Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

Open Access 07-12-2018

Cognitive and Emotional Profiles of CU Traits and Disruptive Behavior in Adolescence: a Prospective Study

Auteurs: Ann-Margret Rydell, Karin C. Brocki

Gepubliceerd in: Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology | Uitgave 6/2019

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

In this study we followed 82–90 adolescents, 50% boys, from 15 to 16 years, investigating CU traits and disruptive behaviors as predictors of cognitive skills and arousal to emotional pictures. At age 15, CU traits were rated by adolescents and disruptive (aggregated ADHD-ODD-delinquent) behaviors were rated by parents and adolescents. At age 16, executive function, reaction time variability (RTV), IQ and arousal to negative pictures were assessed. The results showed that, with control for disruptive behaviors, CU traits predicted lower RTV, higher IQ and lower arousal to negative pictures. With control for CU traits, disruptive behaviors predicted lower spatial working memory, lower interference control and higher RTV. Our findings are of theoretical and clinical relevance as they point to highly diverging cognitive and emotional profiles of CU traits and disruptive behaviors.
Opmerkingen

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Within the area of child and adolescent externalizing behavior problems, callous-unemotional (CU) traits, primarily characterized by low levels of guilt and empathy and lack of responsibility for own behavior (Barry et al. 2000; Frick et al. 2000, 2005) has been in focus during the last decades. CU traits have mostly been identified in children and adolescents with conduct disorder (CD; Frick et al. 2005; Hawes et al. 2014) and were included as a specifier of CD in DSM V (APA 2013). CU traits have also been associated with oppositional/defiant (ODD) and hyperactive/inattentive (ADHD) behavior and are thus related to disruptive behaviors more generally, in both children (Barry et al. 2000) and adolescents (Graziano et al. 2017; Herpers et al. 2012).The joint study of different aspects of disruptive behaviour and CU traits is therefore a relevant issue throughout development and particularly in adolescence, which is the focus of the current study.
Importantly, CU traits and disruptive behaviors are related but separate phenomena as shown by the fact that a limited portion of disruptive children have high levels of CU traits (Barry et al. 2000). There are also children and adolescents high in CU traits without disruptive behaviors (Frick et al. 2003; Herpers et al. 2012; Rowe et al. 2010). As verification of the distinction of disruptive behaviors and CU traits, three separate statistical factors for CU traits, antisocial behaviors and hyperactivity emerged in a community study of elementary school children (Dadds et al. 2005). Further, it has repeatedly been demonstrated that children with conduct disorder and other disruptive behaviors who also exhibit high levels of CU traits, are at heightened risk for persistent, severe antisocial behavior (Frick et al. 2003, 2005; Loeber et al. 2002; Pardini et al. 2006). As demonstrated in a recent review (Frick et al. 2014), this pattern of results survived control for the severity of conduct problems. Finally, treatment response may differ depending on the presence or absence of CU traits. A review covering studies of children and adolescents found evidence for unique associations between CU traits and poor treatment outcomes for conduct problems (Hawes et al. 2014). There are also indications that CU traits and conduct problems are uniquely associated with different treatment outcomes (Haas et al. 2011). For example, it has been shown that in addition to psychological treatment, adolescents with CU traits require medication in order to improve behaviorally (Waschbuch et al. 2007).
Thus, investigating distinguishing characteristics of CU traits and disruptive behavior seems highly important in order to increase knowledge about the possibly different backgrounds to two troublesome aspects of child- and adolescent problems. The present prospective study examined predictive relations from CU traits and disruptive behaviors to cognitive processing skills, such as executive functioning and IQ, and emotional functioning in terms of arousal in response to negative stimuli, in adolescence. We chose to focus on this age group for two important reasons: 1) this active period of transition from childhood to adulthood is particularly relevant for cognitive and emotional functioning and the links to CU traits and disruptive behaviors 2) this age group has been empirically overlooked in terms of the distinctiveness of cognitive and emotional profiles related to CU traits and disruptive disorders, respectively.

Relations between CU Traits, Disruptive Behaviors and Cognitive Functioning

One important aspect regarding the distinctiveness between CU traits and disruptive behaviors regards profiles of impaired cognitive processing, which have been related to disruptive behaviors rather than to CU traits among children (Waller et al. 2015). In contrast, it has been observed that behavior characterizing individuals with high levels of CU traits, such as successfully charming and conning others, would require good intellectual capacities (Salekin et al. 2004). Indeed, there is a body of research to support a link between adequate social cognition and CU traits. For example, as summarized in the review concerning children and adolescents by Frick et al. (2014), youngsters with disruptive behaviors and high levels of CU traits seem unbothered by others’ plight but do not show deficits in cognitive empathy compared with disruptive youngster with low CU levels, i.e., they seem to understand how others feel and think in social situations. Regarding non-social cognitive functioning, the picture is less clear.
Executive Functioning
Fundamental separate, but correlated, aspects of executive dysfunction are low inhibitory control, i.e., inhibition of pre-potent responses and the more complex function of interference control, i.e., overriding the tendency to produce a more dominant response, as well as problems in updating and monitoring of working memory (Miyake et al. 2000). Another important, but non-executive cognitive process, is reaction time variability (RTV), particularly as demonstrated in repetitious tasks. Importantly, the literature on the relation between CU traits and executive functioning is very scarce and particularly so in adolescent samples. However, there are a few studies available on the topic in preschool and school-aged children. For example, Bohlin and colleagues (Bohlin et al. 2012) found that when controlling for externalizing symptoms, longitudinal relations between poor inhibitory control and CU traits from five to seven years disappeared. Further, a longitudinal study by Fanti et al. (2017) showed that school-aged children high in CU traits had poorer executive functioning two years later compared to children low in CU traits, with no control for disruptive behaviors. In another longitudinal study Wall et al. (2016) found that school–aged children high in CU traits, but without conduct problems had better executive functioning one year later compared to children high in both types of problems. In the latter two studies, executive dysfunction was measured using parental ratings, and as parents reported also on CU traits and disruptive behaviors, method variance such as reporter bias, may partly explain the associations. There are two studies available on the association between CU traits and interference control as measured by the color word Stroop-test. In the study by Fanti et al. (2016), no longitudinal association was found between CU traits and interference control across one year when controlling for conduct disorder and ADHD. However, testifying to the importance of executive skills, one recent study identified an interaction effect demonstrating that adolescents high in CU traits and conduct problems and who also had adequate interference control reported the highest number of violent acts (Baskin-Sommers et al. 2015). In sum, knowledge about executive function in relation to CU traits is scarce and needs further empirical attention, especially in the teenage years.
In comparison to CU traits, executive dysfunction in youth with disruptive behavior problems has been extensively investigated. Poor executive functioning and high RTV in ADHD have been demonstrated for decades in samples of various ages (Kofler et al. 2013; Willcutt et al. 2005). Although not as consistently found, deficits in executive functioning and high RTV levels have also been related to ODD and CD and general externalizing problems in studies covering preschool to adolescence (Bohlin et al. 2012; Fanti et al. 2016; Scholtens et al. 2012; Wall et al. 2016; Wiggs et al. 2016; see Sergeant et al. 2002, for a review).
IQ
There has been a widely held view, perhaps spurred by early writings on psychopathy, that high levels of psychopathic traits are associated with high intelligence (for a review, see Allen et al. 2013). However, as regards the CU dimension, research points to levels of average intelligence. There have also been reports of non- significant associations between full-scale, non-verbal and verbal IQ estimates and CU traits in detained/delinquent and typical samples of adolescents (Pardini 2011; Salekin et al. 2004) and children (Fanti et al. 2016; Jezior et al. 2016).
Normal IQ has also been found in relation to CU traits when controlling for conduct disorder in adolescence (Loney et al. 2006). Moreover, in a comprehensive enquiry into this issue, Allen et al. (2013) found no associations between verbal or non-verbal intelligence and CU traits when controlling for hyperactivity and antisocial behavior in a large sample of various ages, including adolescents. Without these controls, CU traits were associated with lower IQ. However, in one study, IQ and CU traits interacted, as detained adolescents high in CU traits and in verbal ability reported most violent delinquency (Munoz et al. 2008). Thus, as in the case of executive functioning, there are some empirical indications that IQ is important to consider in relation to CU traits, but the association is complex and needs further investigation, controlling for co-occurring disruptive behavior. Importantly, in the above reported studies one or two aspects of disruptive behavior were accounted for, but a broader and simultaneous take on disruptive behavior is necessary to clarify the picture of associations between IQ and CU traits. As regards IQ and disruptive behaviors, the picture is quite coherent. ADHD has been associated with lower levels of IQ in children (Langley et al. 2010) and adults (Bridgett and Walker 2006). Deficits in IQ have also been linked to other disruptive problems such as ODD, CD and delinquency in adolescence (Keyes et al. 2017; Murray and Farrington 2010). However, it should be noted, that the IQ – conduct disorder relation is not as clear cut as is the one between IQ and ADHD (see Burke et al. 2002 for a review).

