Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2013

01-07-2013 | Original Article

Conditional automaticity in subliminal morphosyntactic priming

Auteurs: Ulrich Ansorge, Bert Reynvoet, Jessica Hendler, Lennart Oettl, Stefan Evert

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2013

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

We used a gender-classification task to test the principles of subliminal morphosyntactic priming. In Experiment 1, masked, subliminal feminine or masculine articles were used as primes. They preceded a visible target noun. Subliminal articles either had a morphosyntactically congruent or incongruent gender with the targets. In a gender-classification task of the target nouns, subliminal articles primed the responses: responses were faster in congruent than incongruent conditions (Experiment 1). In Experiment 2, we tested whether this congruence effect depended on gender relevance. In line with a relevance-dependence, the congruence effect only occurred in a gender-classification task but was absent in another categorical discrimination of the target nouns (Experiment 2). The congruence effect also depended on correct word order. It was diminished when nouns preceded articles (Experiment 3). Finally, the congruence effect was replicated with a larger set of targets but only for masculine targets (Experiment 4). Results are discussed in light of theories of subliminal priming in general and of subliminal syntactic priming in particular.
Voetnoten
1
It could be argued that even this syntactically incongruent sentence was in fact syntactically congruent. However, these are original example sequences from the study of Sereno. We preferred to take this example because at least with these two sequences, the same prime word ‘your’ was used in congruent and incongruent conditions. This was not the case in many other instances of Sereno’s study in which different prime words were used in congruent than incongruent conditions.
 
2
The gender-orthogonal category membership of every target word (as belonging either to the body parts or to the cutlery/china category) was used for target discrimination in the subsequent Experiment 2. This was done to create conditions in which gender-specific deflections of the determiners were task-irrelevant and, due to the syntactic polyvalence of the determiners, a gender-specific conditionally automatic priming effect should be absent with the same determiners as primes.
 
3
In the present study, this was only true for the two masculine china/cutlery target nouns that had the same singular as plural word form (‘Teller’, and ‘Löffel’). For the two masculine body-part target nouns with different singular (‘Arm’ and ‘Mund’) than plural word forms (‘Arme’ and ‘Münder’, respectively) the German word ‘die’ was not a syntactically fitting plural article. The masculine body-part target conditions allowed us therefore to assess task-independent syntactic prime-target congruence effects in Experiment 2’s gender-independent category task. No such congruence effect was found. Therefore, the task relevance was crucial for the primes’ syntactic use.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Ansorge, U., Fuchs, I., Khalid, S., & Kunde, W. (2011). No conflict control in the absence of awareness. Psychological Research, 75, 351–365.PubMedCrossRef Ansorge, U., Fuchs, I., Khalid, S., & Kunde, W. (2011). No conflict control in the absence of awareness. Psychological Research, 75, 351–365.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ansorge, U., Kiss, M., & Eimer, M. (2009). Goal-driven attentional capture by invisible colours: Evidence from event-related potentials. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 648–653.CrossRef Ansorge, U., Kiss, M., & Eimer, M. (2009). Goal-driven attentional capture by invisible colours: Evidence from event-related potentials. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 648–653.CrossRef
go back to reference Ansorge, U., & Neumann, O. (2005). Intentions determine the effect of invisible metacontrast-masked primes: Evidence for top-down contingencies in a peripheral cueing task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 762–777.PubMedCrossRef Ansorge, U., & Neumann, O. (2005). Intentions determine the effect of invisible metacontrast-masked primes: Evidence for top-down contingencies in a peripheral cueing task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 762–777.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ayora, P., Janssen, N., Dell’Acqua, R., & Alario, F.-X. (2009). Attentional requirements for the selection of words from different grammatical categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1344–1351.PubMedCrossRef Ayora, P., Janssen, N., Dell’Acqua, R., & Alario, F.-X. (2009). Attentional requirements for the selection of words from different grammatical categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1344–1351.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bargh, J. A. (1992). The ecology of automaticity: Toward establishing the conditions needed to produce automatic processing effects. American Journal of Psychology, 105, 181–199.PubMedCrossRef Bargh, J. A. (1992). The ecology of automaticity: Toward establishing the conditions needed to produce automatic processing effects. American Journal of Psychology, 105, 181–199.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bock, K., Loebell, H., & Morey, R. (1992). Frome conceptual roles to structural relations: Bridging the syntactic cleft. Psychological Review, 99, 150–171.PubMedCrossRef Bock, K., Loebell, H., & Morey, R. (1992). Frome conceptual roles to structural relations: Bridging the syntactic cleft. Psychological Review, 99, 150–171.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brants, S., Dipper, S., Hansen, S., Lezius, W., & Smith, G. (2002). The TIGER treebank. Proceedings of the Workshop on Treebanks and Linguistic Theories. Sozopol, Bulgaria. Brants, S., Dipper, S., Hansen, S., Lezius, W., & Smith, G. (2002). The TIGER treebank. Proceedings of the Workshop on Treebanks and Linguistic Theories. Sozopol, Bulgaria.
go back to reference Coane, J. H., & Balota, D. A. (2010). Repetition priming across distinct contexts: Effects of lexical status, word frequency, and retrieval test. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2367–2398. Coane, J. H., & Balota, D. A. (2010). Repetition priming across distinct contexts: Effects of lexical status, word frequency, and retrieval test. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2367–2398.
go back to reference Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.CrossRef Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.CrossRef
go back to reference Damian, M. F. (2001). Congruity effects evoked by subliminally presented primes: Automaticity rather than semantic processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 154–164.PubMedCrossRef Damian, M. F. (2001). Congruity effects evoked by subliminally presented primes: Automaticity rather than semantic processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 154–164.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec’H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., et al. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395, 597–600.PubMedCrossRef Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec’H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., et al. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395, 597–600.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dent, K., Johnston, R. A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Age of acquisition and word frequency effects in picture naming: A dual-task investigations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 282–301.PubMedCrossRef Dent, K., Johnston, R. A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Age of acquisition and word frequency effects in picture naming: A dual-task investigations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 282–301.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fayol, M., Largy, P., & Lemaire, P. (1994). Cognitive overload and orthographic errors: When cognitive overload enhances subject-verb agreement errors. A study in French written language. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 437–464. Fayol, M., Largy, P., & Lemaire, P. (1994). Cognitive overload and orthographic errors: When cognitive overload enhances subject-verb agreement errors. A study in French written language. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 437–464.
