Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Perspectives on Medical Education 3/2014

Open Access 01-06-2014 | Original article

Conscious versus unconscious thinking in the medical domain: the deliberation-without-attention effect examined

Auteurs: Benno Bonke, Robert Zietse, Geoff Norman, Henk G. Schmidt, Roger Bindels, Sílvia Mamede, Remy Rikers

Gepubliceerd in: Perspectives on Medical Education | Uitgave 3/2014

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Previous studies have shown that with important decisions, unconscious thought has surprisingly led to better choices than conscious thought. The present study challenges this so-called ‘deliberation-without-attention effect’ in the medical domain. In a computerized study, physicians and medical students were asked, after either conscious or unconscious thought, to estimate the 5-year survival probabilities of four fictitious patients with varying medical characteristics. We assumed that experienced physicians would outperform students as a result of their superior knowledge. The central question was whether unconscious thought in this task would lead to better performance in experts or novices, in line with the deliberation-without-attention effect. We created four fictitious male 60-year-old patients, each of whom with signs and symptoms related to likely prognosis, from 12 (Complex) or 4 (Simple) categories. This manipulation resulted in objectively different life expectancies for these patients. Participants (86 experienced physicians and 57 medical students) were randomly allocated to the Simple or Complex condition. Statements were randomly presented for 8 s. Next, each participant assessed the life expectancies after either conscious or unconscious thought. As expected, experienced physicians were better in assessing life expectancies than medical students. No significant differences were found in performance between conscious and unconscious thought, nor did we detect a significant interaction between expertise level and mode of thought. In a medical decision task, unconscious thought did not lead to better performance of experienced physicians or medical students than conscious thought. Our findings do not support the deliberation-without-attention effect.

Introduction

Medical knowledge, intelligence, and reflective reasoning [1, 2] are considered to be the cornerstones of a doctor’s daily work. Yet where critical decisions are to be taken, many doctors are aware of the importance of ‘gut feelings’ and ‘educated intuition’ [3, 4], i.e. unconscious reasoning that may guide behaviour more than conscious knowledge does [58]. It is important to distinguish two ways in which the notion of ‘unconscious’ processes has arisen in the literature. The first is a contrast with conscious analytical processing as a component of a dual process model of reasoning [9] and is synonymous with non-analytical or so-called System 1 thinking. The ‘gut feeling’ alluded to above likely reflects this process, which occurs concurrent with or even ahead of analytical reasoning. A very different kind of unconscious thought arises from studies of the deliberation without attention (DWA) effect, wherein individuals may even profit from unconscious thought [10]—i.e. ‘active thought processes that take place while attention is directed elsewhere’ [11] occurring hours or even days after the event—and take better decisions than after conscious thought. [1217] This effect [12] has, however, been challenged [1825] (see Acker [26] for a meta-analysis and Baumeister et al. [27] for a review) and the findings are not uniformly supportive. In particular, in the medical domain, Mamede et al. [24] recently found that conscious thought resulted in better decisions with complex tasks and experts, contradicting the DWA effect, although it provided no advantage over a DWA condition with students. It is not clear why the effect of DWA would have different effects at different levels of expertise. Conversely, De Vries et al. [15] reported more correct diagnoses of psychiatric case vignettes after unconscious processing than after conscious processing.
Studies showing the DWA effect [1217] have mainly focused on decisions that were of importance to the decision maker, such as ‘what is the best car to buy?’ (although often in hypothetical experimental situations). But, does the effect also occur when decisions are made that are important for others, which is daily practice for doctors? In particular, decisions related to prognosis and therapy need careful consideration, and this is exactly what one may expect from experienced physicians. If, in either Simple or Complex situations, delaying a decision to allow time for unconscious processes to intervene is superior to conscious deliberation, physicians would be advised to postpone a decision and ‘sleep on it.’ This would in many ways be at variance with common sense and daily practice. The claims that originate in studies that supported the DWA effect have, however, attained a large audience [e.g. 1417, 28, 29], so we decided to test if these claims hold true for individuals who make far-reaching decisions daily, i.e. physicians. Likewise, if a DWA effect is present in clinical doctors, the medical educational system must incorporate its repercussions.
In the present study, we confronted physicians and medical students with fictitious patients having objectively determined different life expectancies. Medically relevant characteristics were provided in either a Simple or a Complex task, closely following the design of Dijksterhuis et al. [12]. We aimed to simulate clinical practice where doctors receive a host of information for their patients and then must decide on whom to take action most urgently. Our participants assessed the patients’ life expectancies after either conscious or unconscious thought. We included level of expertise because it is at the basis of optimal decision-making and has been largely ignored in most previous DWA studies. Given our previous findings [24] we expected that experienced physicians, as a result of their large knowledge base built up over the years, are not affected in their performance by mode of thought. In particular, we were interested to find out if a DWA effect occurred in the complex task, because this most closely resembled the task where positive effects had been demonstrated [12].