Relations between CU Traits, Disruptive Behaviors, and Emotional Arousal

A phenomenon of interest when separating CU traits and disruptive behavior is how individuals react to aversive stimuli. It has been put forward that an explanation to the low anxiety and fear associated with CU traits (Kahn et al. 2016; Roose et al. 2011) may originate in low reactivity to aversive stimuli (see Herpers et al. 2014 for a review). Specifically, slow and reduced reactions to distress pictures, negative words and violent films, and poor recognition and low recall of distress emotions and emotional stimuli have been associated with CU traits in samples of mixed ages (Blair and Coles 2000), young adults (Fanti et al. 2015) and adolescents (Dolan and Fullam 2010; Keyes et al. 2017; Loney et al. 2003; Pihet et al. 2015).
As regards disruptive behaviors, normative or even high arousal to negative stimuli has been indicated. Associations to low autonomic arousal in response to empathy-eliciting film clips have been mainly driven by CU traits, and disruptive behavior disordered and non-disordered youth did not differ in self-reported arousal in response to negative pictures, controlling for CU traits in mixed and adolescent samples (de Wied et al. 2012; Masi et al. 2014). Furthermore, when taking CU traits into account, disruptive problems have been associated with higher reactivity to negative words, and with higher startle response and higher rated fear in response to violent films in young adult and adolescent samples (Kyranides et al. 2016; Loney et al. 2003). There are reports of autonomic physiological arousal in relation to ADHD in child samples. Generally, the picture is one of elevated levels of sympathetic or parasympathetic arousal, assessed in response to various laboratory tasks, relative to controls (Griffiths et al. 2017; Musser et al. 2011; Ward et al. 2015). Also self-or adult ratings of elevated negative arousal have been found in ADHD (for a review, see Graziano and Garcia 2016). The potential importance of arousal has been demonstrated as low sympathetic arousal has been found in relation to poor treatment response among preschoolers with ADHD (Beauchaine et al. 2013). However, reduced autonomic arousal has also been found in individuals with ADHD (Conzelmann et al. 2014). The indication of heterogeneity in the ADHD dimension may be related to CU traits, which were not assessed in the above studies. In line with this reasoning are findings by Musser and colleagues (Musser et al. 2013) suggesting that children with ADHD categorized as low in pro-sociality, with low pro-sociality treated as a proxy for CU traits, had low levels of autonomic arousal, whereas ADHD children categorized normative in pro-sociality had high autonomic arousal. Thus, low versus high/normative arousal in response to negative stimuli may distinguish CU traits from disruptive behaviors, which may well impact a variety of important social and behavioral outcomes and therefore should be further investigated.
In sum, prior research points to distinct cognitive profiles for CU traits and disruptive behaviors. However, the relation between cognitive functioning and CU-traits, particularly executive functioning, is understudied and the few studies that exist have looked at the occasional EF-test, rather than a battery of tests to clarify possible relations to separate EF-components. For example, differential associations with simple versus complex inhibition may enhance our understanding of CU traits. In addition, disinhibition and low working memory as well as high RTV are particularly relevant to explore in relation to CU traits. These aspects are fairly well researched in relation to disruptive behavior, allowing for comparisons of cognitive correlates of CU traits and disruptive behavior. On the other hand, arousal to emotional stimuli is well documented in relation to CU traits but little investigated in relation to disruptive behaviors, again affording fruitful comparisons. Further, while there are studies on adolescence relating cognitive dysfunction to disruptive behaviors, a gap of knowledge exists when it comes to the association between EF and CU traits in this particular age group, an age in which CU traits and disruptive behavior are particularly linked to unfavorable outcomes. Finally, and perhaps most importantly, there is a shortage of longitudinal studies simultaneously investigating CU traits and broad aspects of associated disruptive disorders in relation to these cognitive and emotional factors, to our knowledge none in adolescence. Longitudinal studies are necessary to assess the robustness of associations between cognitive and emotional factors and these common child/adolescent problems. Providing answers to the above outlined questions should therefore have strong clinical as well as theoretical value.

The Present Study

In the present prospective study, we aimed to extend the understanding of CU traits and disruptive behavior (an aggregate of ADHD-ODD-and delinquent behavior) in adolescence by asking the question whether distinctive profiles of cognitive functioning and arousal in response to negative stimuli are predicted by the two when controlling for their co-variation. Given the associations between CU traits and all three aspects of problem behavior mentioned above and current conceptualizations of child and adolescent disorders (Drabick et al. 2015; Golmaryami and Frick 2015), we focused on disruptive behaviour broadly rather than on specific aspects. Based on theoretical formulations and prior research, we expected that CU-traits at 15 years would primarily predict low arousal to negative stimuli whereas disruptive behavior would predict cognitive problems (i.e., disinhibition, low working memory, high RTV and low IQ level) at 16 years. Finally, in supplementary analyses, we investigated whether using two separate aspects of disruptive behavior as predictors, ADHD symptoms and antisocial/oppositional behaviors, respectively, would render a similar pattern of associations to cognitive and emotional functioning, controlling for CU traits, as that found in the main analyses.

Method

Participants and Procedure

Participants were recruited from an ongoing longitudinal project focusing on disruptive behavior problems. Ninety adolescents, 50% boys, living in and around a major university town in Sweden were studied at ages 15 and 16. The majority, 66% lived with both biological parents and for 79% both parents were born in Sweden. Of the 34 immigrated parents (15 mothers, 19 fathers), 12 were born in Europe, 12 were born outside Europe (Asia, the Middle East, Northern Africa, the USA and Australia) and the remainder had not supplied information about their country of birth. Four adolescents were born abroad. One was adopted as an infant by two Swedish-born parents and the others had immigrated with their families between ages five to ten years, one from Europe and two from non-European countries. Based on reports of highest level of schooling (the 9-year compulsory school or less, 12 years of schooling including high school, post-high school education), parental education was found high, as 68% of mothers and 58% of fathers held a college or university degree.
The recruitment process was as follows: Parents of 1206 ten-year old children recruited from a population based register responded to a mailed questionnaire, including ratings of child ODD- and ADHD-symptoms (for a description of the sample, see Rydell 2010). At adolescent age 15, both parents and adolescents in 602 families in which parents at the first data wave had agreed to be contacted again responded to a web-based questionnaire. In the questionnaire, parents and adolescents had indicated that they could be approached for follow-ups, which was a requirement for inclusion in the study. This sample did not differ in the levels of ODD- and ADHD symptoms at age 10 from those who did not participate, p > 10. At age 15, parents rated the adolescent’s ADHD- and ODD symptoms and CU traits. Adolescent questionnaires contained background information and ratings of CU traits, ADHD symptoms, and delinquency. A security server was used and the questionnaires contained no identifying information. Two reminders were sent out and each respondent received a movie ticket worth five Euros as compensation for his/her participation. After data collection, the questionnaires were transferred to a project computer and each respondent’s personal project number was added, after which all information was erased from the server.
A year later, at adolescent age 16 years, the parent ratings of ODD- and ADHD symptoms were used to select a subsample for a laboratory study of cognitive and emotional functioning, as funding could support the costly lab procedure for only part of the sample. To ensure a variation in disruptive symptoms, equal numbers of adolescents, balanced for sex, were recruited from those with ODD- and ADHD symptom ratings above and below the median. Given the relations between disruptive behaviors and CU traits (Golmaryami and Frick 2015), we expected to achieve variation in CU traits as well. Constituting a pool of prospective participants, one hundred and ninety adolescents received a letter inviting them to visit the department lab, stressing the voluntary nature of participation. The invitation letters were sent in batches of twenty to secure that each adolescent could be contacted by phone within two weeks by a research assistant, who followed a standardized manual for the conversation. If the adolescent accepted to participate, an appointment was scheduled. Ninety-one adolescents participated in the lab procedure, but one person chose to stop halfway through the session. Twenty-one adolescents could not be reached and the remainder declined participation, most of them claiming lack of time. The 90 participants did not differ from the rest of the sample at age 15 in the level of ADHD- or ODD symptoms, delinquency, CU traits, or gender, p > 0.10. The two hour procedure included tests of executive functions, RTV, IQ and arousal in response to emotional pictures. One graduate - and two undergraduate psychology students administered the tests individually to each adolescent. The students had been trained in the manualized procedure and had performed and been evaluated on three-to four test trials before the commencement of data collection. The procedure included nine cognitive and emotional arousal tests and was run according to two alternative orders, composed with the goal that tests should not appear in the same temporal position in the two orders. The tests used in the present study were positioned as no 3/8, 6/4, 7/3, 8/2, 9/1 in the two versions of the procedure. Using a computer random number generator, the tester randomized which order for each participant. There were no differences in the group mean of any test depending on order, p > 0.15. Cognitive tasks and tasks tapping arousal to emotional pictures were presented on a computer screen. The computerized tasks were programmed using the E-Prime version 2.0 and were performed on a HP laptop with a 15, 6′ screen and a 1366 × 768 pixel resolution. Due to technical failure, data on arousal to emotional pictures, disinhibition and spatial working memory was missing for two to four participants, who were omitted from analyses on these measures. The adolescents received a gift voucher worth approximately 50 Euros as an appreciation of their participation.