go back to reference Ferreira, V. F., Bock, K., Wilson, M. P., & Cohen, N. J. (2008). Memory for syntax despite amnesia. Psychological Science, 19, 940–946.PubMedCrossRef Ferreira, V. F., Bock, K., Wilson, M. P., & Cohen, N. J. (2008). Memory for syntax despite amnesia. Psychological Science, 19, 940–946.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ferreira, V. S., & Pashler, H. (2002). Central bottleneck influences on the processing stages of word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 1187–1199.PubMedCrossRef Ferreira, V. S., & Pashler, H. (2002). Central bottleneck influences on the processing stages of word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 1187–1199.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Finkbeiner, M., & Palermo, R. (2009). The role of spatial attention in nonconscious processing: A comparison of face and non-face stimuli. Psychological Science, 20, 42–51.PubMedCrossRef Finkbeiner, M., & Palermo, R. (2009). The role of spatial attention in nonconscious processing: A comparison of face and non-face stimuli. Psychological Science, 20, 42–51.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Flores d’Arcais, G. B. (1988). Automatic processes in language comprehension. In B. Denes, C. Semenza, & P. Bisiach (Eds.), Perspectives on cognitive neuropsychology (pp. 93–114). Mawah: Leah. Flores d’Arcais, G. B. (1988). Automatic processes in language comprehension. In B. Denes, C. Semenza, & P. Bisiach (Eds.), Perspectives on cognitive neuropsychology (pp. 93–114). Mawah: Leah.
go back to reference Forster, K. I. (1979). Levels of processing and the structure of the language processor. In W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walker (Eds.), Sentence processing (pp. 27–85). New Jersey: Erlbaum. Forster, K. I. (1979). Levels of processing and the structure of the language processor. In W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walker (Eds.), Sentence processing (pp. 27–85). New Jersey: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Forster, K. I. (1998). The pros and cons of masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 680–698.CrossRef Forster, K. I. (1998). The pros and cons of masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 680–698.CrossRef
go back to reference Friedrici, A. D., & Jacobsen, T. (1999). Processing grammatical gender during language comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 467–484.CrossRef Friedrici, A. D., & Jacobsen, T. (1999). Processing grammatical gender during language comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 467–484.CrossRef
go back to reference Friederici, A. D., & Weissenborn, J. (2007). Mapping sentence form onto meaning: The syntax-semantic interface. Brain Research, 1146, 50–58.PubMedCrossRef Friederici, A. D., & Weissenborn, J. (2007). Mapping sentence form onto meaning: The syntax-semantic interface. Brain Research, 1146, 50–58.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Garrett, M. F. (1988). Processes in language production. In F. N. Newmeyer (Ed.), Language: Psychological and biological aspects (pp. 69–96). New York: Cambridge University Press. Garrett, M. F. (1988). Processes in language production. In F. N. Newmeyer (Ed.), Language: Psychological and biological aspects (pp. 69–96). New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley. Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273, 1699–1702.PubMedCrossRef Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273, 1699–1702.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gunter, T. C., Friedrici, A. D., & Schriefers, H. (2000). Syntactic gender and semantic expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 556–568.PubMedCrossRef Gunter, T. C., Friedrici, A. D., & Schriefers, H. (2000). Syntactic gender and semantic expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 556–568.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Haagort, P., & Brown, C. M. (1999). Gender electrified: ERP evidence on the syntactic nature of gender processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 715–728.CrossRef Haagort, P., & Brown, C. M. (1999). Gender electrified: ERP evidence on the syntactic nature of gender processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 715–728.CrossRef
go back to reference Hasting, A. S., & Kotz, S. A. (2008). Speeding up syntax: On the relative timing and automaticity of local phrase structure and morphosyntactic processing as reflected in event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1207–1219.PubMedCrossRef Hasting, A. S., & Kotz, S. A. (2008). Speeding up syntax: On the relative timing and automaticity of local phrase structure and morphosyntactic processing as reflected in event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1207–1219.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jescheniak, J. D., & Levelt, J. M. (1994). Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 824–843.CrossRef Jescheniak, J. D., & Levelt, J. M. (1994). Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 824–843.CrossRef
go back to reference Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman, R. Davies, & J. Beatty (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 29–61). New York: Academic Press. Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman, R. Davies, & J. Beatty (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 29–61). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Kiefer, M. (2002). The N400 is modulated by unconsciously perceived masked words: Further evidence for a spreading activation account of N400 priming effects. Cognitive Brain Research, 13, 27–39.PubMedCrossRef Kiefer, M. (2002). The N400 is modulated by unconsciously perceived masked words: Further evidence for a spreading activation account of N400 priming effects. Cognitive Brain Research, 13, 27–39.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kiefer, M., & Martens, U. (2010). Attentional sensitization of unconscious cognition: Task sets modulate subsequent masked semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 464–489.CrossRef Kiefer, M., & Martens, U. (2010). Attentional sensitization of unconscious cognition: Task sets modulate subsequent masked semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 464–489.CrossRef
go back to reference Kjellmer, G. (1991). A mint of phrases. In K. Ajmer & B. Altenberg (Eds.), English corpus linguistics. London: Longman. Kjellmer, G. (1991). A mint of phrases. In K. Ajmer & B. Altenberg (Eds.), English corpus linguistics. London: Longman.
go back to reference Koester, D., Gunter, T. C., & Wagner, S. (2007). The morphosyntactic decomposition and semantic composition of German compound words investigated by ERP. Brain and Language, 102, 64–79.PubMedCrossRef Koester, D., Gunter, T. C., & Wagner, S. (2007). The morphosyntactic decomposition and semantic composition of German compound words investigated by ERP. Brain and Language, 102, 64–79.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kouider, S., & Dupoux, E. (2004). Partial awareness creates the “illusion” of subliminal semantic priming. Psychological Science, 15, 75–81.PubMedCrossRef Kouider, S., & Dupoux, E. (2004). Partial awareness creates the “illusion” of subliminal semantic priming. Psychological Science, 15, 75–81.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition, 88, 223–242.PubMedCrossRef Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition, 88, 223–242.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kunde, W., Reuss, H., & Kiesel, A. (2012). Consciousness and cognitive control. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8, 9–18.PubMed Kunde, W., Reuss, H., & Kiesel, A. (2012). Consciousness and cognitive control. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8, 9–18.PubMed
