Collaboration during encoding
We found a collaborative encoding deficit, whereby individuals who encoded together performed worse than individuals who encoded alone. This negative effect seems to be long-lasting since it remained both at Recall 1 and Recall 2 and occurred regardless of how recall was performed, as revealed by the absence of an interaction. Therefore, encoding deficits and collaborative inhibition seem to depend on different mechanisms. ARC scores were also lower in the collaborative than in the individual encoding condition, albeit only in the second recall, suggesting that the organization of the information is less integrated and more superficial when participants collaborate to classify the words. A possible explanation for the collaborative encoding deficit is that social interactions with other individuals divert attention away from the encoding task (Barber et al.,
2012) and encourage divided attention (DA; Anderson et al.,
1998; Baddeley et al.,
1984; Craik et al.,
1996). In our experiment, the individual encoding conditions allowed the participant to focus exclusively on the encoding task and the stimuli to be remembered, all of which may have favored subsequent recall.
Differently, in conditions involving collaborative encoding, the encoding of information while listening to the partner may be considered as a dual task that can be more or less demanding depending on the complexity of the information being communicated. Moreover, here participants not only have to attend to the memory task, but also to social information about the partner like their attitude, or facial expressions. They may also be more likely to be distracted or to focus their attention on aspects other than the information essential for successful task performance (e.g., discussions with a partner or explanation of why to place a word in a particular category). These additional aspects can be viewed as secondary tasks that compete for cognitive resources and potentially impact the primary task, resulting in an encoding deficit. These findings are in line with previous research from Pereira-Pasarin and Rajaram (
2011) who argued that divided attention during encoding reduced retrieval organization in recall. In studies using various paradigms, DA during the study phase changed the quality of the encoding and shifted from a more semantically elaborated type to a shallow one (Naveh-Benjamin et al.,
2000). In addition, DA negatively affects both memory and secondary task performance, compared to full attention (FA) conditions (Fernandes & Moscovitch,
2000; Naveh-Benjamin et al.,
1998). Although research on collaborative memory has focused mainly on cognitive processes (see Weldon et al.,
2000 for motivational factors), it could be relevant to determine whether other social processes (e.g., such as social conformity or fear of judgment from others) may mediate performance in collaborative groups.
Collaboration during retrieval
Regarding the effects of collaboration during retrieval, we found the classic effect of collaborative inhibition, whereby dyads who recall individually have better nominal performance (Andersson et al.,
2006; Marion & Thorley,
2016; Rajaram,
2011; Rajaram & Pereira-Pasarin,
2010; Saraiva et al.
2023). These results are also in line with findings from a previous study by Weldon and Balinger (
1997) who observed that collaborative triads in the first recall evoked more information than individual subjects but less than nominal groups (the sum of the three non-redundant individual recalls). The effect of collaborative inhibition was also evident when encoding was collaborative. Indeed, the group that encoded collaboratively and recalled individually (Col–Ind group) remembered more information than the group that recalled dyadically with a different partner from the encoding one (Col–Col
diff group). This effect could be explicated through the retrieval disruption hypothesis (Basden et al.,
1997), whereby the participant's retrieval strategy is interrupted by the partner's output. The pattern of the ARC scores, an index of organization in recall, is consistent with this explanation. Lower scores were observed in the Col–Col
diff than in the other groups, which suggests that only when the partners in the study and the recall phases were different the retrieval strategy of a participant was disrupted by the new partner.
Interestingly, the retrieval disruption hypothesis would also predict that the effect of collaborative inhibition should be eliminated or attenuated when participants organize information in a similar way (i.e., encoding with the same partner). However, quite unexpectedly, the negative effect on recall was present even when the participants in the dyad encoded the information together (Col–Col
sam group). It could be argued that the participants in this group did not use the organization of the information that they collaboratively generated; however, the analysis of the ARC scores indicates that recall output was compatible with the way in which the information was categorized. Specifically, there were no differences in ARC scores between groups that recalled individually or dyadically with the same partner (Col–Col
sam and Col–Ind groups). These findings contrast the results of Harris et al., (
2013) who observed a collaborative inhibition elimination when the partners at study and at recall were the same. It is noteworthy, however, that Harris et al., (
2013) included an individual recall phase before the recall in the different conditions. Thus, participants had already had a first individual recall in which the interruption of retrieval did not occur; perhaps the information after this first recall could become more consolidated and less susceptible to the negative effects of collaboration during the second (collaborative) recall.