Measures1

Parent Ratings at Ages 10, 15, and Adolescent Ratings at Age 15

All scale scores were computed as the mean of items.
CU Traits
Parent reports of CU traits at age 15 were used to validate adolescent self-reported CU traits. Parents rated 12 items reflecting CU traits from the YPI self-report (see below), e.g., “Does not express guilt when he/she has done something not allowed”, “Seems completely calm when others are upset”, “Is not bothered if others come to harm”, α = 0.88. Adolescent CU traits were rated using items from the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory (YPI; Andershed et al. 2002). The YPI has demonstrated adequate psychometric properties and is extensively used in research (Andershed et al. 2002, 2007). The CU traits scale comprised the average of three 5-item subscales measuring remorselessness, un-emotionality and callousness (e.g., “I have the ability not to feel guilt and regret about things I think other people would feel guilty about”, “To be nervous and worried is a sign of weakness”, “When other people have problems, it is often their own fault, therefore, one should not help them”. A 4-step response format was used, from 1 = Does not apply at all to 4 = Applies very well, α = 0.80. Adolescent self-reports were related to parent ratings of CU traits, pr = 0.37, p < 0.01, controlling for gender.
Disruptive Behaviors
Parents and adolescents rated adolescent’s ODD- and ADHD symptoms from DSM–IV (APA 2000) using two widely used scales, the 8-item scale for ODD symptoms (Bussing et al. 2008; α = 0.88 and 0.90 at ages 10 and 15, respectivley) and the 18-item ADHD scale (DuPaul et al. 1998; α = 0.94 and 0.94). Items had a 4-step response format, from 1 = Does not apply at all to 4 = Applies very well to the child. Ratings at age 10 were used to assess possible differences in distributions between the original sample and the sample of 602 at age 15. Adolescents rated ADHD-symptoms from DSM-IV with 18 items (Kessler et al. 2005). Items had a 5-step response format, 1 = Never happens to me to 5 = Happens to me very often, α = 0.87. Delinquency was measured with 14 well-used self-report items (Kerr et al. 2012) and validated among Swedish youth through police reports (Magnusson et al. 1975). Adolescents answered questions about the extent to which they had performed various acts during the last year, e.g., vandalizing property, taking money at home, breaking into someone’s house, stolen money, bikes or cars, street fighting, carrying a weapon, threatening another person, forcing another person, coded 0 = Never happened,= Happened once,= Happened 2–3 times,= Happened 4–10 times and 4 = Happened more than 10 times, α = 72. To compose a measure of disruptive behavior, parent and adolescent ratings of ADHD symptoms were aggregated, a = 0.64, after which the standardized ADHD symptom scores were averaged with standardized parent rated ODD-scores and standardized adolescent self-reports of delinquency. Correlations between the three components were r = 0.43 to r = 0.56, p < 01, α = 0.74.
Cognitive Functioning and Arousal to Emotional Stimuli at 16 Years
Throughout, standardized instructions were given according to a manual. Before the session began, the tester welcomed the adolescent and informed that the visit would include computerized tasks dealing with memory, thinking and attention. The adolescent was reminded of his/her right to stop participation at any time without explanation.
Executive Functioning
To assess spatial working memory, beach balls of different sizes, ranging from 0, 4–2, 4 cm in diameter, were presented on a circular yellow frame (2.5 cm in diameter) on a red background (Forssman et al. 2010). Validity has been established in relation to short-term memory, verbal working memory and IQ (Tillman et al. 2008). Participants were informed that they would be presented with balls of different sizes, one at a time, at different locations on the screen. This was illustrated with a print out of a screen with two balls. After a series of balls had been shown, the screen would turn blue, and the task was to indicate with the computer mouse the location on the screen of each ball in order of size, starting with the smallest. There would be more balls as the test went on, making it more difficult. Each presentation lasted 1 millisecond with blank intervals of 1 millisecond between presentations. All responses within a radius of 14 mm from the perimeter of the yellow circle were considered hits. The test had four levels with three trials per level, i.e. a total of 12 trials. The first level had two beach balls per trial and for each level one ball was added, giving five locations to recall at level four. For each consecutive pair of locations remembered in the correct order, one score was given. Therefore, a trial with two balls could give a score of one and a trial with five balls could give a score of 4. The score total was calculated as the sum of scores across trials, with a theoretical minimum of 0 and a theoretical maximum of 30. Split-half reliability using the Spearman-Brown formula was 0.77.
Response Disinhibition
A computerized go/no-go task was used to measure response disinhibition (Forssman et al. 2010). The go/no-go task has been used extensively in research and has repeatedly shown associations to ADHD- and other disruptive symptoms (Brocki et al. 2007; Scholtens et al. 2012). In this task, participants viewed five different geometrical figures appearing at the center of a computer screen. Participants were told that they would see figures on the screen, one at a time, and were instructed how to respond to figures by pressing a button except when the figure was a triangle. The tester ascertained that the adolescent had understood when to press and when not to press the button, informed that the task would take some time, and that the adolescent should try to press the button as quickly as possible, without making mistakes. The figures appeared for 750 ms (or until the button was pressed) with an inter stimulus interval of 1000 ms. Preceded by 15 practice trials, the task consisted of 150 trials, 26% (n = 39) of which were no-go trials. We used the number of commission errors (i.e., responding to no-go trials) as a measure of response disinhibition. Split-half reliability was r = 0.70, using the Spearman-Brown formula. RTV was measured as the standard deviation of reaction time to hits on the go stimuli from this task, with a split-half reliability coefficient of 0.73.
Interference Control
A more complex aspect of inhibition, interference control, was assessed with a version of the Antisaccade task (Carr et al. 2010). Poor performance on antisaccadic tasks has been related to ADHD-symptoms in samples of various ages (Carr et al. 2010; Soncin et al. 2016). The task involved a fixator, a cue and a target stimulus. The fixator was a small black square appearing on a white background in the middle of the computer screen for 1500–3500 ms (a loop with a consecutive increase of 250 ms). The cue was a small black arrow appearing for 225 ms to the left or to the right of the fixator. 50 ms after the disappearance of the cue, the target stimulus (an identical arrow) was presented for 150 ms on the opposite side, after which the cue was covered by a checkered mask for 3000 ms or until a button was pressed. The procedure was explained and demonstrated in detail. Participants were instructed to focus on the fixator and when seeing a flash (the cue) to either the left or the right, to look toward the opposite side of the screen, where the target stimulus would appear. They were asked to indicate in which direction the target arrow was pointing (either to the left, to the right or upward) by pressing one of three buttons marked with corresponding arrows. The tester informed that the stimuli would present in rapid succession, and again stressed that it was the direction of the arrow, not the side of the screen on which it was presented, that should be indicated, and that the main thing was to try to press the right button, and not to speed through the task. The session started with twelve practice trials, and the task had two blocks of 42 trials, with a short break between blocks. A score of one was given for each correct response and summed to produce a total score, with a maximum of 82. Split-half reliability was 0.92.
IQ was measured by Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices, a reliable measure of non-verbal fluid intelligence (Raven et al. 2000). The test has five sections (A-E) with 12 problems each, and has no time limit. Standard instructions from the manual were used. Problems 1 and 6 from Set A were used as training items. The score was computed as the sum of correctly solved problems (maximum 12 in each set) from sets B to E, the split-half reliability coefficient being 0.76.
Arousal in Response to Emotional Stimuli
Emotional responding was measured with 50 pictures from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang et al. 2005) viewed on a computer screen. The selection of pictures was guided by norms from the manual, in order to compose a set with variation in arousal (Lang et al. 2005). Twenty-three pictures were emotionally neutral (e.g., coffee mug, a neutral or happy face) and 27 pictures were aversive (e.g., close-ups of a pointed gun or a crying child). Participants were told that we were interested to learn how people react to depictions of incidents and objects one might encounter in real life or on TV or other media. Each picture was displayed for 6000 ms, and, after a 500 ms pause, the participant was asked to indicate on a keyboard, by pressing the appropriate key, the level of arousal while viewing the picture, 1 = completely calm to 9 = highly aroused/on edge. There was no preset response time, but participants were instructed not to think too long and to stick with the choice that first came to mind. There was a 3000 ms pause between the response slide and the next picture. The order of the stimuli pictures was the same for each participant. Each session began with three practice trials. Based on factor analyses, three arousal scores were composed as the mean of items: one for emotionally neutral pictures e.g., neutral face, dancing women, hands cupping a deck of cards; 21 pictures; one for threatening pictures, e.g., pointed gun, soldiers fighting, hooded men led by soldiers, man strangling a girl; 14 pictures, and one for distress pictures, e.g., close-up of crying child, old woman in bed with old man beside her with face turned away, a couple by a tombstone; 8 pictures, α = 0.91–0.88. For the sample as a whole, arousal was higher in response to threatening and distress pictures, respectively, compared to neutral pictures, t = 17.3 and 12.9, p < 0.01. Self-reports of arousal have been positively related to physiological indices of arousal (Hetzel-Riggin and Wilber 2010). Response time in ms to each picture was recorded in order to validate self-reports of arousal. A difference score was computed as the mean response time to neutral pictures minus the response time to emotional (threat and distress pictures collapsed), M = −71.20 (SD = 370.40), difference scores ranging from −973.26 to 1015.64. Positive values indicate longer response times to neutral than to emotional pictures, indicating being less alerted by them than by the emotional pictures. There was a negative relationship between the difference score and the level of arousal to emotional pictures, r = −0.34, p < 0.05, i.e., quicker responding to emotional than to neural pictures was associated with higher ratings of arousal.

Results

As seen in Table 1, there was variation in all variables. Boys were higher than girls in CU traits, t(88) = 3. 34, p < 0.01 and marginally higher in interference control, t(86) = 1.72, p < 0.10, but lower than girls in arousal in response to distress and threat pictures, t(86 = 2.25, and t(86) = 2.54, p < 0.05. Parental education and family composition were unrelated to disruptive behavior and CU traits, t(88) = 0.19–1.63, p > 0.10. Adolescents with one or two foreign born parents had higher levels of disruptive behavior than adolescents with two Swedish born parents, t(83) = 2.24, p < 0.05, but this variable was unrelated to all other measures, t(79–83) = 0.21–1.63, p > 0.10. Analyses involving CU traits, interference control and arousal to distress and threat pictures were controlled for gender.
Table 1
Distribution for all variables (N = 82–90)
Variables
M (SD)
Min
Max
Age 15
 CU traits
1.83(.43)
1.07
3.00
 Disruptive behaviors
0.00(.81)
−1.14
2.21
Age 16
 Cognitive functioning
  Spatial working memory
17.42(5.47)
2.00
29.00
  Response disinhibition
8.17(4.52)
2.00
29.00
  Interference control
71.58(9.79)
37.00
84.00
  RTV
87.85(24.67)
50.15
169.63
  IQ
38.28(5.74)
19.00
48.00
 Arousal to emotional pictures
  Distress pictures
5.37(1.67)
1.00
8.88
  Threat pictures
6.22(1.77)
1.00
8.79
  Neutral pictures
3.22(1.18)
1.00
5.52
Controlling for gender, CU traits were correlated with disruptive behavior, pr = 0.48, p < 0.01, and with all three components of disruptive behavior (i.e., ADHD- and ODD symptoms and delinquency), pr = 0.32–0.44, p < 01.There were correlations between the cognitive variables, see Table 2. Interference control was positively correlated with IQ and negatively with RTV. IQ was negatively correlated with RTV. Spatial working memory was negatively correlated with RTV. The two arousal variables were highly correlated but unrelated to cognitive skills, with the exception of spatial working memory, with which there were negative correlations, especially with arousal to distress pictures.
Table 2
Correlations between outcome variables at age 16 (N = 86–90)
 