Methods

Participants

We recruited 150 participants from the departments of internal medicine of academic and non-academic hospitals and from a university medical centre in the Netherlands between April 2009 and May 2011. They included consultants and residents in internal medicine (experts) and advanced medical students starting their clerkships (novices). Experts differed substantially in experience. The data of six participants with inconsistent response sheets and of one participant with language problems were removed. The remaining participants were 86 experts and 57 novices.

Design and materials

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee at the Department of Psychology, Erasmus University Rotterdam, the Netherlands. We created stimulus materials (i.e. cases) by attributing medically relevant characteristics to four fictitious male patients, phrased in statements. We used 12 signs and symptoms that are known to affect a patient’s life expectancy, such as blood pressure, serum creatinine, body mass index (BMI), and alcohol use. Common Dutch surnames (e.g. Klaassens) were used for the patients, renamed here as Lo, Med1, Med2, and Hi (see below). Statements were randomly displayed on a computer screen and each statement clearly identified the patient to whom the information belonged, e.g. Mr Klaassens drinks 4–5 units of alcohol daily. If a statement included a numerical value (e.g. from lab tests) a qualification was added, i.e. elevated, lowered, or normal (or obesity for BMIs >30), as follows: Mr Jansen´s serum creatinine is 70 μmol/L (normal) or Mr De Vries’s blood pressure is 170/110 mmHg (elevated). We attributed neutral (or favourable) and unfavourable statements to the patients in such a way that they differed in the number of neutral/favourable and unfavourable characteristics. Patient Lo was intended to have the lowest life expectancy with 25 % neutral/favourable and 75 % unfavourable characteristics, patients Med1 and Med2 intermediate with both 50 % neutral/favourable and 50 % unfavourable, and patient Hi the highest life expectancy with 75 % neutral/favourable and 25 % unfavourable characteristics. Also, we defined a subset of four characteristics for each patient, with the same distribution of neutral/favourable and unfavourable characteristics, to be used in the simple versions of the study (Table 1). The effectiveness of our manipulation was confirmed by two board-certified experts in internal medicine who independently studied the characteristics of our fictitious patients. Both considered these patients to be typical for what one might expect in an outpatient clinic of internal medicine. They rated the life expectancy of patient Lo as the lowest, that of patient Hi as the highest, and that of patients Med1 and Med2 in between. We used psychological software (E-Prime 2.0 Professional: www.​scienceplus.​nl) to instruct our participants and present the statements and anagram task (see below) on a laptop computer.
Table 1
Medical characteristics as distributed over the four patients; each characteristic was neutral or favourable (+) in two patients and unfavourable (−/−) in the other two
Medical characteristica
Patient Lo
Patient Med1
Patient Med2
Patient Hi
Smoking
−/−
+
−/−
+
Body mass index (kg/m2)
+ (22)
−/− (30)↑
+ (23.5)
−/− (30.5)↑
Family history
−/−
+
−/−
+
Total cholesterol (mmol/L)
−/− (7.3)↑
−/− (7.2)↑
+ (5.3)
+ (5.1)
Blood pressure (mm/Hg)
−/− (170/110)↑
+ (140/80)
−/− (180/105)↑
+ (135/85)
Alcohol use
−/−
+
−/−
+
Haemoglobin (mmol/L)
−/− (6.3)↓
−/− (6.0)↓
+ (8.9)
+ (9.2)
Proteinuria (g/days)
−/− (1.3)↑
−/− (1.0)↑
+
+
Fasting blood sugar (mmol/L)
−/− (10.5)↑
−/− (11)↑
+ (5.0)
+ (4.5)
Body exercise
+
+
−/−
−/−
C-reactive protein (mg/L)
+ (3)
−/− (22)↑
+ (2)
−/− (23)↑
Serum creatinine (μmol/L)
−/− (125)↑
+ (70)
−/− (127)↑
+ (72)
 
