Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Perspectives on Medical Education 4/2018

Open Access 18-04-2018 | Original Article

Communication skills training and the conceptual structure of empathy among medical students

Auteurs: Daisuke Son, Ikuo Shimizu, Hirono Ishikawa, Muneyoshi Aomatsu, Jimmie Leppink

Gepubliceerd in: Perspectives on Medical Education | Uitgave 4/2018

Abstract

Introduction

Medical and healthcare professionals’ empathy for patients is crucially important for patient care. Some studies have suggested that a significant decline in empathy occurs during clinical training years in medical school as documented by self-assessed empathy scales. Moreover, a recent study provided qualitative evidence that communication skills training in an examination context, such as in an objective structured clinical examination, might stimulate perspective taking but inhibit the development of compassionate care. Therefore, the current study examined how perspective taking and compassionate care relate to medical students’ willingness to show empathic behaviour and how these relations may change with communication skills training.

Methods

A total of 295 fourth-year Japanese medical students from three universities completed the Jefferson Empathy Scale and a newly developed set of items on willingness to show empathic behaviour twice after communication skills training, pertaining to post-training and retrospectively for pre-training.

Results

The findings indicate that students’ willingness to show empathic behaviour is much more correlated with perspective taking than with compassionate care. Qualitative descriptive analysis of open-ended question responses revealed a difficulty of feeling compassion despite showing empathic behaviour.

Discussion

These findings shed light on the conceptual structure of empathy among medical students and generate a number of hypotheses for future intervention and longitudinal studies on the relation between communication skills training and empathy.
Opmerkingen

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s40037-018-0431-z) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

What this paper adds

Medical and healthcare professionals’ empathy for patients is crucially important for patient care. Some studies have suggested that a significant decline in empathy occurs during clinical training years in medical school as documented by self-assessed empathy scales. This study indicates that students’ willingness to show empathic behaviour is much more related to perspective taking than to compassionate care.

Introduction

Physicians’ empathy for patients is crucially important for gathering appropriate information from patients and for understanding patients’ illnesses and suffering. Empathy enables a physician to provide quality patient care [1] without becoming too emotionally involved [2, 3]. Moreover, empathy appears to be associated with personal and professional growth, well-being and career satisfaction [46]. Finally, empathy is positively related to patient satisfaction, increased compliance to treatment and fewer complaints of malpractice by patients [7].
Some studies suggest that a significant decline in empathy occurs during the clinical training years in medical school as documented by self-assessed empathy scales along with an increased cynicism and emotional exhaustion in later years [4, 5, 810]. However, more recent work underlines that changes in empathy during medical school should not be conceived in terms of some ‘overall’ decline but that different components of empathy have to be assessed to understand where changes take place [7]. Whether or not we detect and come to understand changes in empathy may to some extent be a matter of perspective and definition. If we perceive empathy mainly in terms of understanding of patients’ experiences, concerns and perspectives combined with a capacity to communicate this understanding with an intention to help [1113], we view empathy through a cognitive lens. Some have distinguished between a perspective taking or cognitive component on the one hand and a compassionate care or emotive component on the other hand [14, 15]. Others have gone one step further by arguing that compassionate care is an integral part of sympathy rather than of empathy [16, 17].
When we adopt the view that perspective taking and compassionate care are two components of empathy, an instrument that attempts to measure these components is found in the Jefferson Scale of Empathy [1012, 15]. This instrument was designed to measure empathy in physicians and other healthcare professionals as well as students in medicine and healthcare. It has been translated into more than 50 languages and has been used in more than 80 countries.
When administered together with an instrument that can measure students’ willingness to show empathic behaviour, we may gain understanding of the roles of perspective taking and compassionate care in students’ willingness to show empathic behaviour. If compassionate care has a substantial role, students who are more willing to show empathic behaviour should be inclined to give higher ratings on compassionate care. Consequently, we should be able to establish a positive correlation between these two variables. Moreover, we might see changes in that correlation with communication skills training. On the one hand, if the role of compassionate care in students’ willingness to show empathic behaviour increases with training, we should see a higher correlation between compassionate care and willingness to show empathic behaviour after than before training. On the other hand, if the role of compassionate care in students’ willingness to show empathic behaviour is reduced with training, we should see a lower correlation between compassionate care and willingness to show empathic behaviour after than before training.
The latter scenario appears to receive support from a cross-sectional cohort study in Japan: empathy scores did not differ substantially across the six years of medical school and focus group sessions revealed that communication skills training in an examination context, such as in an objective structured clinical examination (OSCE), might inhibit the development of compassionate care and stimulate perspective taking instead [18, 19]. In other words, even if compassionate care relates to empathy in the initial stage, it might start doing so to a lesser extent with communication skills training.
Therefore, the current study examined how perspective taking and compassionate care relate to medical students’ willingness to show empathic behaviour and how these relations may change with communication skills training in a regular training and assessment context in a medical curriculum. In line with the findings from the aforementioned cross-sectional cohort study in Japan, we expected that the relation between perspective taking and willingness to show empathic behaviour would increase whereas the relation between compassionate care and willingness to show empathic behaviour would decrease with communication skills training.