SpatialWMa
Response disinhibition
IFCb
RTV
IQ
Arousal to distress pictures
Response disinhibition
−0.01
     
IFCb
0.21+
−0.18+
    
RTV
−0.30**
0.15
0.54**
   
IQ
0.12
0.09
0.49**
−0.38**
  
Arousal to distress pictures
−0.26*
0.11
−0.08
0.04
−0.03
 
Arousal to threat pictures
−0.19+
−0.12
−0.09
−0.02
0.00
0.87**
aWorking memory, bInterference control, + = p < 0.10, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01
In Table 3, correlations between CU traits and disruptive behaviors and cognitive variables and arousal to emotional pictures a year later, respectively, are depicted. CU traits did not correlate with any cognitive measure. High levels of disruptive behavior were negatively correlated with spatial working memory scores, and positively with response disinhibition and RTV. CU traits were negatively correlated with self-reported arousal in response to distress and threat pictures, while disruptive behaviors did not correlate with arousal in response to emotional pictures. Attesting to the validity of the self-reported arousal ratings, arousal in response to neutral pictures were not correlated with neither CU traits nor disruptive behavior.
Table 3
Correlations between CU traits and disruptive behaviors (DB) at Age 15 and cognitive functioning and arousal to negative and neutral pictures at age 16 (N = 86–90)
 
Cognitive functioning
Arousal to negative and neutral pictures
Spatial WMa
Response Disinhibition
IFCb
RTV
IQ
Arousal to distress Pictures
Arousal to threat pictures
Arousal to neutral pictures
CU traits
−0.07
0.15
0.09c
−0.10
−0.14
−0.26*c
−0.22*c
−0.02c
DB behaviors
−0.22*
0.23*
0.15
0.29**
0.11
0.11
0.13
0.06
aWorking memory, b Interference control,c with control for gender + = p < 0.10, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01
To respond to our main question, we further investigated the above associations in a series of regressions analyses, with CU traits and disruptive behavior as predictors and each of the cognitive variables and arousal to emotional pictures as outcomes, see Table 4. With control for disruptive behavior, CU traits predicted higher IQ and lower RTV. CU traits also marginally predicted higher interference control. With control for CU traits, bivariate associations with lower spatial working memory scores and higher RTV were confirmed for disruptive behaviors, and there was a marginally significant prediction of higher disinhibition. High levels of disruptive behavior also predicted lower interference control and, marginally, lower IQ. The regressions confirmed bivariate associations between CU traits and low self-reported arousal in response to, especially, distress pictures. The prediction of lower response to threat pictures was only marginally significant. Neither with control for CU traits did disruptive behaviors predict arousal to emotional pictures. Thus, the regressions further stressed distinctive patterns of associations between CU traits and disruptive behavior, respectively, and cognitive variables and self-reported arousal to emotional pictures.
Table 4
Regression Analyses on Cognitive Functioning and Arousal to Negative Pictures at age 16 with CU Traits and Disruptive Behavior (DB) at Age 15 as Predictors (N = 86–90)
  
Cognitive functioning
Arousal to negative pictures
Spatial WMa
Response disinhibition
IFCb
RTV
IQ
Arousal to distress pictures
Arousal to threat pictures
Predictors
 Model
Adj R2
0.04+
0.03+
0.06*
0.14**
0.04+
0.09* c
0.08* c
 CU traits
β
0.04
0.05
0.22+
−0.30**
0.25*
−0.27*
−0.21+
 DB behaviors
β
−0.27**
0.21+
−0.26*
0.44**
0.22+
0.02
0.02
a Working memory, b Interference control,c with control for gender,+ = p < 0.10, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01

Supplementary Analyses

Finally, to investigate how different types of disruptive behaviors were associated with the outcome measures, with control for CU traits, we separated the aggregated disruptive behavior measure into two domains; ADHD symptoms and aggregated ODD and delinquent behavior (antisocial/oppositional, AO behavior, α = 0.63). The motivation for this split was that ADHD symptoms have solid theoretical and empirical connections with executive dysfunction, whereas delinquent behaviors and ODD symptoms express general externalizing problem behaviors and are less consistently associated with executive dysfunction (Bohlin et al. 2012; Fanti et al. 2016; Scholtens et al. 2012; Wall et al. 2016). Both measures were based on parent and self-reports (see Methods), giving them equal representation of data sources. By so doing, we tested the validity of using an aggregated disruptive behavior measure in this context. As seen in Table 5, the correlations were similar to those presented in Table 3. Both ADHD symptoms and AO behaviors were positively correlated with response disinhibition, although the latter was only marginally significant. Further, both domains were positively correlated with RTV, ADHD symptoms marginally so. AO behaviors were marginally negatively correlated with spatial working memory. As in analyses on the aggregated disruptive measure, neither ADHD symptoms nor AO behaviors were related to interference control or to self-reported arousal to negative pictures.
Table 5
Correlations between ADHD symptoms and AO Behaviors at age 15 and cognitive functioning and arousal to negative pictures at age 16 (N = 86–99)
 
Cognitive functioning
Arousal to negative pictures
Spatial WMa
Response disinhibition
IFCb
RTV
IQ
Arousal to distress pictures
Arousal to threat pictures
ADHD symptoms
−0.18
0.23*
−0.10
0.22+
−0.04
−0.09
−0.04
AO behaviorsc
−0.20+
0.19+
−0.15
0.29**
−0.13
−0.11
−0.16
aWorking memory; bInterference control,c antsocial/oppositional,+ = p < 0.10, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01
Next, we performed two sets of regression analyses; one with ADHD symptoms and CU traits as the predictors and one with AO behaviors and CU traits as predictors. This division was made to avoid that the common variance between ADHD symptoms and AO behaviors (r = 0.58, p < 0.01) should balance out the effect of each when used as predictor together with CU traits. Again, the pattern of results agrees with the findings from the main analyses in important respects, see Table 6. In none of these regressions did CU traits predict spatial working memory or response disinhibition, while ADHD symptoms predicted marginally higher levels of response disinhibition and AO behaviors predicted marginally lower spatial WM. Further, in correspondence with the main analyses, AO behaviors predicted lower interference control, higher RTV and lower IQ. ADHD symptoms predicted higher RTV. In these analyses, CU traits were associated with lower RTV and with higher IQ in the regression with AO behaviors. Again, as in the main analyses, none of the ADHD symptom or AO behavior variables predicted self-reported arousal to negative pictures while CU traits independently and significantly predicted lower arousal to distress pictures in both regressions, as well as lower arousal to threat pictures, controlling for ADHD symptoms. In sum, the results when using separate aspects of disruptive behaviors were similar to the main results using a broad measure of disruptive behavior, although the coefficients tended to be lower and less often reached significance. To be more specific, disruptive behavior spectrum aspects were associated with poorer cognitive skills, whereas CU traits were primarily associated with low self-reported arousal when confronted with, especially, depictions of others’ distress, and with higher cognitive skills. These analyses again stressed the importance of control for each other when investigating associated features of disruptive behaviors and CU traits.
Table 6
Regression Analyses on Cognitive Functioning and Arousal to Negative Pictures at age 16 with CU Traits and ADHD Symptoms (Model 1) and CU traits and AO Behaviors (Model2) at Age 15 as Predictors (N = 86–90)
  
Cognitive functioning
Arousal to negative pictures
Spatial WMa
Response disinhibition
IFCbd
RTV
IQ
Arousal to distress picturesd
Arousal to threat picturesd
Predictors
Model 1
Adj R 2
0.01
0.04+
0.03
0.07*
0.01
0.09*
0.09*
 CU traits
β
0.00
0.06
0.17
−0.23*
0.19
−0.28*
−0.26*
 ADHD symptoms
β
−0.18
0.21+
−0.19
0.32**
−0.11
0.04
0.09*
Model 2
Adj R 2
0.02
0.02
0.05
0.12**
0.04+
0.09*
0.09*
 CU traits
β
0.02
0.08
0.20
−0.27*
0.24*
−0.27*
−0.18
 AO behaviorsc
β
−0.21+
0.16
−0.24*
0.40*
−0.23*
0.00
−0.08
aWorking Memory, bInterference control,c antisocial/oppositional behaviors, d with control for gender+ = p < 0.10, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01

Discussion

In this prospective study, we demonstrated that CU traits and disruptive behaviors predicted distinct profiles of cognitive functioning and arousal to emotional pictures across one year in adolescence. As hypothesized, CU traits were longitudinally associated with low arousal in response to emotional pictures. As also predicted, and with control for CU traits, disruptive behaviors were longitudinally associated with lower cognitive functioning and higher RTV. In addition, CU traits predicted higher cognitive functioning on most measures as well as higher IQ levels, which formed an open question in the current study.