25 + 75 % −/−
50 + 50 % −/−
50 + 50 % −/−
75 + 25 % −/−
aOnly the first four characteristics (in italics) were used in the Simple versions; ↑ = elevated (or obesity in case of BMI); ↓ = lowered
We devised an anagram task with randomly presented anagrams of the names of Dutch towns to divert attention in the unconscious thinking mode. We programmed four equivalent versions of our study, i.e. a Simple (SIM) and a Complex (CPX) version, each with both a Conscious (CSC) and an Unconscious (UNC) thinking task after presentation of the statements. Table 2 presents the study design.
Table 2
Study design
Mode of thought
Simple versions (SIM)
Complex versions (CPX)
Conscious (CSC)
4 × 4 statements
4 × 12 statements
4 min conscious thought
4 min conscious thought
Unconscious (UNC)
4× 4 statements
4× 12 statements
4 min anagrams
4 min anagrams

Procedure

Participants volunteered and consented to take part in what was announced as an anonymous, computerized study into memory processes. They were seated in front of a laptop computer and informed that they would be presented, consecutively and in random order, with statements pertaining to four fictitious patients regarding relevant signs and symptoms obtained through history taking, physical examination, or laboratory findings. All patients were supposedly 60-year-old males visiting the outpatient internal medicine clinic, referred by their general practitioners with vague complaints. We randomly assigned participants to one of the four study versions and informed them that after presentation of the statements they were to assess the patients’ life expectancies. We urged them to pay close attention to each statement and to remember which statement belonged to which patient, with a view to assessing the life expectancies, without taking notes. Next, we showed three examples of statements and invited them to ask any questions about the procedure. Then they received, in random order, 16 (SIM-CSC and SIM-UNC) or 48 (CPX-CSC and CPX-UNC) statements—i.e. 4 or 12 statements × 4 patients—each for a duration of 8 s. After presentation of the statements, participants in the CSC versions were prompted to ‘think carefully about the life expectancies of the four patients for a few minutes’ and those in the UNC versions did the anagram task instead. After 4 min in all versions, participants received a response sheet to assess the patients’ life expectancies by writing down the names of the two patients who—in their opinion—had the most and the least favourable life expectancies. Next, they filled in, for each patient separately, their estimated probability that he would be alive in 5 years’ time, as follows: I estimate the probability that Mr De Boer will be alive in 5 years’ time to be …%. The order in which the names appeared on the sheet varied between participants to prevent order effects. Participants stated the number of years they had had experience in internal medicine (none, ‘a few months or less’, 6–12 months, 1–5, >5 years). No further biographical data were obtained. All participants carried out their tasks individually, without being disturbed, and received a small token of appreciation for their participation.

Data reduction

For each participant we ranked the percentages given to the four patients as follows. Rank 1 for the patient who had been given the lowest percentage, rank 4 for the one with the highest percentage given. In case of equal percentages, ranks were averaged. We then computed the rank correlation (Rho) between participant ranks and objective ranks (Lo: rank 1, Med1 and Med2: both rank 2.5, Hi: rank 4) as our dependent variable (range −1.0 to +1.0). Thus Rho = −1.0 if a participant´s assessments ranked completely incorrectly; Rho = +1.0 if a participant ranked correctly.

Statistical analysis

We analyzed differences in Rho for experts and novices using a 2 × 2 × 2 ANOVA with Expertise level (physician, medical student), Mode of thought (CSC, UNC), and Condition (Simple, Complex) as between-factors and Rho as the dependent variable. To generate sufficient statistical power (following Dijksterhuis et al. [12], Studies 1 and 2) we aimed at groups of at least 15 participants in the various conditions. All data were analyzed using SPSS version 18.

Results

For the 57 novices, years of experience was ‘none’ for 55, and ‘a few months or less’ for 2 (one Simple/UNC, one Complex/UNC). For the 86 experts, i.e. residents or consultants with at least some experience, the levels of experience (‘a few months or less’, 6–12 months, 1–5, >5 years) were distributed as follows over the four conditions: Simple/CSC (N = 1, 3, 5, 5, respectively), Simple/UNC (N = 1, 2, 7, 3, respectively), Complex/CSC (N = 1, 2, 13, 12, respectively), Complex/UNC (N = 1, 2, 17, 11, respectively).
As a check upon our manipulation in the Simple and Complex versions, we compared mean ranks for percentages given for patients Lo, Med1, Med2, and Hi, respectively. With minor exceptions for novices in both Complex conditions and for experts in the Simple/UNC condition, mean ranks were in line with our manipulation: lowest for patient Lo, highest for patient Hi, and in between for the two other patients. We considered these results to be confirmation that our manipulation had created fictitious patients that clearly differed in life expectancy in the intended direction (Table 3).
Table 3
Mean ranks of percentages given by novices and experts in their assessments of life expectancy, after conscious or unconscious thought (lowest percentage: rank = 1; highest percentage: rank = 4)
Level of expertise
Simple/Complex
Conscious/Unconscious
Patienta
Mean
SE
Novices (N = 57)
Simple (N = 29)
CSC (N = 15)
Lo
2.03
0.27
Med1
2.40
0.25
Med2
2.37
0.24
Hi
3.20
0.29
UNC (N = 14)
Lo
2.14
0.28
Med1
2.57
0.25
Med2
2.57
0.25
Hi
2.71
0.30
Complex (N = 28)
CSC (N = 15)
Lo
2.43
0.27
Med1
2.47
0.25
Med2
2.20
0.24
Hi
2.90
0.29
UNC (N = 13)
Lo
1.54
0.29
Med1
2.89
0.26
Med2
2.81
0.26
Hi
2.77
0.31
Expertsb (N = 86)
Simple (N = 27)
CSC (N = 14)
Lo
2.18
0.28
Med1
2.25
0.25
Med2
2.46
0.25
Hi
3.11
0.30
UNC (N = 13)
Lo
1.96
0.29
Med1
1.92
0.26
Med2
3.12
0.26
Hi
3.00
0.31
Complex (N = 59)
CSC (N = 28)
Lo
1.68
0.20
Med1
2.54
0.18
Med2
2.41
0.18
Hi
3.38
0.21
UNC (N = 31)
Lo
1.98
0.19
Med1
2.24
0.17
Med2
2.52
0.17
Hi
3.26
0.20
CSC conscious thought, UNC unconscious thought, SE standard error
aPatient Lo has the lowest life expectancy, patient Hi the highest, and patients Med1 and Med2 in between; the lower the mean rank for Patient Lo and the higher the mean rank for Patient Hi, the more accurate the assessments
bWe recruited a greater number of experts to participate in the complex conditions to examine differences in level of expertise; there was no significant difference, therefore we collapsed the two groups
In an initial analysis of the Rho coefficients, the difference in mean scores between experts and novices (Expert, M = 0.44, Novice, M = 0.26) did not quite reach statistical significance (F(1,142) = 3.20, p = 0.08). However, a simplified analysis omitting Mode of thought and Condition did result in a significant difference (p = 0.04). We found no significant main or interaction effects for any of the other factors in the ANOVA (Mode of thought: F(1,142) = 0.23, p = 0.64; Condition: F(1,142) = 0.15, p = 0.70; interactions: all ps > 0.1). When focusing on the DWA effect, we did not find a significant main effect of DWA (F(1,142) = 0.23, p = 0.64 for all cases, F(1,86) = 0.19, p = 0.67 for complex cases). Average rank correlations for all conditions are presented in Table 4.
Table 4
Mean rank correlations (Rho) between participant ranks and objective ranks
Simple/Complex
Conscious/Unconscious
Mean
SE
Novices
 Simple
CSC
0.35
0.15
UNC
0.19
0.16
 Complex
CSC
0.12
0.15
UNC
0.37
0.16
Experts
 Simple
CSC
0.31
0.16
UNC
0.47
0.16
 Complex
CSC
0.53
0.11
UNC
0.47
0.11
CSC conscious thought, UNC unconscious thought; higher means correspond with better task performance