Method

The current study was conducted from October 2015 to February 2016 in medical schools in three Japanese universities.

Participants and design

A total of 295 fourth-year Japanese medical students from three universities completed the Jefferson Scale of Empathy and a newly developed willingness to show empathic behaviour scale twice after communication skills training, pertaining to post-training and retrospectively for pre-training. In other words, students completed the questionnaire once for post-training and once retrospectively on their empathy before training. While the retrospective pre/post self-report approach comes with the limitations of possible memory effects and participants’ tendency to think that some change—preferably learning—must have taken place (i.e., good subject effect) [20], it has the advantage over an actual pre/post design that it does not suffer from a response shift bias due to a relative inability to assess one’s own ability when prior knowledge or experience is lacking [21]. One of the consequences of such a response shift bias could be that students give lower post-OSCE than pre-OSCE ratings on perspective taking, compassionate care or willingness to show empathic behaviour because the OSCE has made them more aware of how much they can still grow rather than that the OSCE results in an actual decrease in any of these factors.

Materials and procedure

The OSCE is a common achievement test conducted every year in all medical schools in Japan, usually in the late fourth year in a six-year curriculum, to assess students’ clinical competencies. Most medical schools, including the three medical schools that participated in the current study, teach communication skills to fourth-year students using simulated patients in several medical interviewing training sessions before the OSCE takes place. Questionnaire administration took place right after the OSCE.
We used the Japanese student version (i.e., S‑Version) of the Jefferson Scale of Empathy [18]. This questionnaire has 20 items, each to be rated on a seven-point Likert scale. In previous studies, factor analysis revealed three factors: perspective taking (10 items), compassionate care (8 items) and standing in the patient’s shoes (2 items) [22]. As we were interested in how perspective taking and compassionate care relate to willingness to show empathic behaviour, we developed a new three-item scale that aimed to measure students’ willingness to show empathic behaviour and fit in the context of the training. The training has been a regular part of the medical curriculum in the universities that participated in this study. Face validity of the willingness to show empathic behaviour items was acquired from the experiences and comments from tutors engaged in the training as well as from students. Table 1 presents the entire questionnaire of perspective taking, compassionate care and willingness to show empathic behaviour items.
Table 1
The questionnaire used in this study. Assumptions: items 2, 4, 5, 9, 10, 13, 15–17 and 20 are indicators of perspective taking; items 1, 7, 8, 11, 12, 14, 18 and 19 are indicators of compassionate care; and items 21–23 are indicators of willingness to show empathic behaviour
1
I believe that empathy is an important therapeutic factor in medical treatment
2
Patients feel better when their physicians understand their feelings
3
It is difficult for a physician to view things from a patient’s perspectives
4
Understanding body language is as important as verbal communication in physician-patient relationships
5
A physician’s sense of humour contributes to a better clinical outcome
6
Because people are different, it is difficult to see things from the patient’s perspective
7
Attention to patients’ emotions is not important in history taking
8
Attentiveness to patients’ personal experiences does not influence treatment outcomes
9
Physicians should try to stand in their patients’ shoes when providing care to them
10
Patients value a physician’s understanding of their feelings, which is therapeutic in its own right
11
Patients’ illnesses can be cured only by medical or surgical treatment; therefore physicians’ emotional ties with the patients do not have a significant influence in medical or surgical treatment
12
Asking patients about what is happening in their personal lives is not helpful in understanding their physical complaints
13
Physicians should try to understand what is going on in their patients’ minds by paying attention to their non-verbal cues and body language
14
I believe that emotion has no place in the treatment of medical illness
15
Empathy is a therapeutic skill without which the physician’s success is limited
16
Physicians’ understanding of the emotional status of their patients, as well as that of their families, is one important component of the physician-patient relationship
17
Physicians should try to think like their patients in order to render better care
18
Physicians should not allow themselves to be influenced by strong personal bonds between their patients and their family members
19
I do not enjoy reading non-medical literature of the arts
20
I believe that empathy is an important therapeutic factor in medical treatment
21
I will show empathic behaviour to patients when I see their distress
22
I will show empathic behaviour to patients when I see them feeling pain
23
I will show empathic behaviour to patients when I hear their difficult experiences
Additionally, we included one open-ended question: ‘How did medical interviewing training sessions and the OSCE influence your communication skills in terms of empathy?’ Finally, we also asked students to indicate the number of medical interviewing training sessions they had attended.

Data analysis

Although 295 students participated in the study, seven students (2.4% of 295 students) completed fewer than 53% of the items. Furthermore, of the 288 students who completed the questionnaire (217 men, 66 women, and 5 did not specify; average age 23.1 years, eight students did not specify), one student omitted 13% of the items, one student omitted 11% of the items, 20 other students omitted less than 7% of the items, and the remaining 268 students responded to all items.
To examine the relation between perspective taking (10 items), compassionate care (8 items) and willingness to show empathic behaviour (3 items), we performed confirmatory factor analysis in Jamovi 0.8.1.18 [23] , conform the questionnaire structure displayed in Table 1, with full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing response [24], for both post-training and pre-training, respectively. Moreover, to gain an additional understanding of the relation between the number of medical interviewing training sessions and each of perspective taking, compassionate care and willingness to show empathic behaviour, we performed linear mixed-effects analysis in Jamovi 0.8.1.18 [23] , treating retrospective pre-training and post-training as repeated measurements, the number of medical interviewing training sessions as between-subjects factor and student-level intercept as random effect. The latter was based on only 234 students (79% of 295 students) due to non-response of the other students to the number of medical interviewing training sessions and only one student indicating 0 medical interviewing training sessions. Thus, the number of medical interviewing training sessions consisted of three categories: 1 (n = 31), 2 (n = 135) and 3 (n = 68).
Finally, responses to the open-ended question on the self-perceived influence of the number of medical interviewing training sessions and OSCE on their communication skills in terms of empathy were subjected to qualitative descriptive analysis [25] to check for recurring themes that could help triangulate the findings from confirmatory factor analysis and/or linear mixed-effects analysis. First author DS conducted the initial coding and three other authors (IS, HI and MA) reviewed that coding. Disagreements were discussed until consensus was reached.

Ethical considerations

Students received an informed consent form, and those who understood the purpose of the research and agreed to participate received the questionnaire. Questionnaires were administered anonymously. Participants were informed that the survey was not mandatory and had no relation to grading or study progress. The study was conducted under the approval of the Institutional Review Board of the University of Tokyo, Shinshu University, and Nagoya University, where the study was implemented.

Results

Tables 2 and 3 present the outcomes of confirmatory factor analysis of retrospective pre-training and post-training. Additionally, a correlation matrix of the items for each of retrospective pre-training and post-training is included in an Appendix, which can be found online as Electronic Supplementary Material.
Table 2
Confirmatory factor analysis for retrospective pre-training (comparative fit index = 0.947; Tucker-Lewis index = 0.940; root mean square error of approximation = 0.057) and post-training (comparative fit index = 0.918; Tucker-Lewis index = 0.908; root mean square error of approximation = 0.068): standardized loadings
Factor
Item
Pre
Post
Perspective taking
 2
0.522
0.494
 4
0.631
0.651
 5
0.319
0.319
 9
0.736
0.652
10
0.467
0.474
13
0.773
0.732
15
0.495
0.397
16
0.799
0.744
17
0.432
0.404
20
0.721
0.724
Compassionate care
 1
0.499
0.455
 7
0.782
0.714
 8
0.862
0.779
11
0.794
0.836
12
0.774
0.756
14
0.684
0.785
18
0.183
0.253
19
0.542
0.580
Willingness to show Empathic behaviour
21
0.958
0.949
22
0.986
0.973
23
0.946
0.941
Table 3
Confirmatory factor analysis for retrospective pre-training: factor correlations
Factor
Factor
Pre
Post
Perspective taking
Compassionate care
0.494
0.469
Perspective taking
Willingness to show empathic behaviour
0.631
0.699
Compassionate care
Willingness to show empathic behaviour
0.244
0.334
The comparative fit index and Tucker-Lewis index are estimates of the proportion of sample information explained by the model, with a theoretical range of 0–1, and values above 0.90 generally being considered adequate [26]. The root mean square error of approximation should be below 0.08 and preferably below 0.06 [27]. In other words, the comparative fit index values of 0.947 (retrospective pre-training) and 0.918 (post-training) indicate good fit, and the root mean square error of approximation values of 0.057 (retrospective pre-training) and 0.068 (post-training) are also reasonable.