Cognitive Functioning, CU Traits and Disruptive Behavior

The present study afforded some new insights regarding CU traits and cognition. A few previous studies have investigated CU traits in relation to executive functioning, finding no associations when controlling for conduct problems (Bohlin et al. 2012; Fanti et al. 2016). Using a battery of executive function tests we found positive predictive associations between CU traits and the more complex aspect of inhibitory control referred to as interference control, and associations between CU traits and lower RTV. In line with Bohlin et al. (2012) we found no associations to simple response disinhibition. Interestingly, these findings indicate that staying in focus and withstanding distractions may characterize individuals with higher CU levels, rather than managing to keep up a steady stream of accurate simple responses. Possibly, these skills may be instrumental in successful manipulation and conning practices, more so than skills in simple disinhibition.
Second, when accounting for the overlap with disruptive behaviors, CU traits predicted higher IQ. This finding is at odds with previous studies, which did not find associations between CU traits and full- scale verbal or non-verbal IQ, when controlling for disruptive disorders (Allen et al. 2013; Loney et al. 2006). The discordant findings may be explained by measurement issues. We used a non-verbal IQ measure, whereas the studies referred to above used aggregated measures of verbal and non-verbal IQ or looked at the two aspects separately. There are suggestions that CU traits are differentially related to verbal and non-verbal IQ, with results on non-verbal IQ being somewhat higher (Allen et al. 2013). Our results indicate that higher levels of CU traits are associated with better logical thinking and problem solving skills, but clearly, the issue of CU traits and IQ is not settled.
As amply demonstrated in previous studies, disruptive behaviors have been associated with a range of cognitive problems, in terms of lower IQ (Bridgett and Walker 2006; Langley et al. 2010), EF deficits and high levels of RTV (Kofler et al. 2013). We found prospective links between disruptive behaviors and a range of lower cognitive skills, such as poor working memory and poor interference control, higher RTV and, although only marginally significant, higher disinhibition and lower IQ. These findings are by no means new, but contribute to the empirical knowledge base about components of, and possible backgrounds to, disruptive behaviors. Extending prior research, however, is our finding that when controlling for CU traits, cognitive flaws stand out as specifically predicted by the disruptive aspect of adolescent problems as measured one year earlier. Thus, our prospective analyses confirm the general picture in the literature, conveyed by previous investigations, that cognitive dysfunction is a prime and perhaps specific characteristic of disruptive behaviors.

CU Traits, Disruptive Behavior, and Arousal to Emotional Pictures

As was expected, CU traits predicted lower levels of self-reported arousal to negative pictures. This finding agrees with several previous investigations in which low reactivity to aversive stimuli has been associated with CU traits (Blair and Coles 2000; Dolan and Fullam 2010; Fanti et al. 2017; Herpers et al. 2014; Kimonis et al. 2007; Loney et al. 2003; Pihet et al. 2015). Thus, the general picture suggests that low reactivity to aversive stimuli is associated with, and possibly constitutes, a contributing factor to CU traits. Longitudinal investigations with measures of arousal predicting later CU traits, initially controlling for CU traits, would shed light on this apparent link. Further, CU traits significantly predicted lower arousal to distress pictures whereas the prediction to low arousal to threat pictures was marginal, a finding that agrees with lower levels of empathy for others, which is a core characteristic of CU traits. (Barry et al. 2000; Frick et al. 2003). In contrast, no longitudinal associations were found between disruptive behaviors and level of self-reported arousal to negative picture, which corroborates reports of non-associations with arousal in response to empathy provoking stimuli when controlling for CU traits (de Wied et al. 2012; Kyranides et al. 2016; Masi et al. 2014). Still, higher reactivity to negative words in response to violent films has been related to disruptive behavior, controlling for CU traits (Kyranides et al. 2016; Loney et al. 2003). Perhaps, more engaging and threatening contexts, than were our still pictures, may be needed to trigger arousal in individuals with disruptive behavior problems. In sum, CU traits and disruptive behavior predicted distinct patterns of emotional responding.

Strengths and Limitations

Before drawing conclusions, some strengths as well as drawbacks of the present study should be mentioned. First, we used a prospective design, with the specific associations from CU traits and disruptive behavior to cognitive and emotional phenomena spanning one year, attesting to the robustness of results. Our results thus give support to compatible findings gained in previous studies. Second, we focused on adolescents, which has rarely been done before in the context of CU traits, disruptive behaviors, and, especially, EF deficits. We secured a sample of 90 adolescents with similar levels of earlier disruptive problems as a large population sample, thus our results may be considered relatively representative of Swedish adolescents. However, the sample size was somewhat restricted, which is the rationale for our choice to report results on the p < 0.10 level. Although not significant according to standards, small effects may, especially if they fit into a pattern of significant results, add to the general picture of findings. Third, in contrast to previous studies on this issue, we investigated several aspects of executive functioning as well as IQ in relation to CU traits, thus enhancing knowledge about CU traits and specific dimensions of cognitive skills considered to be some of the core features of executive functioning (Miyake et al. 2000). Fourth, our measure of disruptive behaviors combined ratings by parents and adolescents and included manifestations of ADHD-ODD-delinquent behaviors, i.e., rendering a broad picture of disruptive behaviors. However, this strategy does not inform about how different aspects of disruptive behavior relate to outcomes. Importantly, in the supplementary analyses with the disruptive behavior measure split to form two aspects, the pattern of results was very similar to that of the main analyses, although the strength of the correlations were lower and did not reach significance in some cases. Thus, as our main purpose was to investigate the cognitive and emotional characteristics of CU traits and disruptive behaviors, controlling for each other, a broad measure of disruptive behaviors proved fruitful. Little information was lost, compared with analyses on separate disruptive behavior aspects in relation to CU traits and outcomes. Rather, the results were somewhat stronger. This is perhaps no surprise, given the associations between the three dimensions of disruptive behavior investigated, and the association with CU traits with all three (Herpers et al. 2012).
There are also limitations. First, it should be noted that CU trait levels were low in this sample and the range somewhat restricted (see Table 1). We did not capture adolescents with maximum levels, which may perhaps not to be expected in a non-clinical sample. Still, the fact that we did find associations with outcomes at these levels of CU traits points to the robustness of findings. Second, the longitudinal effects from predictors to cognitive and emotional phenomena were modest in size. Possibly, this may be due to the one-year gap between measurements, naturally giving much room for other influences. On the other hand, it may indicate that, although obviously not without influence, most of the variation in cognitive skills and emotional arousal is explained by other factors than problematic behaviors. Third, we did not have a verbal IQ measure, which might have implications for the picture of IQ and CU traits. Fourth, the validity of the self- reports used to measure responses to pictures of various emotional content may be questioned. However, arousal was higher in response to emotional pictures, compared to neutral pictures, and, more importantly, response time was lower in response to emotional than to neutral pictures, which should attest to self-reports as indicating arousal.

Conclusions

The present study underscores the importance of taking both CU traits and disruptive behavior into account, when investigating either. By so doing we prospectively identified a distinctive cognitive and emotional profile for each of these two related but separate problem behaviors. An individual with high levels of disruptive behaviors seems to have a range of cognitive deficits, both executive and non-executive, but to have normative levels of self-reported arousal in the face of aversive stimuli, at least as assessed in a laboratory context. As CU traits were related to good cognitive functioning and low arousal to emotional stimuli, our findings suggest an individual with high levels of CU traits to fit the description of “cool and cunning”. Our findings support the notion that CU traits, in relation to disruptive problems, are specific in etiology and background factors. However, further studies on adolescent samples are called for to substantiate our findings. Finally, important further research venues within the field of adolescent externalizing problems would be prospective studies of how cognitive and emotional functioning predicts later problems, and investigations of how the individual’s cognitive make-up interact with disruptive behavior and CU traits in predicting adaptation. Targeted interventions for adolescent externalizing problems may be called for, depending on cognitive skills or lack thereof.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Funding

This research was supported by grant no 2009/2015–0167 from the Research Council for Health, Working Life and Welfare.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