Discussion

The present study investigated task performance in medical doctors and students in evaluating fictitious patients with different life expectancies. Medically relevant characteristics were provided in either a Simple or a Complex task, closely following the design of Dijksterhuis et al. [12]. We expected doctors not to be influenced by mode of thought (i.e. conscious vs. unconscious). As predicted, the deliberation-without-attention effect was absent. Contrary to what this effect predicts, task performance after unconscious thought was not better than after conscious thought [1217].
We verified that our fictitious patients indeed differed in the assumed direction: both the objective statements by the two board-certified internists and the mean ranks over all participants (Table 3) supported our intended manipulation. Compared with Studies 1 and 2 by Dijksterhuis et al. [12], in which such verification was lacking, our double manipulation check can be considered a strength of our study. Also, our participants had a specific expertise in the domain in which they were tested and the chosen age and gender of our fictitious patients made assessments of the life expectancies relevant. Lastly, we carefully selected characteristics that were objectively relevant for a patient’s life expectancy, which resulted in combinations of characteristics that could go together well in real patients.
It would be interesting to see whether a DWA effect occurs when medical decisions are directly important for decision-makers themselves. On the other hand, the present study reaffirms previous research showing that expert doctors do not benefit from unconscious thought while making decisions in complex situations. [24].
The important lesson that can be learned from the present study, and one that opposes the central claim of the DWA framework, is that there is no reason to assume that practising physicians do well to distract themselves with other tasks before making important decisions for patients.
A possible weakness in our study is the artificiality of the experimental setup. Doctors in clinical practice who receive relevant patient information and medical characteristics seldom have to rely on their memory only before deciding what action to take.

Conclusion

In a relevant medical decision task (Simple or Complex), unconscious thought did not lead to better performance than conscious thought. Our findings seem to indicate that the DWA effect, if theoretically valid, does not take place in physicians or in medical students. Our decision task, although theoretically similar to previous studies, failed to reveal a superior effect of unconscious thinking.

Essentials

  • The basic design for the deliberation-without-attention effect was successfully extrapolated to a decision task in the medical domain, where doctors and medical students assessed the life expectancies of virtual patients as a computerized medical decision task.
  • The deliberation-without-attention effect could not be replicated.
  • Unconscious thought did not lead to better performance of experienced physicians or medical students than conscious thought.
  • Experts made more accurate assessments of life expectancies than novices.