Perspective taking, compassionate care, willingness to show empathic behaviour interrelations

The high loadings for willingness to show empathic behaviour indicate that the attempt to create a unidimensional willingness to show empathic behaviour scale has succeeded. The correlation between perspective taking and compassionate care was 0.494 for retrospective pre-training and 0.469 for post-training. The correlation between perspective taking and willingness to show empathic behaviour was 0.631 for retrospective pre-training and 0.699 for post-training. The correlation between compassionate care and willingness to show empathic behaviour was 0.244 for retrospective pre-training and 0.334 for post-training.

How the number of medical interviewing training sessions relates to perspective taking, compassionate care and willingness to show empathic behaviour

Table 4 shows the mean scores and standard deviations of perspective taking, compassionate care and willingness to show empathic behaviour for retrospective pre-training and post-training, and for each number of medical interviewing training sessions (i.e., 1, 2 or 3).
Table 4
Means (and standard deviations) for perspective taking, compassionate care and willingness to show empathic behaviour for retrospective pre- and post-training per number of medical interviewing training sessions
Occasion
Medical
Perspective
Compassionate
Willingness to show
Interviewing training sessions
Taking
Care
Empathic behaviour
Pre
1
5.522 (0.829)
5.372 (0.783)
5.033 (1.108)
2
5.262 (0.940)
5.097 (0.866)
5.346 (1.294)
3
5.209 (0.986)
5.380 (0.793)
5.392 (1.173)
Pre
1
5.560 (0.999)
5.679 (0.678)
5.544 (1.030)
2
5.522 (1.041)
5.548 (0.858)
5.843 (1.163)
3
5.552 (1.022)
5.740 (0.748)
5.912 (1.167)
Although there was a modest increase in perspective taking (95% confidence interval: 0.057–0.150 points), compassionate care (95% confidence interval: 0.150–0.240) and willingness to show empathic behaviour (95% confidence interval: 0.179–0.333) from retrospective pre-training to post-training, both the main effect of the number of medical interviewing training sessions and its interaction with occasion were small and not statistically significant for any of these factors. In other words, we failed to find evidence for any relation between the number of medical interviewing training sessions and perspective taking, compassionate care or willingness to show empathic behaviour.

Self-perceived effects of medical interviewing training sessions and OSCE on communication skills with regard to empathy

Table 5 highlights the 16 themes that emerged, with 12 themes on positive aspects of learning and 4 themes on difficulties of learning.
Table 5
Qualitative descriptive analysis of students’ open-ended responses
What students learned
Learning how to show empathic attitudes to patients
Learning the procedure of medical interviewing
Learning the importance of patients’ perspective taking
Learning the importance of plain explaining
Recognizing that communication skill are learnable
Influence of communication on physician-patient healing relationship
Learning the importance of listening to patients
Recognizing that medical interviewing training is a process of socialization
Variety of patients’ responses to the same question
Learning the importance of efficacy of communication
Recognizing that empathy is an essential characteristic of a good physician
Learning that appropriate spacing is important
What students found difficult to learn
Difficulty of patients’ perspective taking
Difficulty of empathizing patients’ feelings
Difficulty of conveying what doctor wants to patients
Difficulty of feeling compassion despite showing empathic behaviour
On the positive aspects, students reported among others ‘learning how to show empathic attitudes to patients’ and ‘recognizing that communications skills are learnable.’ Recurring difficulties were the ‘difficulty of empathizing with patients’ feelings’ and the ‘difficulty of feeling compassion despite showing empathic behaviour’. Some students elaborated further. For instance, one student proceeded that ‘even if I try to show compassion, sometimes I cannot care about it due to my lack of skills,’ and another student wrote ‘I learned some skills of interviewing patients, but I could not empathize with patients very well; I felt like I had lied when I said emphasizing words to them.’ Taken together, these findings from the open-ended responses appear to indicate that students experience difficulties in empathizing with patients emotionally yet do manage to acquire the cognitive skills that are needed to communicate with patients. This interpretation is in line with the confirmatory factor analysis outcomes in that the correlation between compassionate care and willingness to show empathic behaviour was substantially lower than the correlation between perspective taking and willingness to show empathic behaviour.