The project was approved by the Regional Ethics Committe (No 2010/2013/122).
Informed consent was secured from all participants at each data wave of the project.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Voetnoten
1
All reliability estimates below refer to the present sample
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Allen, J. L., Briskman, J., Humayun, S., Dadds, M. R., & Scott, S. (2013). Heartless and cunning? Intelligence in adolescents with antisocial behavior and psychopathic traits. Psychiatry Research, 2010, 1147–1153.CrossRef Allen, J. L., Briskman, J., Humayun, S., Dadds, M. R., & Scott, S. (2013). Heartless and cunning? Intelligence in adolescents with antisocial behavior and psychopathic traits. Psychiatry Research, 2010, 1147–1153.CrossRef
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV-TR). In American Psychiatric Association. Washington, DC: USA. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV-TR). In American Psychiatric Association. Washington, DC: USA.
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edition (DSM-5). In American Psychiatric Association. Washington, DC: USA. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edition (DSM-5). In American Psychiatric Association. Washington, DC: USA.
go back to reference Andershed, H., Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Levander, S. (2002). Psychopathic traits in non-referred youths: Initial test of a new assessment tool. In E. Blaauwand & L. Sheridan (Eds.), Psychopaths: Current international perspectives (pp. 131–158). The Hague: Elsevier. Andershed, H., Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Levander, S. (2002). Psychopathic traits in non-referred youths: Initial test of a new assessment tool. In E. Blaauwand & L. Sheridan (Eds.), Psychopaths: Current international perspectives (pp. 131–158). The Hague: Elsevier.
go back to reference Andershed, H., Hodgins, S., & Tengström, A. (2007). Convergent validity of the youth psychopathic traits inventory (YPI). Association with the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version. Assessment, 14, 144–154.CrossRefPubMed Andershed, H., Hodgins, S., & Tengström, A. (2007). Convergent validity of the youth psychopathic traits inventory (YPI). Association with the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version. Assessment, 14, 144–154.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Barry, C. T., Frick, P. J., DeShazo, T. M., McCoy, M. G., Ellis, M., & Looney, B. R. (2000). The importance of callous-unemotional traits for extending the concept of psychopathy to children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 335–340.CrossRefPubMed Barry, C. T., Frick, P. J., DeShazo, T. M., McCoy, M. G., Ellis, M., & Looney, B. R. (2000). The importance of callous-unemotional traits for extending the concept of psychopathy to children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 335–340.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Baskin-Sommers, A. R., Waller, R., Fish, A. M., & Hyde, L. W. (2015). Callous-unemotional traits trajectories interact with earlier conduct problems and executive control to predict violence and substance abuse among high risk male adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 1529–1541.CrossRefPubMed Baskin-Sommers, A. R., Waller, R., Fish, A. M., & Hyde, L. W. (2015). Callous-unemotional traits trajectories interact with earlier conduct problems and executive control to predict violence and substance abuse among high risk male adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 1529–1541.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Beauchaine, T. P., Gastzke-Kopp, L., Neuhaus, E., Chipman, J., Reid, M. J., & Webster-Stratton, C. (2013). Sympathetic-and parasympathetic-linked cardiac function and prediction of externalizing behavior, emotion regulation, and prosocial behavior among preschoolers treated for ADHD (2013). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 81, 481–493.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Beauchaine, T. P., Gastzke-Kopp, L., Neuhaus, E., Chipman, J., Reid, M. J., & Webster-Stratton, C. (2013). Sympathetic-and parasympathetic-linked cardiac function and prediction of externalizing behavior, emotion regulation, and prosocial behavior among preschoolers treated for ADHD (2013). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 81, 481–493.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Blair, R. J. R., & Coles, M. (2000). Expression recognition and behavioral problems in early adolescence. Cognitive Development, 15, 421–434.CrossRef Blair, R. J. R., & Coles, M. (2000). Expression recognition and behavioral problems in early adolescence. Cognitive Development, 15, 421–434.CrossRef
go back to reference Bohlin, G., Eninger, L., Brocki, K. C., & Thorell, L. B. (2012). Disorganized attachment and inhibitory capacity: Predicting externalizing problem behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 449–458.CrossRefPubMed Bohlin, G., Eninger, L., Brocki, K. C., & Thorell, L. B. (2012). Disorganized attachment and inhibitory capacity: Predicting externalizing problem behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 449–458.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bridgett, D. J., & Walker, M. E. (2006). Intellectual functioning in adults with ADHD: A meta-analytic examination of full scale IQ differences between adults with and without ADHD. Psychological Assessment, 18, 1–14.CrossRefPubMed Bridgett, D. J., & Walker, M. E. (2006). Intellectual functioning in adults with ADHD: A meta-analytic examination of full scale IQ differences between adults with and without ADHD. Psychological Assessment, 18, 1–14.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Brocki, K. C., Nyberg, L., Thorell, L. B., & Bohlin, G. (2007). Early concurrent and longitudinal symptoms of ADHD and ODD: Relations to different types of inhibitory control and working memory. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 1033–1041.CrossRefPubMed Brocki, K. C., Nyberg, L., Thorell, L. B., & Bohlin, G. (2007). Early concurrent and longitudinal symptoms of ADHD and ODD: Relations to different types of inhibitory control and working memory. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 1033–1041.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Burke, J. D., Loeber, R., & Birmaher, B. (2002). Oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: A review of the past ten years, part II. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 1275–1293.CrossRefPubMed Burke, J. D., Loeber, R., & Birmaher, B. (2002). Oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: A review of the past ten years, part II. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 1275–1293.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bussing, R., Fernandez, M., Harwood, M., Hiu, W., Wilson Garvan, C., Eyberg, S. M., & Swanson, J. (2008). Parent and teacher SNAP-IV ratings of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder behaviors: Psychometric properties and normative ratings from a school district sample. Assessment, 15, 317–328.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bussing, R., Fernandez, M., Harwood, M., Hiu, W., Wilson Garvan, C., Eyberg, S. M., & Swanson, J. (2008). Parent and teacher SNAP-IV ratings of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder behaviors: Psychometric properties and normative ratings from a school district sample. Assessment, 15, 317–328.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Carr, L. A., Henderson, J., & Nigg, J. T. (2010). Cognitive control and attentional selection in adolescents with ADHD versus ADD. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39, 726–740.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Carr, L. A., Henderson, J., & Nigg, J. T. (2010). Cognitive control and attentional selection in adolescents with ADHD versus ADD. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39, 726–740.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Conzelmann, A. B., Gerdes, A. B. M., Mucha, R. F., Weyers, P., Lesch, K.-P., Bähne, C. G., et al. (2014). Autonomic hypoactivity in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the influence of methylphenidate. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 15, 56–65.CrossRefPubMed Conzelmann, A. B., Gerdes, A. B. M., Mucha, R. F., Weyers, P., Lesch, K.-P., Bähne, C. G., et al. (2014). Autonomic hypoactivity in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the influence of methylphenidate. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 15, 56–65.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dadds, M. R., Fraser, J., Frost, A., & Hawes, D. J. (2005). Disentangling the underlying dimension of psychopathy and conduct problems in children. A community study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 400–410.CrossRefPubMed Dadds, M. R., Fraser, J., Frost, A., & Hawes, D. J. (2005). Disentangling the underlying dimension of psychopathy and conduct problems in children. A community study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 400–410.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference De Wied, M., van Boxtel, A., Matthys, W., & Meeus, W. (2012). Verbal, facial and autonomic responses to empathy-eliciting film clips by disruptive male adolescents with high versus low callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 211–223.CrossRefPubMed De Wied, M., van Boxtel, A., Matthys, W., & Meeus, W. (2012). Verbal, facial and autonomic responses to empathy-eliciting film clips by disruptive male adolescents with high versus low callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 211–223.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dolan, M. C., & Fullam, R. (2010). Emotional memory and psychopathic traits in conduct disordered adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 327–331.CrossRef Dolan, M. C., & Fullam, R. (2010). Emotional memory and psychopathic traits in conduct disordered adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 327–331.CrossRef
go back to reference Drabick, D. A. G., Steinberg, E., & Hampton Shields, A. (2015). Overview of DSM disruptive behavior disorders. In T. P. Beacuchaine & S. P. Hinshaw (Eds.), pp. 3–18. The Oxford handbook of externalizing spectrum disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Drabick, D. A. G., Steinberg, E., & Hampton Shields, A. (2015). Overview of DSM disruptive behavior disorders. In T. P. Beacuchaine & S. P. Hinshaw (Eds.), pp. 3–18. The Oxford handbook of externalizing spectrum disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., & Reid, R. (1998). ADHD rating scale - IV. Checklists, norms, and clinical interpretation. New York: Guilford Press. DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., & Reid, R. (1998). ADHD rating scale - IV. Checklists, norms, and clinical interpretation. New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Fanti, K. A., Kyranides, M. N., & Panayiotou, G. (2015). Facial reactions to violent and comedy films: Association with callous-unemotional traits and impulsive aggression. Cognition and Emotion, 31, 209–224.CrossRefPubMed Fanti, K. A., Kyranides, M. N., & Panayiotou, G. (2015). Facial reactions to violent and comedy films: Association with callous-unemotional traits and impulsive aggression. Cognition and Emotion, 31, 209–224.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fanti, K. A., Kimonis, E. R., Hadjicharalambous, M. Z., & Steinberg, L. (2016). Do neurocognitive deficits in decision making differentiate conduct disorder subtypes? European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 25, 989–996. Fanti, K. A., Kimonis, E. R., Hadjicharalambous, M. Z., & Steinberg, L. (2016). Do neurocognitive deficits in decision making differentiate conduct disorder subtypes? European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 25, 989–996.