go back to reference Marcel, A. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 197–237.PubMedCrossRef Marcel, A. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 197–237.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Martens, U., Ansorge, U., & Kiefer, M. (2011). Controlling the unconscious: Attentional task sets modulate subliminal semantic and visuo-motor processes differentially. Psychological Science, 22, 282–291.PubMedCrossRef Martens, U., Ansorge, U., & Kiefer, M. (2011). Controlling the unconscious: Attentional task sets modulate subliminal semantic and visuo-motor processes differentially. Psychological Science, 22, 282–291.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mattler, U. (2006). On the locus of priming and inverse priming effects. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 975–991.CrossRef Mattler, U. (2006). On the locus of priming and inverse priming effects. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 975–991.CrossRef
go back to reference Michelbacher, L., Evert, S., & Schütze, H. (2007). Asymmetric association measures. Proceedings of the international conference on recent advances in natural language processing. Borovets, Bulgaria. Michelbacher, L., Evert, S., & Schütze, H. (2007). Asymmetric association measures. Proceedings of the international conference on recent advances in natural language processing. Borovets, Bulgaria.
go back to reference Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2001). Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli. Cognition, 80, 215–229.PubMedCrossRef Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2001). Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli. Cognition, 80, 215–229.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading of activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226–254.CrossRef Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading of activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226–254.CrossRef
go back to reference Norris, D., & Kinoshita, S. (2008). Perception as evidence accumulation and Bayesian inference: Insights from masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 433–455.CrossRef Norris, D., & Kinoshita, S. (2008). Perception as evidence accumulation and Bayesian inference: Insights from masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 433–455.CrossRef
go back to reference Plaut, D. C., & Booth, J. R. (2000). Individual and developmental differences in semantic priming: Empirical and computational support for a single-mechanism account of lexical processing. Psychological Review, 107, 786–823.PubMedCrossRef Plaut, D. C., & Booth, J. R. (2000). Individual and developmental differences in semantic priming: Empirical and computational support for a single-mechanism account of lexical processing. Psychological Review, 107, 786–823.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Pulvermüller, F., Shtyrov, Y., Hasting, A. S., & Carlyon, R. P. (2008). Syntax as a reflex. Neurophysiological evidence for early automaticity of grammatical processing. Brain and Language, 104, 244–253.PubMedCrossRef Pulvermüller, F., Shtyrov, Y., Hasting, A. S., & Carlyon, R. P. (2008). Syntax as a reflex. Neurophysiological evidence for early automaticity of grammatical processing. Brain and Language, 104, 244–253.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Reingold, E. M., & Merikle, P. (1988). Using direct and indirect measures to study perception without awareness. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 563–575.CrossRef Reingold, E. M., & Merikle, P. (1988). Using direct and indirect measures to study perception without awareness. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 563–575.CrossRef
go back to reference Reingold, E. M., & Merikle, P. (1990). On the inter-relatedness of theory and measurement in the study of unconscious processes. Mind and Language, 5, 9–28.CrossRef Reingold, E. M., & Merikle, P. (1990). On the inter-relatedness of theory and measurement in the study of unconscious processes. Mind and Language, 5, 9–28.CrossRef
go back to reference Reynvoet, B., Gevers, W., & Caessens, B. (2005). Unconscious primes activate motor codes through semantics. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 991–1000.PubMedCrossRef Reynvoet, B., Gevers, W., & Caessens, B. (2005). Unconscious primes activate motor codes through semantics. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 991–1000.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sereno, J. A. (1991). Graphemic, associative, and syntactic priming effects at a brief stimulus onset asynchrony in lexical decision and naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 459–477.CrossRef Sereno, J. A. (1991). Graphemic, associative, and syntactic priming effects at a brief stimulus onset asynchrony in lexical decision and naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 459–477.CrossRef
go back to reference Van den Bussche, E., & Reynvoet, B. (2007). Masked priming effects in semantic categorization are independent of category size. Experimental Psychology, 54, 225–235.PubMedCrossRef Van den Bussche, E., & Reynvoet, B. (2007). Masked priming effects in semantic categorization are independent of category size. Experimental Psychology, 54, 225–235.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference van Opstal, F., Reynvoet, B., & Verguts, T. (2005). Unconscious semantic categorization and mask interactions: An elaborate response to Kunde et al. (2005). Cognition, 97, 107–113.CrossRef van Opstal, F., Reynvoet, B., & Verguts, T. (2005). Unconscious semantic categorization and mask interactions: An elaborate response to Kunde et al. (2005). Cognition, 97, 107–113.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Conditional automaticity in subliminal morphosyntactic priming
Auteurs
Ulrich Ansorge
Bert Reynvoet
Jessica Hendler
Lennart Oettl
Stefan Evert
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2013
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2013
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0442-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2013

Psychological Research 4/2013 Naar de uitgave