Discussion

The current study examined how perspective taking and compassionate care relate to willingness to show empathic behaviour and how these relations may change with communication skills training. Although for both perspective taking and compassionate care the correlation with willingness to show empathic behaviour was a bit higher for post-training than for retrospective pre-training, the correlation between perspective taking and willingness to show empathic behaviour was on both occasions substantially higher than the correlation between compassionate care and willingness to show empathic behaviour. In this light, the open-ended question responses revealed among others a difficulty of feeling compassion despite showing empathic behaviour. These findings shed light on the conceptual structure of empathy among medical students and generate a number of hypotheses for future intervention and longitudinal studies on the relation between communication skills training and empathy.

Cultural context

Future studies could replicate the current study in a different stage of the medical curriculum and subsequent career as well as in different cultural settings. The associations of medical students’ empathy scores with outcomes such as professionalism, empathic behaviour in later years of the curriculum, or patient satisfaction and compliance may have been studied quite well in Western contexts [16, 2830] but remain relatively underexplored in non-Western cultures. In the Japanese context, empathy is also considered a critical component of professionalism, especially linked with the Bushido concept of jin or benevolence [31]. However, future studies are needed with regard to how Japanese physicians or medical students show empathic behaviour to patients and how this is linked to patient outcomes.

Curriculum factors

Apart from considering cultural context, some new studies should focus on the longitudinal development of perspective taking, compassionate care and willingness to show empathic behaviour. The current study provided some indication that fourth-year medical students may not be able to care a lot about showing compassion because they lack skills that may be needed to facilitate perspective taking. With their rather preliminary cognitive schemas of the type of cases under consideration, the average fourth-year medical student may need to allocate their working memory resources to cognitive processes that are needed for perspective taking to such an extent that fewer resources are available for dealing with emotion or compassionate care [32, 33]. Although this may sound odd at first, research has demonstrated that emotion in patient encounter may—certainly in the early stages of learning—consume working memory resources that are needed for cognitive processes related to learning [34, 35]. Longitudinal studies could provide more insight into how cognitive and emotive processes affect students’ willingness to show empathic behaviour as they learn and enter subsequent stages of training.

Assessment effects

Another factor that may influence students’ focus or lack of focus on compassionate care, even when their cognitive schemas are sufficiently developed to engage in compassionate care, is that of the assessment context. There is some evidence that adding a hypothesis-driven component to OSCEs can foster the engagement of clinical reasoning when preparing for an OSCE if students expect that they are going to be assessed on clinical reasoning as well [36]. Future experimental studies could test to what extent such (pre-)assessment effects also occur when adding a compassionate care component to the OSCE assessment. Given the apparent importance of developed cognitive schemas, one might expect an interaction between cognitive schemas and assessment context: the assessment context might not matter that much among students whose cognitive schemas are still limited (e.g., fourth-year medical students), but might well make a difference at a later stage. If so, the interaction might explain why in a previous study fifth-year medical students weighed more on the emotive component of empathy, whereas residents weighed more on the cognitive component of empathy [15]. If the students in that study expected to be assessed predominantly on the cognitive component, any focus on the emotive component might be extraneous activity in the context of their preparation for that assessment.

Different approaches to training

The training in the current study has been a regular part of the medical curriculum in the universities that participated in this study and, as in the vast majority of cases, did not undergo additional validation. Future studies could consider alternative approaches to training such as the Process Communication Model ® [37, 38]. Based on motivational and personality theory, this model provides an approach to teaching communication and training communication skills.

Measurement and triangulation

Finally, our willingness to show empathic behaviour items did not undergo validation additional to the establishment of face validity. Although the willingness to show empathic behaviour items developed for this study fit in the context of the training of focus in this study and are easy to administer, this set of items could be developed further and adapted for different contexts. Additionally, future studies should consider using direct observation and other alternatives to self-reported willingness to show empathic behaviour.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
Bijlagen