The fact that participants in groups that collaborated during retrieval showed lower recall performance than those in groups recalling individually, regardless of the degree of organization of the output, suggests the involvement of additional factors in the collaborative inhibition effect. This could be explained by retrieval blocking or inhibition, a phenomenon for which remembering causes forgetting of other information in memory (Bäuml,
2008). Retrieval blocking occurring in collaborative recall conditions is thought to inhibit the recall of unquoted words by suppressing their memory representations and making them unavailable for retrieval (Anderson et al.,
1994; Bäuml & Aslan,
2004). Moreover, in these conditions, part-set cuing, for which cuing is detrimental to memory performance, occurs due to blocking of retrieval, whereby cue words are believed to become stronger candidates for retrieval than noncue words; therefore, people in collaborative recall groups retrieve the reinforced cue words, blocking access to the unmentioned words (Rundus,
1973). It is also possible that an inhibitory mechanism as the social shared retrieval-induced forgetting plays a role (Coman & Hirst,
2015; Cuc et al.,
2007). When individuals focus on a partner's recall of previously learned information, they may forget related information that the speaker did not mention.
An alternative explanation may be linked to the social contagion of memory errors (Basden et al.,
2002; Davis & Meade,
2013; Huff et al.,
2013; McNabb & Meade,
2014; Meade & Roediger,
2002; Roediger & McDermott,
1995; Roediger et al.,
2001). For example, in collaborative encoding conditions, participants may incorporate their partner's erroneous suggestions into their own memories (Numbers et al.,
2014). In the present experiment, participants in the group that collaborated in both the encoding and the recall phases (Col–Col
sam) showed more intrusions in the first recall trial than the group that collaborated in the encoding phase but recalled individually (Col–Ind).It is possible that these participants incorporated each other’s errors during collaborative encoding and later, during collaborative recall, they were unable to correct each other, leading them to commit more errors.
Also in this case, as with collaboration during encoding, the DA hypothesis is applicable. Effectively the conditions where participants collaborate in retrieval may generate distraction and DA, unlike situations where recall is individual and thus FA is more feasible. DA is known to be deleterious to memory and secondary task performance, compared to FA conditions (Fernandes & Moscovitch,
2000; Naveh-Benjamin et al.,
1998).
To recapitulate, collaboration during encoding and retrieval may lead to different effects driven by separate mechanisms. Specifically, the encoding deficit appears to be a consequence of inadequate learning of the material, potentially attributed to the divided attention demands in collaborative learning situations. On the other hand, the reduction of the collaborative inhibition effect arises, at least partially, from the similarity of retrieval strategies. Nevertheless, it is worth noting that this may not be the sole mechanism involved in the collaborative inhibition effect, given the distinct patterns observed for nominal recall and ARC measures. Consequently, it remains plausible that, in addition to retrieval disruption, other mechanisms such as inhibition, blocking, error contagion, and even divided attention during retrieval may also contribute to this effect.
The emotional valence of stimuli did not affect any of the effects described. Thus, both the collaboration deficit and the collaborative inhibition effects do not seem to be modulated by the valence of the material. An unexpected result was that the most remembered words were neutral words in comparison to both positive and negative words which in turn did not differ from each other. In general, increased memory performance has been repeatedly demonstrated for emotional material (Fairfield et al.,
2013; Ferré et al.,
2015; Talmi et al.,
2013; Siddiqui & Unsworth,
2011; Zimmerman & Kelley,
2010), where negative or positive valanced stimuli are remembered better than neutral ones (Ke et al.,
2017). A post hoc explanation for the pattern of results observed in the present study is that neutral words could be more concrete than negative and positive words and therefore they were better remembered (Yui et al.,
2017).
Finally, the present study allowed us to confirm the generalizability of the results obtained with the new encoding task across two separate samples from different countries. Using the same paradigm with words from the same database adapted to two different languages (Bradley & Lang,
1999; Montefinese et al.,
2014; Redondo et al.,
2007), we found that memory performance was not affected by the different socio-cultural backgrounds of the different experimental samples. The study also confirmed the classic results of the collaborative memory paradigm such as collaborative encoding deficit, collaborative inhibition, and post-collaborative individual benefit.
The encoding task devised for the present study has proved to be useful for generating collaborative organization between participants. Nonetheless, the advantage of a shared organization may have been offset by the divided attention required to perform the task when collaborating with a partner. It could certainly be useful in the future to implement the encoding task in which the creation of a shared scheme of the encoded information is favored, but in a more controlled way, favoring focused attention. Regarding the emotional words used, certainly future studies should consider the concreteness of the words used as stimuli, to evaluate the emotive effect of the material.