plant at Kongsvinger. In N. Corlett, J. Wilson, & I. Manenica (Eds.), The ergonomics of working postures (pp. 380–397). London: Taylor & Francis.
go back to reference Teasdale, N., Bard, C., LaRue, J., & Fleury, M. (1993). On the cognitive penetrability of posture control. Experimental Aging Research, 19, 1–13.PubMedCrossRef Teasdale, N., Bard, C., LaRue, J., & Fleury, M. (1993). On the cognitive penetrability of posture control. Experimental Aging Research, 19, 1–13.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wallace, R. J. (1971). S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 354–360.PubMedCrossRef Wallace, R. J. (1971). S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 354–360.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wang, D.-Y. D., Proctor, R. W., & Pick, D. F. (2003). The Simon effect with wheel-rotation responses. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, 261–273.PubMedCrossRef Wang, D.-Y. D., Proctor, R. W., & Pick, D. F. (2003). The Simon effect with wheel-rotation responses. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, 261–273.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wascher, E. (2005). The timing of stimulus localisation and the Simon effect: an ERP study. Experimental Brain Research, 163, 430–439.PubMedCrossRef Wascher, E. (2005). The timing of stimulus localisation and the Simon effect: an ERP study. Experimental Brain Research, 163, 430–439.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wickens, D. C., Lee, J. D., Liu, Y., & Becker, S. E. G. (2004). Human factors engineering. New Jersey: Pearson. Wickens, D. C., Lee, J. D., Liu, Y., & Becker, S. E. G. (2004). Human factors engineering. New Jersey: Pearson.
go back to reference Worringham, C. J., & Beringer, D. B. (1989). Operator orientation and compatibility in visual-motor task performance. Ergonomics, 32, 387–399.PubMedCrossRef Worringham, C. J., & Beringer, D. B. (1989). Operator orientation and compatibility in visual-motor task performance. Ergonomics, 32, 387–399.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Worringham, C. J., & Beringer, D. B. (1998). Directional stimulus-response compatibility: a test of three alternative principles. Ergonomics, 41, 864–880.PubMedCrossRef Worringham, C. J., & Beringer, D. B. (1998). Directional stimulus-response compatibility: a test of three alternative principles. Ergonomics, 41, 864–880.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Constrained postures and spatial S–R compatibility as measured by the Simon effect
Auteurs
Magali Kreutzfeldt
Marco Leisten
Jochen Müsseler
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2015
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0601-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2015

Psychological Research 4/2015 Naar de uitgave