Caption Electronic Supplementary Material

Literatuur
1.
go back to reference Hojat M, Louis DZ, Markham FW, Wender R, Rabinowitz C, Gonnella JS. Physicians’ empathy and clinical outcomes for diabetic patients. Acad Med. 2011;86:359–64.CrossRef Hojat M, Louis DZ, Markham FW, Wender R, Rabinowitz C, Gonnella JS. Physicians’ empathy and clinical outcomes for diabetic patients. Acad Med. 2011;86:359–64.CrossRef
2.
go back to reference Hojat M, Vergare MJ, Isenberg G, Cohen M, Spandorfer J. Underlying construct of empathy, optimism, and burnout in medical students. Int J Med Educ. 2015;6:12–6.CrossRef Hojat M, Vergare MJ, Isenberg G, Cohen M, Spandorfer J. Underlying construct of empathy, optimism, and burnout in medical students. Int J Med Educ. 2015;6:12–6.CrossRef
3.
go back to reference Santen SA, Holt DB, Kemp JD, Hemphill RR. Burnout in medical students: examining the prevalence and associated factors. South Med J. 2010;103:758–63.CrossRef Santen SA, Holt DB, Kemp JD, Hemphill RR. Burnout in medical students: examining the prevalence and associated factors. South Med J. 2010;103:758–63.CrossRef
4.
go back to reference Hojat M, Vergare MJ, Maxwell K, Brainard G, Herrine SK, Isenberg GA, Veloski J, Gonnella JS. The devil is in the third year: a longitudinal study of erosion of empathy in medical school. Acad Med. 2009;84:1182–91.CrossRef Hojat M, Vergare MJ, Maxwell K, Brainard G, Herrine SK, Isenberg GA, Veloski J, Gonnella JS. The devil is in the third year: a longitudinal study of erosion of empathy in medical school. Acad Med. 2009;84:1182–91.CrossRef
5.
go back to reference Neumann M, Edelhäuser F, Tauschel D, et al. Empathy decline and its reasons: a systematic review of studies with medical students and residents. Acad Med. 2011;86:996–1009.CrossRef Neumann M, Edelhäuser F, Tauschel D, et al. Empathy decline and its reasons: a systematic review of studies with medical students and residents. Acad Med. 2011;86:996–1009.CrossRef
6.
go back to reference Thomas MR, Dyrbye LN, Huntington JL, Lawson KL, Novotny PJ, Sloan JA, Shanafelt TD. How do distress and well-being relate to medical student empathy? A multicentre study. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22:177–83.CrossRef Thomas MR, Dyrbye LN, Huntington JL, Lawson KL, Novotny PJ, Sloan JA, Shanafelt TD. How do distress and well-being relate to medical student empathy? A multicentre study. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22:177–83.CrossRef
7.
go back to reference Smith KE, Norman GJ, Decety J. The complexity of empathy during medical school training: evidence for positive changes. Med Educ. 2017;51:1146–59.CrossRef Smith KE, Norman GJ, Decety J. The complexity of empathy during medical school training: evidence for positive changes. Med Educ. 2017;51:1146–59.CrossRef
8.
go back to reference Hojat M, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, et al. An empirical study of decline in empathy in medical school. Med Educ. 2004;38:934–41.CrossRef Hojat M, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, et al. An empirical study of decline in empathy in medical school. Med Educ. 2004;38:934–41.CrossRef
9.
go back to reference Hojat M, Gonnella JS, Veloski J. Rebuttals to critics of studies of the decline of empathy. Acad Med. 2010;85:1812.CrossRef Hojat M, Gonnella JS, Veloski J. Rebuttals to critics of studies of the decline of empathy. Acad Med. 2010;85:1812.CrossRef
10.
go back to reference Roff S. Reconsidering the ‘decline’ of medical student empathy as reported in studies using the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy-Student version (JSPE-S). Med Teach. 2015;37:783–6.CrossRef Roff S. Reconsidering the ‘decline’ of medical student empathy as reported in studies using the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy-Student version (JSPE-S). Med Teach. 2015;37:783–6.CrossRef
11.
go back to reference Hojat M, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, et al. The Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy: development and preliminary psychometric data. Educ Psychol Meas. 2001;61:349–65.CrossRef Hojat M, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, et al. The Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy: development and preliminary psychometric data. Educ Psychol Meas. 2001;61:349–65.CrossRef
12.
go back to reference Hojat M, Gonnella JS, Nasca TJ, Mangione S, Veloski J, Magee M. The Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy: further psychometric data and differences by gender and specialty at item level. Acad Med. 2002;77:S58–S60.CrossRef Hojat M, Gonnella JS, Nasca TJ, Mangione S, Veloski J, Magee M. The Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy: further psychometric data and differences by gender and specialty at item level. Acad Med. 2002;77:S58–S60.CrossRef