Acknowledgments

We thank all volunteers who took part in our study and the two internists who rated the life expectancies for our manipulation check. A special word of thanks goes to our colleagues who recruited participants at the various hospitals. The first author confirms that he had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are credited.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
Literatuur
1.
go back to reference Mamede S, Schmidt HG. The structure of reflective practice in medicine. Med Educ. 2004;38:1302–8.PubMedCrossRef Mamede S, Schmidt HG. The structure of reflective practice in medicine. Med Educ. 2004;38:1302–8.PubMedCrossRef
2.
go back to reference Mamede S, Schmidt HG, Rikers RM, et al. Breaking down automaticity: case ambiguity and the shift to reflective approaches in clinical reasoning. Med Educ. 2007;41:1185–92.PubMedCrossRef Mamede S, Schmidt HG, Rikers RM, et al. Breaking down automaticity: case ambiguity and the shift to reflective approaches in clinical reasoning. Med Educ. 2007;41:1185–92.PubMedCrossRef
3.
go back to reference Van den Bruel A, Thompson M, Buntinx F, Mant D. Clinicians’ gut feeling about serious infections in children: observational study. Br Med J. 2012;345:e6144 10.1136.CrossRef Van den Bruel A, Thompson M, Buntinx F, Mant D. Clinicians’ gut feeling about serious infections in children: observational study. Br Med J. 2012;345:e6144 10.1136.CrossRef
4.
go back to reference Wiswell J, Tsao K, Bellolio F, Hess EP, Cabrera D. ‘Sick’ or ‘not-sick’: accuracy of System 1 diagnostic reasoning for the prediction of disposition and acuity in patients presenting to an academic ED. Am J Emerg Med. 2013;31:1448–52.PubMedCrossRef Wiswell J, Tsao K, Bellolio F, Hess EP, Cabrera D. ‘Sick’ or ‘not-sick’: accuracy of System 1 diagnostic reasoning for the prediction of disposition and acuity in patients presenting to an academic ED. Am J Emerg Med. 2013;31:1448–52.PubMedCrossRef
5.
go back to reference Tversky A, Kahneman D. Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science. 1974;185:1124–31.PubMedCrossRef Tversky A, Kahneman D. Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science. 1974;185:1124–31.PubMedCrossRef
6.
go back to reference Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science. 1997;275:1293–5.PubMedCrossRef Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science. 1997;275:1293–5.PubMedCrossRef
7.
go back to reference Kahneman D, Slovic P, Tversky A. Judgment under uncertainty. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1982.CrossRef Kahneman D, Slovic P, Tversky A. Judgment under uncertainty. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1982.CrossRef
8.
go back to reference Bargh J, Chartrand TL. The unbearable automaticity of being. Am Psychol. 1999;54:462–79.CrossRef Bargh J, Chartrand TL. The unbearable automaticity of being. Am Psychol. 1999;54:462–79.CrossRef
9.
go back to reference Norman G. Dual processing and diagnostic errors. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2009;14(Suppl 1):37–49.PubMedCrossRef Norman G. Dual processing and diagnostic errors. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2009;14(Suppl 1):37–49.PubMedCrossRef
10.
go back to reference Custers R, Aarts H. The unconscious will: how the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science. 2010;329:47–50.PubMedCrossRef Custers R, Aarts H. The unconscious will: how the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science. 2010;329:47–50.PubMedCrossRef
11.
go back to reference Dijksterhuis A, Nordgren LF. A theory of unconscious thought. Persp Psychol Sci. 2006;1:95–109.CrossRef Dijksterhuis A, Nordgren LF. A theory of unconscious thought. Persp Psychol Sci. 2006;1:95–109.CrossRef
12.
go back to reference Dijksterhuis A, Bos MW, Nordgren LF, van Baaren RB. On making the right choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect. Science. 2006;311:1005–7.PubMedCrossRef Dijksterhuis A, Bos MW, Nordgren LF, van Baaren RB. On making the right choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect. Science. 2006;311:1005–7.PubMedCrossRef
13.
go back to reference Dijksterhuis A, van Olden Z. On the benefits of thinking unconsciously: unconscious thought can increase post-choice satisfaction. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2006;42:627–31.CrossRef Dijksterhuis A, van Olden Z. On the benefits of thinking unconsciously: unconscious thought can increase post-choice satisfaction. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2006;42:627–31.CrossRef
14.
go back to reference Dijksterhuis A, Bos MW, van der Leij A, van Baaren RB. Predicting soccer matches after unconscious and conscious thought as a function of expertise. Psychol Sci. 2009;20:1381–7.PubMedCrossRef Dijksterhuis A, Bos MW, van der Leij A, van Baaren RB. Predicting soccer matches after unconscious and conscious thought as a function of expertise. Psychol Sci. 2009;20:1381–7.PubMedCrossRef
15.