go back to reference Fanti, K. A., Colins, O. F., Andershed, H., & Sikki, M. (2017). Stability and change in callous-unemotional traits: Longitudinal associations with potential individual and contextual risk and protective factors. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 87, 62–75.CrossRefPubMed Fanti, K. A., Colins, O. F., Andershed, H., & Sikki, M. (2017). Stability and change in callous-unemotional traits: Longitudinal associations with potential individual and contextual risk and protective factors. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 87, 62–75.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Forssman, L., Eninger, L., Tillman, C. M., Rodriguez, A., & Bohlin, G. (2010). Cognitive functioning and family risk factors in relation to symptom behaviors of ADHD and ODD in adolescents. Journal of Attention Disorders, 16, 284–294.CrossRefPubMed Forssman, L., Eninger, L., Tillman, C. M., Rodriguez, A., & Bohlin, G. (2010). Cognitive functioning and family risk factors in relation to symptom behaviors of ADHD and ODD in adolescents. Journal of Attention Disorders, 16, 284–294.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Frick, P. J., Bodin, S. D., & Barry, C. T. (2000). Psychopathic traits and conduct problems in community and clinic-referred samples of children: Further development of the psychopathy screening device. Psychological Assessment, 12, 382–393.CrossRefPubMed Frick, P. J., Bodin, S. D., & Barry, C. T. (2000). Psychopathic traits and conduct problems in community and clinic-referred samples of children: Further development of the psychopathy screening device. Psychological Assessment, 12, 382–393.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Frick, P. J., Cornell, A. H., Bodin, A. S. D., Dane, H. E., Barry, C. T., & Loney, B. R. (2003). Callous-unemotional traits and developmental pathways to severe conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 39, 246–260.CrossRefPubMed Frick, P. J., Cornell, A. H., Bodin, A. S. D., Dane, H. E., Barry, C. T., & Loney, B. R. (2003). Callous-unemotional traits and developmental pathways to severe conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 39, 246–260.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Frick, P. J., Stickle, T. R., Dandreaux, D., Farrell, J. M., & Kimonis, E. (2005). Callous-unemotional traits in predicting severity and stability of conduct problems and delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 471–487.CrossRefPubMed Frick, P. J., Stickle, T. R., Dandreaux, D., Farrell, J. M., & Kimonis, E. (2005). Callous-unemotional traits in predicting severity and stability of conduct problems and delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 471–487.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Frick, P. J., Ray, J. V., Thornton, L. C., & Kahn, R. E. (2014). Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1–57.CrossRefPubMed Frick, P. J., Ray, J. V., Thornton, L. C., & Kahn, R. E. (2014). Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1–57.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Golmaryami, F. N., & Frick, P. J. (2015). Callous-unemotional traits and the development of externalizing spectrum disorders. In: T.P. Beacuchaine & S.P. Hinshaw (Eds), pp 360–374. The Oxford handbook of externalizing spectrum disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Golmaryami, F. N., & Frick, P. J. (2015). Callous-unemotional traits and the development of externalizing spectrum disorders. In: T.P. Beacuchaine & S.P. Hinshaw (Eds), pp 360–374. The Oxford handbook of externalizing spectrum disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Graziano, P. A., & Garcia, A. (2016). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and children’s emotion dysregulation; a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 46, 106–123.CrossRefPubMed Graziano, P. A., & Garcia, A. (2016). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and children’s emotion dysregulation; a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 46, 106–123.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Graziano, P. A., Fabiano, G., Willoughby, M. T., Waschbusch, D., Morris, K., Schatz, N., & Vujnovic, R. (2017). Callous-unemotional traits among adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Associations with parenting. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 48, 18–31.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Graziano, P. A., Fabiano, G., Willoughby, M. T., Waschbusch, D., Morris, K., Schatz, N., & Vujnovic, R. (2017). Callous-unemotional traits among adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Associations with parenting. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 48, 18–31.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Griffiths, K. R., Quintana, D. S., Hermens, D. F., Spooner, C., Tsang, T. W., Clarke, S., & Kohn, M. R. (2017). Sustained attention and heart rate variability in children and adolescents with ADHD. Biological Psychiatry, 124, 11–20.CrossRef Griffiths, K. R., Quintana, D. S., Hermens, D. F., Spooner, C., Tsang, T. W., Clarke, S., & Kohn, M. R. (2017). Sustained attention and heart rate variability in children and adolescents with ADHD. Biological Psychiatry, 124, 11–20.CrossRef
go back to reference Haas, S. M., Waschbusch, D. A., Pelham, W. E., King, S., Andrade, B. F., & Carrey, N. J. (2011). Treatment response in CP/ADHD children with callous/unemotional traits. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 541–552.CrossRefPubMed Haas, S. M., Waschbusch, D. A., Pelham, W. E., King, S., Andrade, B. F., & Carrey, N. J. (2011). Treatment response in CP/ADHD children with callous/unemotional traits. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 541–552.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hawes, D. J., Price, M. J., & Dadds, M. R. (2014). Callous-unemotional traits and the treatment of conduct problems in childhood and adolescence: A comprehensive review. Clinical Child and Family Psychological Review, 17, 248–267.CrossRef Hawes, D. J., Price, M. J., & Dadds, M. R. (2014). Callous-unemotional traits and the treatment of conduct problems in childhood and adolescence: A comprehensive review. Clinical Child and Family Psychological Review, 17, 248–267.CrossRef
go back to reference Herpers, P. C. M., Rommelse, N. N. J., Bons, D. M. A., Buitelaar, J. K., & Scheepers, F. E. (2012). Callous-unemotional traits as a cross-disorder construct. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 47, 2045–2064.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Herpers, P. C. M., Rommelse, N. N. J., Bons, D. M. A., Buitelaar, J. K., & Scheepers, F. E. (2012). Callous-unemotional traits as a cross-disorder construct. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 47, 2045–2064.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Herpers, P. C. M., Scheepers, F. E., Bons, D. M. A., Buitelaar, J. K., & Rommelse, N. N. J. (2014). The cognitive and neural correlates of psychopathy and especially callous-unemotional traits in youths: A systematic review of the evidence. Development and Psychopathology, 26, 245–273.CrossRefPubMed Herpers, P. C. M., Scheepers, F. E., Bons, D. M. A., Buitelaar, J. K., & Rommelse, N. N. J. (2014). The cognitive and neural correlates of psychopathy and especially callous-unemotional traits in youths: A systematic review of the evidence. Development and Psychopathology, 26, 245–273.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hetzel-Riggin, M. D., & Wilber, M. S. (2010). To dissociate or suppress? Predicting automatic vs. conscious cognitive avoidance. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 11, 444–457.CrossRefPubMed Hetzel-Riggin, M. D., & Wilber, M. S. (2010). To dissociate or suppress? Predicting automatic vs. conscious cognitive avoidance. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 11, 444–457.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jezior, K. L., McKenzie, M. E., & Lee, S. S. (2016). Narcissism and callous-unemotional traits prospectively predict child conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 45, 579–590.CrossRefPubMed Jezior, K. L., McKenzie, M. E., & Lee, S. S. (2016). Narcissism and callous-unemotional traits prospectively predict child conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 45, 579–590.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kahn, R. E., Frick, P. J., Golmaryami, F. N., & Marsee, M. A. (2016). The moderating role of anxiety in the associations of callous-unemotional traits with self-report and laboratory measures of affective and cognitive empathy. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45, 583–596.CrossRef Kahn, R. E., Frick, P. J., Golmaryami, F. N., & Marsee, M. A. (2016). The moderating role of anxiety in the associations of callous-unemotional traits with self-report and laboratory measures of affective and cognitive empathy. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45, 583–596.CrossRef
go back to reference Kerr, M., Van Zalk, M., & Stattin, H. (2012). Psychopathic traits moderate peer influence on adolescent delinquency. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 826–835.CrossRefPubMed Kerr, M., Van Zalk, M., & Stattin, H. (2012). Psychopathic traits moderate peer influence on adolescent delinquency. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 826–835.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kessler, R. C., Adler, L., Ames, M., Demler, O., Faraone, S., Hiripi, E., et al. (2005). The World Health Organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): A short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychological Medicine, 35, 245–256.CrossRefPubMed Kessler, R. C., Adler, L., Ames, M., Demler, O., Faraone, S., Hiripi, E., et al. (2005). The World Health Organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): A short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychological Medicine, 35, 245–256.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Keyes, K. M., Platt, J., Kaufman, A. S., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2017). Association of fluid intelligence and psychiatric disorders in a population-representative sample of US adolescents. JAMA Psychiatry, 74, 179–188.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Keyes, K. M., Platt, J., Kaufman, A. S., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2017). Association of fluid intelligence and psychiatric disorders in a population-representative sample of US adolescents. JAMA Psychiatry, 74, 179–188.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Kimonis, E. R., Frick, P. J., Munoz, L. C., & Aucoin, K. J. (2007). Can a laboratory measure of emotional processing enhance the statistical prediction of aggression and delinquency in detained adolescents with callous-unemotional traits? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 773–785.CrossRefPubMed Kimonis, E. R., Frick, P. J., Munoz, L. C., & Aucoin, K. J. (2007). Can a laboratory measure of emotional processing enhance the statistical prediction of aggression and delinquency in detained adolescents with callous-unemotional traits? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 773–785.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kofler, M. J., Rapport, M. D., Sarver, D. E., Raiker, J. S., Orban, S. A., Friedman, L. M., & Kolomeyer, E. G. (2013). Reaction time variability in ADHD: A meta-analytic review of 319 studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 795–811.CrossRefPubMed Kofler, M. J., Rapport, M. D., Sarver, D. E., Raiker, J. S., Orban, S. A., Friedman, L. M., & Kolomeyer, E. G. (2013). Reaction time variability in ADHD: A meta-analytic review of 319 studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 795–811.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kyranides, M. H., Fanti, K. A., & Panayiotou, G. (2016). The disruptive adolescent as grown-up: Predicting adult startle response to violent and erotic films from adolescent conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 38, 183–194.CrossRef Kyranides, M. H., Fanti, K. A., & Panayiotou, G. (2016). The disruptive adolescent as grown-up: Predicting adult startle response to violent and erotic films from adolescent conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 38, 183–194.CrossRef
go back to reference Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical report A-6, Center for Research in psychophysiology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical report A-6, Center for Research in psychophysiology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL.