13.
go back to reference Hojat M, Gonnella JS, Nasca TJ, Mangione S, Veloski J, Magee M. Physician empathy: definition, components, measurement, and relationship to gender and specialty. Am J Psychol. 2002;159:1563–9.CrossRef Hojat M, Gonnella JS, Nasca TJ, Mangione S, Veloski J, Magee M. Physician empathy: definition, components, measurement, and relationship to gender and specialty. Am J Psychol. 2002;159:1563–9.CrossRef
14.
go back to reference Gladstein GA. Understanding empathy: integrating counselling, developmental, and social psychology perspectives. J Couns Psychol. 1983;30:467–82.CrossRef Gladstein GA. Understanding empathy: integrating counselling, developmental, and social psychology perspectives. J Couns Psychol. 1983;30:467–82.CrossRef
15.
go back to reference Hojat M, Gonnella JS, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, Magee M. Physician empathy in medical education and practice: experience with the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy. Semin Integr Med. 2003;1:25–41.CrossRef Hojat M, Gonnella JS, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, Magee M. Physician empathy in medical education and practice: experience with the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy. Semin Integr Med. 2003;1:25–41.CrossRef
16.
go back to reference Hojat M, Louis DZ, Maxwell K, Markham F, Wender R, Gonnella JS. Patient perceptions of physician empathy, satisfaction with physician, interpersonal trust, and compliance. Int J Med Educ. 2010;1:83.CrossRef Hojat M, Louis DZ, Maxwell K, Markham F, Wender R, Gonnella JS. Patient perceptions of physician empathy, satisfaction with physician, interpersonal trust, and compliance. Int J Med Educ. 2010;1:83.CrossRef
17.
go back to reference Hojat M. Empathy in patient care: antecedents, development, measurement, and outcomes. New York: Springer; 2007. Hojat M. Empathy in patient care: antecedents, development, measurement, and outcomes. New York: Springer; 2007.
18.
go back to reference Kataoka HU, Koide N, Ochi K, Hojat M, Gonnella JS. Measurement of empathy among Japanese medical students: psychometrics and score differences by gender and level of medical education. Acad Med. 2009;84:1192–7.CrossRef Kataoka HU, Koide N, Ochi K, Hojat M, Gonnella JS. Measurement of empathy among Japanese medical students: psychometrics and score differences by gender and level of medical education. Acad Med. 2009;84:1192–7.CrossRef
19.
go back to reference Aomatsu M, Otani T, Tanaka A, Ban N, Van Dalen J. Medical students’ and residents’ conceptual structure of empathy: a qualitative study. Educ Health (Abingdon). 2013;26:4–8.CrossRef Aomatsu M, Otani T, Tanaka A, Ban N, Van Dalen J. Medical students’ and residents’ conceptual structure of empathy: a qualitative study. Educ Health (Abingdon). 2013;26:4–8.CrossRef
20.
go back to reference Orne MT. On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: with particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. Am Psychol. 1962;17:776.CrossRef Orne MT. On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: with particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. Am Psychol. 1962;17:776.CrossRef
21.
go back to reference Howard GS, Dailey PR. Response-shift bias: a source of contamination of self-report measures. J Appl Psychol. 1979;62:144.CrossRef Howard GS, Dailey PR. Response-shift bias: a source of contamination of self-report measures. J Appl Psychol. 1979;62:144.CrossRef
22.
go back to reference Hojat M, LaNoue M. Exploration and confirmation of the latent variable structure of the Jefferson scale of empathy. Int J Med Educ. 2014;5:73–81.CrossRef Hojat M, LaNoue M. Exploration and confirmation of the latent variable structure of the Jefferson scale of empathy. Int J Med Educ. 2014;5:73–81.CrossRef
24.
go back to reference Enders CK. A primer on maximum likelihood algorithms available for use with missing data. Struct Equ Modeling. 2001;8:128–41.CrossRef Enders CK. A primer on maximum likelihood algorithms available for use with missing data. Struct Equ Modeling. 2001;8:128–41.CrossRef
25.
go back to reference Sandelowski M. Focus on research methods – whatever happened to qualitative description? Res Nurs Health. 2000;23:334–40.CrossRef Sandelowski M. Focus on research methods – whatever happened to qualitative description? Res Nurs Health. 2000;23:334–40.CrossRef
26.
go back to reference Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. 1999;6(1):55.CrossRef Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. 1999;6(1):55.CrossRef