go back to reference De Vries M, Witteman CL, Holland RW, Dijksterhuis A. The unconscious thought effect in clinical decision making: an example in diagnosis. Med Decis Making. 2010;30:578–81.PubMedCrossRef De Vries M, Witteman CL, Holland RW, Dijksterhuis A. The unconscious thought effect in clinical decision making: an example in diagnosis. Med Decis Making. 2010;30:578–81.PubMedCrossRef
16.
go back to reference Strick M, Dijksterhuis A, van Baaren RB. Unconscious-thought effects take place off-line, not on-line. Psychol Sci. 2010;21:484–8.PubMedCrossRef Strick M, Dijksterhuis A, van Baaren RB. Unconscious-thought effects take place off-line, not on-line. Psychol Sci. 2010;21:484–8.PubMedCrossRef
17.
19.
go back to reference González-Vallejo C, Lassiter GD, Bellezza FS, Lindberg MJ. ‘Save angels perhaps’: a critical examination of unconscious thought theory and the deliberation-without-attention effect. Rev Gen Psychol. 2008;12:282–96.CrossRef González-Vallejo C, Lassiter GD, Bellezza FS, Lindberg MJ. ‘Save angels perhaps’: a critical examination of unconscious thought theory and the deliberation-without-attention effect. Rev Gen Psychol. 2008;12:282–96.CrossRef
20.
go back to reference González-Vallejo C, Phillips N. Predicting soccer matches: a reassessment of the benefit of unconscious thinking. Judgment Decis Making. 2010;5:200–6. González-Vallejo C, Phillips N. Predicting soccer matches: a reassessment of the benefit of unconscious thinking. Judgment Decis Making. 2010;5:200–6.
21.
go back to reference Newell BR, Wong KY, Cheung JC, Rakow T. Think, blink or sleep on it? The impact of modes of thought on complex decision making. Q J Exp Psychol. 2009;62:707–32.CrossRef Newell BR, Wong KY, Cheung JC, Rakow T. Think, blink or sleep on it? The impact of modes of thought on complex decision making. Q J Exp Psychol. 2009;62:707–32.CrossRef
22.
go back to reference Rey A, Goldstein RM, Perruchet P. Does unconscious thought improve complex decision making? Psychol Res. 2009;73:372–9.PubMedCrossRef Rey A, Goldstein RM, Perruchet P. Does unconscious thought improve complex decision making? Psychol Res. 2009;73:372–9.PubMedCrossRef
23.
go back to reference Lassiter GD, Lindberg MJ, González-Vallejo C, et al. The deliberation-without-attention effect: evidence for an artifactual interpretation. Psychol Sci. 2009;20:671–5.PubMedCrossRef Lassiter GD, Lindberg MJ, González-Vallejo C, et al. The deliberation-without-attention effect: evidence for an artifactual interpretation. Psychol Sci. 2009;20:671–5.PubMedCrossRef
24.
go back to reference Mamede S, Schmidt HG, Rikers RM, et al. Conscious thought beats deliberation without attention in diagnostic decision-making: at least when you are an expert. Psychol Res. 2010;74:586–92.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Mamede S, Schmidt HG, Rikers RM, et al. Conscious thought beats deliberation without attention in diagnostic decision-making: at least when you are an expert. Psychol Res. 2010;74:586–92.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
25.
go back to reference Aczel B, Lukacs B, Komlos J, Aitken MRF. Unconscious intuition or conscious analysis? Critical questions for the deliberation-without-attention paradigm. Judgm Decis Mak. 2011;6:351–8. Aczel B, Lukacs B, Komlos J, Aitken MRF. Unconscious intuition or conscious analysis? Critical questions for the deliberation-without-attention paradigm. Judgm Decis Mak. 2011;6:351–8.
26.
go back to reference Acker F. New findings on unconscious versus conscious thought in decision making: additional empirical data and meta-analysis. Judgm Decis Mak. 2008;4:292–303. Acker F. New findings on unconscious versus conscious thought in decision making: additional empirical data and meta-analysis. Judgm Decis Mak. 2008;4:292–303.
27.
go back to reference Baumeister RF, Masicampo EJ, Vohs KD. Do conscious thoughts cause behavior? Annu Rev Psychol. 2011;62:331–61.PubMedCrossRef Baumeister RF, Masicampo EJ, Vohs KD. Do conscious thoughts cause behavior? Annu Rev Psychol. 2011;62:331–61.PubMedCrossRef
28.
go back to reference Brunelli A, Pompili C, Salati M. Patient selection for operation: the complex balance between information and intuition. J Thorac Dis. 2013;5:8–11.PubMedCentralPubMed Brunelli A, Pompili C, Salati M. Patient selection for operation: the complex balance between information and intuition. J Thorac Dis. 2013;5:8–11.PubMedCentralPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Conscious versus unconscious thinking in the medical domain: the deliberation-without-attention effect examined
Auteurs
Benno Bonke
Robert Zietse
Geoff Norman
Henk G. Schmidt
Roger Bindels
Sílvia Mamede
Remy Rikers
Publicatiedatum
01-06-2014
Uitgeverij
Bohn Stafleu van Loghum
Gepubliceerd in
Perspectives on Medical Education / Uitgave 3/2014
Print ISSN: 2212-2761
Elektronisch ISSN: 2212-277X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40037-014-0126-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2014

Perspectives on Medical Education 3/2014 Naar de uitgave