go back to reference Langley, K., Fowler, T., Ford, T., Thapar, A. K., van den Bree, M., Harold, G., Thapar, A. K., van den Bree, M., Harold, G., Owen, M. J., O'Donovan, M. C., & Thapar, A. (2010). Adolescent clinical outcomes for young people with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 196, 235–240.CrossRefPubMed Langley, K., Fowler, T., Ford, T., Thapar, A. K., van den Bree, M., Harold, G., Thapar, A. K., van den Bree, M., Harold, G., Owen, M. J., O'Donovan, M. C., & Thapar, A. (2010). Adolescent clinical outcomes for young people with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 196, 235–240.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Loeber, R., Burke, J. D., & Lahey, B. B. (2002). What are adolescent antecedents to antisocial personality disorder? Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 12, 24–36.CrossRefPubMed Loeber, R., Burke, J. D., & Lahey, B. B. (2002). What are adolescent antecedents to antisocial personality disorder? Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 12, 24–36.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Loney, B. R., Frick, P. J., Clements, C. B., Ellis, M. L., & Kerlin, K. (2003). Callous-unemotional traits, impulsivity, and emotional processing in adolescents with antisocial behavior problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32, 66–80.CrossRefPubMed Loney, B. R., Frick, P. J., Clements, C. B., Ellis, M. L., & Kerlin, K. (2003). Callous-unemotional traits, impulsivity, and emotional processing in adolescents with antisocial behavior problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32, 66–80.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Loney, B. R., Butler, M. A., Lima, E. N., Counts, C. A., & Eckel, L. A. (2006). The relation between salivary cortisol, callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in an adolescent non-referred sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 30–36.CrossRefPubMed Loney, B. R., Butler, M. A., Lima, E. N., Counts, C. A., & Eckel, L. A. (2006). The relation between salivary cortisol, callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in an adolescent non-referred sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 30–36.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Magnusson, D., Dunér, A., & Zetterbloom, G. (1975). Adjustment: A longitudinal study. New York: Wiley. Magnusson, D., Dunér, A., & Zetterbloom, G. (1975). Adjustment: A longitudinal study. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Masi, G., Milone, A., Pisano, S., Lenzi, F., Muratori, P., Gemo, I., Bianchi, L., Mazzone, L., Postorino, V., Sanges, V., Williams, R., & Vicari, S. (2014). Emotional reactivity in referred youth with disruptive behavior disorders. The role of callous-unemotional traits. Psychiatry Research, 220, 426–432.CrossRefPubMed Masi, G., Milone, A., Pisano, S., Lenzi, F., Muratori, P., Gemo, I., Bianchi, L., Mazzone, L., Postorino, V., Sanges, V., Williams, R., & Vicari, S. (2014). Emotional reactivity in referred youth with disruptive behavior disorders. The role of callous-unemotional traits. Psychiatry Research, 220, 426–432.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wäger, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.CrossRefPubMed Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wäger, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Munoz, L. C., Frick, P. J., Kimonis, E. R., & Aucoin, K. J. (2008). Verbal ability and delinquency: Testing the moderating role of psychopathic traits. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 414–421.CrossRefPubMed Munoz, L. C., Frick, P. J., Kimonis, E. R., & Aucoin, K. J. (2008). Verbal ability and delinquency: Testing the moderating role of psychopathic traits. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 414–421.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Murray, J., & Farrington, D. P. (2010). Risk factors for conduct disorder and delinquency: Key findings from longitudinal studies. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 55, 633–642.CrossRefPubMed Murray, J., & Farrington, D. P. (2010). Risk factors for conduct disorder and delinquency: Key findings from longitudinal studies. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 55, 633–642.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Musser, E. D., Backs, R. W., Schmitt, C. F., Ablow, J. C., Measelle, J. R., & Nigg, J. T. (2011). Emotion regulation via the autonomic nervous system in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 841–852.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Musser, E. D., Backs, R. W., Schmitt, C. F., Ablow, J. C., Measelle, J. R., & Nigg, J. T. (2011). Emotion regulation via the autonomic nervous system in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 841–852.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Musser, E. D., Galloway-Long, H. S., Frick, P. J., & Nigg, J. T. (2013). Emotion regulation and heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychology, 52, 163–171.CrossRef Musser, E. D., Galloway-Long, H. S., Frick, P. J., & Nigg, J. T. (2013). Emotion regulation and heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychology, 52, 163–171.CrossRef
go back to reference Pardini, D. (2011). Perceptions of social conflicts among incarcerated adolescents with callous-unemotional traits: You’re going to pay. It’s going to hurt, but I don’t care. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 248–255.CrossRefPubMed Pardini, D. (2011). Perceptions of social conflicts among incarcerated adolescents with callous-unemotional traits: You’re going to pay. It’s going to hurt, but I don’t care. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 248–255.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pardini, D., Obradovic, J., & Loeber, R. (2006). Interpersonal callousness, hyperactivity/impulsivity, inattention and conduct problems as precursors to delinquent persistent in boys: A comparison of three grade-based cohorts. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35, 46–59.CrossRefPubMed Pardini, D., Obradovic, J., & Loeber, R. (2006). Interpersonal callousness, hyperactivity/impulsivity, inattention and conduct problems as precursors to delinquent persistent in boys: A comparison of three grade-based cohorts. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35, 46–59.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pihet, S., Etter, S., Schmid, M., & Kimonis, E. R. (2015). Assessing callous-unemotional traits in adolescents: Validity of the inventory of callous-unemotional traits across gender, age and community/institutionalized status. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 37, 407–421.CrossRef Pihet, S., Etter, S., Schmid, M., & Kimonis, E. R. (2015). Assessing callous-unemotional traits in adolescents: Validity of the inventory of callous-unemotional traits across gender, age and community/institutionalized status. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 37, 407–421.CrossRef
go back to reference Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2000, Updated 2003). Manual for Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices. Pearson Pearson, Texas. Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2000, Updated 2003). Manual for Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices. Pearson Pearson, Texas.
go back to reference Roose, A., Bijttebier, P., Claes, L., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011). Psychopathic traits in adolescence: Associations with the revised reinforcement sensitivity theory systems. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 201–205.CrossRef Roose, A., Bijttebier, P., Claes, L., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011). Psychopathic traits in adolescence: Associations with the revised reinforcement sensitivity theory systems. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 201–205.CrossRef
go back to reference Rowe, R., Costello, E. J., Angold, A., Copeland, W. E., & Maughan, B. (2010). Developmental pathways in oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 726–738.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Rowe, R., Costello, E. J., Angold, A., Copeland, W. E., & Maughan, B. (2010). Developmental pathways in oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 726–738.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Rydell, A.-M. (2010). Family factors and children’s disruptive behavior: An investigation of links between demographic characteristics, negative life events and symptoms of ODD and ADHD. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 45(2), 233–244.CrossRefPubMed Rydell, A.-M. (2010). Family factors and children’s disruptive behavior: An investigation of links between demographic characteristics, negative life events and symptoms of ODD and ADHD. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 45(2), 233–244.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Salekin, R. T., Neuman, C. S., Leisticio, A.-M. R., & Zalot, A. A. (2004). Psychopathy in youth and intelligence: An investigation of Checkley’s hypothesis. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 731–742.CrossRefPubMed Salekin, R. T., Neuman, C. S., Leisticio, A.-M. R., & Zalot, A. A. (2004). Psychopathy in youth and intelligence: An investigation of Checkley’s hypothesis. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 731–742.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Scholtens, S., Diamantopoulou, S., Tillman, C. M., & Rydell, A.-M. (2012). Effects of symptoms of ADHD, ODD, and cognitive functioning on social acceptance and the positive illusory bias in children. Journal of Attention Disorders, 16, 685–696.CrossRefPubMed Scholtens, S., Diamantopoulou, S., Tillman, C. M., & Rydell, A.-M. (2012). Effects of symptoms of ADHD, ODD, and cognitive functioning on social acceptance and the positive illusory bias in children. Journal of Attention Disorders, 16, 685–696.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., & Oosterlan, J. (2002). How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Behavioral Brain Research, 130, 3–28.CrossRef Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., & Oosterlan, J. (2002). How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Behavioral Brain Research, 130, 3–28.CrossRef
go back to reference Soncin, S., Brien, D. C., Coe, B. C., Marin, A., & Munoz, D. P. (2016). Contrasting emotions processing and executive functioning in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and bipolar disorder. Behavioral Neuroscience, 130, 531–543.CrossRefPubMed Soncin, S., Brien, D. C., Coe, B. C., Marin, A., & Munoz, D. P. (2016). Contrasting emotions processing and executive functioning in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and bipolar disorder. Behavioral Neuroscience, 130, 531–543.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tillman, C. M., Nyberg, L., & Bohlin, G. (2008). Working memory components and intelligence in children. Intelligence, 36, 394–402.CrossRef Tillman, C. M., Nyberg, L., & Bohlin, G. (2008). Working memory components and intelligence in children. Intelligence, 36, 394–402.CrossRef
go back to reference Wall, T. D., Frick, P. J., Fanti, K. A., Kimonis, E. R., & Lordos, A. (2016). Factors differentiating callous-unemotional children with and without conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 976–983.CrossRefPubMed Wall, T. D., Frick, P. J., Fanti, K. A., Kimonis, E. R., & Lordos, A. (2016). Factors differentiating callous-unemotional children with and without conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 976–983.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Waller, R., Wright, A. G. C., Shaw, D. S., Gardner, F., Dishion, T. J., Wilson, M. N., & Hyde, L. W. (2015). Factor structure and construct validity of the parent-reported inventory of callous-unemotional traits among high-risk 9-year-olds. Assessment, 22, 561–580.CrossRefPubMed Waller, R., Wright, A. G. C., Shaw, D. S., Gardner, F., Dishion, T. J., Wilson, M. N., & Hyde, L. W. (2015). Factor structure and construct validity of the parent-reported inventory of callous-unemotional traits among high-risk 9-year-olds. Assessment, 22, 561–580.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ward, A. R., Alarcon, G., Nigg, J. T., & Musser, E. D. (2015). Variation in parasympathetic dysregulation moderates short-term memory problems in childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 1573–1583.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ward, A. R., Alarcon, G., Nigg, J. T., & Musser, E. D. (2015). Variation in parasympathetic dysregulation moderates short-term memory problems in childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 1573–1583.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Waschbuch, D. A., Carrey, M. T., Willoughby, M. T., King, S., & Andrade, B. F. (2007). Effects of methylphenidate and behavior modification on the social and academic behavior of children with disruptive behavior disorders: The moderating role of callous/unemotional traits. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36, 629–644.CrossRef Waschbuch, D. A., Carrey, M. T., Willoughby, M. T., King, S., & Andrade, B. F. (2007). Effects of methylphenidate and behavior modification on the social and academic behavior of children with disruptive behavior disorders: The moderating role of callous/unemotional traits. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36, 629–644.CrossRef
go back to reference Wiggs, K., Elmore, A. L., Nigg, J. T., & Nikolas, M. (2016). Pre- and perinatal risk for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Does neuropsychological weakness explain the link? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44, 1473–1485.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wiggs, K., Elmore, A. L., Nigg, J. T., & Nikolas, M. (2016). Pre- and perinatal risk for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Does neuropsychological weakness explain the link? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44, 1473–1485.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336–1346.CrossRefPubMed Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336–1346.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Cognitive and Emotional Profiles of CU Traits and Disruptive Behavior in Adolescence: a Prospective Study
Auteurs
Ann-Margret Rydell
Karin C. Brocki
Publicatiedatum
07-12-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology / Uitgave 6/2019
Print ISSN: 2730-7166
Elektronisch ISSN: 2730-7174
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0496-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2019

Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology 6/2019 Naar de uitgave