27.
go back to reference Leppink J, Paas F, Van der Vleuten CPM, Van Gog T, Van Merriënboer JJG. Development of an instrument for measuring different types of cognitive load. Behav Res Methods. 2013;45:1058–72.CrossRef Leppink J, Paas F, Van der Vleuten CPM, Van Gog T, Van Merriënboer JJG. Development of an instrument for measuring different types of cognitive load. Behav Res Methods. 2013;45:1058–72.CrossRef
28.
go back to reference Glaser KM, Markham FW, Adler HM, McManus PR, Hojat M. Relationships between scores on the Jefferson Scale of physician empathy, patient perceptions of physician empathy, and humanistic approaches to patient care: a validity study. Med Sci Monit. 2007;13:CR291–CR4. Glaser KM, Markham FW, Adler HM, McManus PR, Hojat M. Relationships between scores on the Jefferson Scale of physician empathy, patient perceptions of physician empathy, and humanistic approaches to patient care: a validity study. Med Sci Monit. 2007;13:CR291–CR4.
29.
go back to reference Hojat M, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, Gonnella JS, Magee M. Empathy scores in medical school and ratings of empathic behavior in residency training 3 years later. J Soc Psychol. 2005;145:663–72.CrossRef Hojat M, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, Gonnella JS, Magee M. Empathy scores in medical school and ratings of empathic behavior in residency training 3 years later. J Soc Psychol. 2005;145:663–72.CrossRef
30.
go back to reference Pohl CA, Hojat M, Arnold L. Peer nominations as related to academic attainment, empathy, personality, and specialty interest. Acad Med. 2011;86:747–51.CrossRef Pohl CA, Hojat M, Arnold L. Peer nominations as related to academic attainment, empathy, personality, and specialty interest. Acad Med. 2011;86:747–51.CrossRef
31.
go back to reference Nishigori H, Harrison R, Busari J, Dornan T. Bushido and medical professionalism in Japan. Acad Med. 2014;89:560–3.CrossRef Nishigori H, Harrison R, Busari J, Dornan T. Bushido and medical professionalism in Japan. Acad Med. 2014;89:560–3.CrossRef
32.
go back to reference Leppink J, Duvivier R. Twelve tips for medical curriculum design from a cognitive load theory perspective. Med Teach. 2016;38:669–74.CrossRef Leppink J, Duvivier R. Twelve tips for medical curriculum design from a cognitive load theory perspective. Med Teach. 2016;38:669–74.CrossRef
33.
go back to reference Leppink J, Van den Heuvel A. The evolution of cognitive load theory and its application to medical education. Perspect Med Educ. 2015;4:119–27.CrossRef Leppink J, Van den Heuvel A. The evolution of cognitive load theory and its application to medical education. Perspect Med Educ. 2015;4:119–27.CrossRef
34.
go back to reference Fraser K, Huffman J, Ma I, Sobczak M, McIlwrick J, Wright B, McLaughlin K. The emotional and cognitive impact of unexpected simulated patient death: a randomized controlled trial. Chest. 2014;145:958–63.CrossRef Fraser K, Huffman J, Ma I, Sobczak M, McIlwrick J, Wright B, McLaughlin K. The emotional and cognitive impact of unexpected simulated patient death: a randomized controlled trial. Chest. 2014;145:958–63.CrossRef
35.
go back to reference Fraser K, Ma I, Teteris E, Baxter H, Wright B, McLaughlin K. Emotion, cognitive load and learning outcomes during simulation training. Med Educ. 2012;46:1055–62.CrossRef Fraser K, Ma I, Teteris E, Baxter H, Wright B, McLaughlin K. Emotion, cognitive load and learning outcomes during simulation training. Med Educ. 2012;46:1055–62.CrossRef
36.
go back to reference Lafleur A, Côté L, Leppink J. Influences of OSCE design on students’ diagnostic reasoning. Med Educ. 2015;49:203–14.CrossRef Lafleur A, Côté L, Leppink J. Influences of OSCE design on students’ diagnostic reasoning. Med Educ. 2015;49:203–14.CrossRef
37.
go back to reference Gilbert M. The process communication model: understanding ourselves and others. NASSP Bull. 1996;80:75–80.CrossRef Gilbert M. The process communication model: understanding ourselves and others. NASSP Bull. 1996;80:75–80.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Communication skills training and the conceptual structure of empathy among medical students
Auteurs
Daisuke Son
Ikuo Shimizu
Hirono Ishikawa
Muneyoshi Aomatsu
Jimmie Leppink
Publicatiedatum
18-04-2018
Uitgeverij
Bohn Stafleu van Loghum
Gepubliceerd in
Perspectives on Medical Education / Uitgave 4/2018
Print ISSN: 2212-2761
Elektronisch ISSN: 2212-277X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40037-018-0431-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2018

Perspectives on Medical Education 4/2018 Naar de uitgave