Skip to main content

Everyday Concepts of Emotions Following Every-Other-Day Errors in Joint Plans

  • Chapter
Everyday Conceptions of Emotion

Part of the book series: NATO ASI Series ((ASID,volume 81))

Abstract

There has been recent criticism of the idea that we humans know about our mental states. In social psychology, for example, forced compliance experiments have produced results that question whether we know why we do what we do. Nisbett and Wilson (1977) have shown that subjects in their (non-naturalistic) studies gave reasons for their behaviour that were, some might say regrettably, guided by folk theories. Thus, women who were asked to choose from a table displaying identical pairs of stockings gave as the reason for choosing the stockings they selected that they liked their sheerness, weave and knit — generally accepted standards for assessing the quality of hosiery. The experimenters found, by contrast, that the position of the stockings on the table was the determining factor in their choice. These women thought they knew why they chose a pair of stockings but were merely applying the culture’s folk theoretical explanation for actions that were determined in quite another way.

The existence of the experimental method makes us think we have the means of solving the problems which trouble us; though problem and method pass one another by.

Ludwig Wittgenstein, Philosophical Investigationsg p. 232

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 259.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bratman, M. E. (1989). What is intention? In P. R. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. E. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions in communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P. S. (1986). Neurophilosophy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, P. R., & Levesque, H. J. (1989). Rational interaction as the basis for communication. In P. R. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. E. Pollack (Eds), Intentions in communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, K. (1943). The nature of eexplanation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, K. (1993). How scientists really reason: Scientific reasoning in real-world laboratories. In R. J. Sternberg & J. Davidson (Eds), Mechanisms of insight. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1957). Meaning. Philosophical Review, 66, 377–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1968). Utterer’s meaning and intentions. Philosophical Review, 78, 147–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grosz, B. J., & Sidner, C. L. (1989). Plans for discourse. In P. R. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. E. Pollack (Eds), Intentions in communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1983). The principles ofpsychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published in 1890).

    Google Scholar 

  • Larocque, L, & Oatley, K. (1994). Emotions and errors in joint plans. Submitted.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, D. (1969). Convention: A philosophical study. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we know: Reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett R., & Ross, I. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K., & Duncan, E. (1992). Incidents of emotion in daily life. In K. Strongman (Ed), International review of studies of emotion (Vol.2, pp. 249–293). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K., & Duncan, E. (1994). The experience of emotions in everyday life. Cognition and Emorion, 8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 29–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 25–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollack, M. E. (1989). Plans as complex mental attitudes. In P. R. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. E. Pollack (Eds), Intentions in communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Power, R. (1979). The organisation of purposeful dialogues. Linguistics, 17, 107–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Power, R. (1984). Mutual intention. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 14(1), 85–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. T. (1984). Absent-mindedness and cognitive control. In J. Harris & P. Morris (Eds), Everyday memory, actions and absent-nmindedness. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. T. (1991). Humnan Error. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rimé, B., Philippot, P., Boca, S., & Mesquita, B. (1992). Longlasting cognitive and social consequences of emotion: social sharing and rumination. European Review of Social Psychology, 3, 225–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, S. (1972). Meaning. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suchman, L. A. (1987). Plans and situated action: The problem of human-machine communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Scienca, 185, 1124–1131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiener, B., & Graham, S. (1989). Understanding the motivational role of affect: Lifespan research from an attributional perspective. Cognition and Ernotion, 3, 401–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D. (1985). Strangers to ourselves: The origins and accuracy of beliefs about one’s own mental states. In J. Harvey & G. Weary (Eds), Attribution: Basic issues and applications. Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1995 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Oatley, K., Larocque, L. (1995). Everyday Concepts of Emotions Following Every-Other-Day Errors in Joint Plans. In: Russell, J.A., Fernández-Dols, JM., Manstead, A.S.R., Wellenkamp, J.C. (eds) Everyday Conceptions of Emotion. NATO ASI Series, vol 81. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-8484-5_8

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-8484-5_8

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-481-4551-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-015-8484-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics