Skip to main content

Relevance, Meaning and the Cognitive Science of Wisdom

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Scientific Study of Personal Wisdom

Abstract

Wisdom involves the enhancement of cognition (broadly construed). Cognitive science is converging on the conclusion that the central process of cognition that makes us intelligent agents is the ability to realize relevance. Therefore, a powerful way to enhance cognition is to enhance the process of relevance realization. Yet intelligence, although necessary, is not sufficient for wisdom. This makes good sense because there is good evidence that intelligence is not sufficient for rationality. Rationality involves the recursive application of intelligence to the problem of using intelligence well. In a similar manner, wisdom is the recursive application of rationality to the problem of developing good use of rationality. Wisdom is a process whereby rationality transcends itself in a rational manner so as to greatly enhance our central abilities of relevance realization. In particular, people who engage in this development should have enhanced abilities of active open-mindedness, insight, self-regulation and perspectival knowing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Weisberg and Alba (1981) explicitly asked subjects to ‘go outside the box’ in an attempt to facilitate the solution of the problem. They found that this admonition had very little effect, sufficiently so that Weisberg and Alba called the very existence of insight into question. This undermines the cultural currency of the commonly used phrase; it has been empirically shown to not accomplish precisely what we seek to accomplish when we use it. Saying “be insightful” does not in fact provoke insight.

  2. 2.

    Note that here we are invoking a version of dual processing theory, as reviewed in Stanovich (2002).

  3. 3.

    We owe this idea to work currently being done with Greg Katsoras.

  4. 4.

    A distinction needs to be made here between temperament and style; historically, the concept of temperament in developmental psychology refers to some innate element of self or personality, which expresses itself in certain preferences and behaviours. In contrast, the term ‘style’ has usually referred to an acquired set of sensitivities and abilities. This can lead to some confusion, as ‘style’ can be taken to be equivalent to manner, which could be due either to innate temperament, learned style, or both. For the purposes of this chapter, ‘cognitive style’ specifically invokes the historical concept of an acquired suite of abilities and sensitivities.

  5. 5.

    This language of affordances and constraints is a hallmark of mathematical modeling within dynamical systems theory.

  6. 6.

    ‘Need for cognition’ refers to that quality in people of seeking mental challenge and learning opportunities.

References

  • Adams, L. T., Kasserman, J. E., Yearwood, A. A., Perfetto, G. A., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1988). Memory access: The effects of fact-oriented versus problem-oriented acquisition. Memory & Cognition, 16, 167–175. doi:10.3758/BF03213486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anders Ericsson, K., & Towne, T. J. (2010). Expertise. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(3), 404–416. doi:10.1002/wcs.47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ardelt, M. (2004). Wisdom as expert knowledge system: A critical review of a contemporary operationalization of an ancient concept. Human Development, 47, 257–285. doi:10.1159/000079154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, K. (2007). The great transformation: The beginning of our religious traditions. New York: Random House of Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ayduk, O., & Mischel, W. (2002). When smart people behave stupidly: Reconciling inconsistencies in social-emotional intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Why smart people can be so stupid (pp. 86–105). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 125–143. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bpg015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bak, P., Tang, C., & Wiesenfeld, K. (1987). Self-organized criticality: An explanation of the 1/f noise. Physical Review Letters, 59(4), 381. doi:10.1103/PhysRevLett.59.381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Wisdom: A metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. American Psychologist, 55, 122–136. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J. (2008). Thinking and deciding. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bassett, D. S., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Achard, S., Duke, T., & Bullmore, E. (2006). Adaptive reconfiguration of fractal small-world human brain functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(51), 19518–19523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchaine, T. (2001). Vagal tone, development, and Gray’s motivational theory: Toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 183–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beggs, J. M., & Plenz, D. (2003). Neuronal avalanches in neocortical circuits. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(35), 11167–11177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beggs, J. M., & Plenz, D. (2004). Neuronal avalanches are diverse and precise activity patterns that are stable for many hours in cortical slice cultures. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24(22), 5216–5229. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0540-04.2004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bellah, R. N. (2005). What is Axial about the Axial Age? European Journal of Sociology, 46(1), 69–89. Retrieved from http://resolver.scholarsportal.info/resolve/00039756/v46i0001/69_wiaataa.xml

  • Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., Segal, Z. V., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 230–241. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bph077.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bluck, S., & Glück, J. (2005). From the inside out: People’s implicit theories of wisdom. In R. J. Sternberg & J. Jordan (Eds.), A handbook of wisdom: Psychological perspectives (pp. 84–109). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, W. S. (2000). Understanding wisdom: Sources, science & society. Philadelphia: Templeton Foundation Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullmore, E., & Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(3), 186–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cherniak, C. (1990). Minimal rationality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chisholm, R. M. (1982). The foundations of knowing. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cox, B. D., & Lightfoot, C. (1997). Sociogenetic perspectives on internalization. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Rathmunde, K. (1990). The psychology of wisdom: An evolutionary interpretation. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 25–51). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (2000). The “What” and “Why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennet, D. (1984). Cognitive wheels: The frame problem in AI. In C. Hookway (Ed.), Minds, machines, and evolution (pp. 128–151). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. American Psychologist, 55, 34–43. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276–302. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dietrich, A., Riese, H., Sondeijker, F. E., Greaves-Lord, K., van Roon, A. M., Ormel, J., & Rosmalen, J. G. M. (2007). Externalizing and internalizing problems in relation to autonomic function: A population-based study in preadolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 378–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenstadt, S. N. (1982). The axial age: The emergence of transcendental visions and the rise of clerics. European Journal of Sociology, 23(02), 294–314. doi:10.1017/S0003975600003908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farb, N. A. S., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., McKeon, D., Fatima, Z., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Attending to the present: Mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(4), 313–322. doi:10.1093/scan/nsm030.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (2000, July 20). A science of Tuesdays. London Review of Books, 22(14), 21–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frankfurt, H. G. (2006). The reasons of love. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, E. (2005). The wisdom paradox: How your mind can grow stronger as your brain grows older. New York: Gotham Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hadot, P., & Chase, M. (2004). What is ancient philosophy? Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hadot, P., & Davidson, A. I. (1995). Philosophy as a way of life: Spiritual exercises from Socrates to Foucault. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, S. S. (2010). Wisdom: From Philosophy to Neuroscience. Random House Digital, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. (1949). The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 56, 51–65. doi:10.1037/h0062474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. M., & Feldman, G. (2004). Clarifying the construct of mindfulness in the context of emotion regulation and the process of change in therapy. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 255–262. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bph080.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogarth, R. M. (2001). Educating intuition (1st ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaspers, K. (2011). Origin and goal of history. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kekes, J. (1986). The informed will and the meaning of life. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 47(1), 75–90. doi:10.2307/2107725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kekes, J. (1995). Moral wisdom and good lives. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kekes, J. (2000). The meaning of life. In P. A. French & H. K. Wettstein (Eds.), Life and death: Metaphysics and ethics (pp. 17–34). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kekes, J. (2005). The art of life. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kekes, J. (2006). The enlargement of life: Moral imagination at work. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M., & Moulton, S. T. (2008). Mental imagery and implicit memory. In K. D. Markman, W. M. P. Klein, & J. A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of imagination and mental simulation (pp. 35–51). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozhevnikov, M. (2007). Cognitive styles in the context of modern psychology: Toward an integrated framework of cognitive style. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 464–481. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.3.464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E. J. (1989). Mindfulness. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Pub. CO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E. J. (1997). The power of mindful learning. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E. J., & Moldoveanu, M. (2000). The construct of mindfulness. Journal of Social Issues, 56(1), 1–9. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M. D. (2000). The promise of dynamic systems approaches for an integrated account of human development. Child Development, 71(1), 36–43. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lockhart, R. S., Lamon, M., & Gick, M. L. (1988). Conceptual transfer in simple insight problems. Memory & Cognition, 16, 36–44. doi:10.3758/BF03197743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Long, A. A. (2004). Epictetus: A Stoic and Socratic Guide to Life. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGhee, M. (2000). Transformations of mind: Philosophy as spiritual practice. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McKee, P., & Barber, C. (1999). On defining wisdom. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 49(2), 149–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meeks, T. W., & Jeste, D. V. (2009). Neurobiology of wisdom: A literature overview. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 66(4), 355–365. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercado, E. (2008). Neural and cognitive plasticity: From maps to minds. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 109–137. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3–19. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myrseth, K. O. R., & Fishbach, A. (2009). Self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(4), 247–252. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01645.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Needham, D. R., & Begg, I. M. (1991). Problem-oriented training promotes spontaneous analogical transfer: Memory-oriented training promotes memory for training. Memory & Cognition, 19, 543–557. doi:10.3758/BF03197150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, A. (1990). An essay on wisdom: Towards organismic processes that make it possible. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 240–278). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, D. (2002). The engine of folly. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Why smart people can be so stupid (pp. 64–85). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russon, J. E. (2009). Bearing witness to epiphany: Persons, things, and the nature of erotic life. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmid, W. T. (1998). Plato’s Charmides and the Socratic ideal of rationality. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, J. W., Ohlsson, S., & Brooks, K. (1993). Thoughts beyond words: When language overshadows insight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 166–183. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.122.2.166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. I. (1975). The age of transcendence. Daedalus, 104(2), 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (1992). The rediscovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, D. J. (2007). The mindful brain: Reflection and attunement in the cultivation of well-being. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, D. (2010). Mindsight: The new science of personal transformation. New York: Random House of Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. (2002). Rationality, intelligence and levels of analysis in cognitive science: Is dysrationalia possible? In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Why smart people can be so stupid (pp. 124–158). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (2005). The Robot’s Rebellion: Finding meaning in the Age of Darwin. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (2009). What intelligence tests miss: The psychology of rational thought. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E, & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(5), 645–665; discussion 665–726.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staudinger, U. M., Dorner, J., & Mickler, C. (2005). Wisdom and personality. In R. J. Sternberg & J. Jordan (Eds.), A handbook of wisdom: Psychological perspectives (pp. 191–220). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1998). A balance theory of wisdom. Review of General Psychology, 2, 347–365. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.4.347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1999). Thinking styles. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (2002). Why smart people can be so stupid. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Wisdom, intelligence, and creativity synthesized. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., & Jordan, J. (2005). A handbook of wisdom: Psychological perspectives. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D. (1999). Metacognition, mindfulness and the modification of mood disorders. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 6(2), 146–155. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0879(199905)6:2<146::AID-CPP195>3.0.CO;2-E.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124–1131. doi:10.1126/science.185.4157.1124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vervaeke, J. A. (1997). The naturalistic imperative in cognitive science. Electronic thesis or dissertation. Retrieved September 8, 2011, from https://tspace.library.utoronto.ca/handle/1807/10696

  • Vervaeke, J., Lillicrap, T. P., & Richards, B. A. (2009). Relevance realization and the emerging framework in cognitive science. Journal of Logic and Computation. doi:10.1093/logcom/exp067.

  • Vicente, K. J., & Wang, J. H. (1998). An ecological theory of expertise effects in memory recall. Psychological Review, 105, 33–57. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.105.1.33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and Language. (A. Kozulin, Ed.) (revised ed.). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weisberg, R. W., & Alba, J. W. (1981). An examination of the alleged role of “fixation” in the solution of several “insight” problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 169–192. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.110.2.169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1984). The zone of proximal development: Some conceptual issues. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1984(23), 7–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J., Minick, N., & Arns, F. (1984). The creation of context in joint problem-solving. In B. Rogoff & J. Lave (Eds.), Everyday cognition: Its development in social context (pp. 151–171). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, S. (1997). Happiness and meaning: Two aspects of the good life. Social Philosophy and Policy, 14(01), 207–225. doi:10.1017/S0265052500001734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, S. R., & Koethe, J. (2010). Meaning in life and why it matters. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Greg Katsoras for his help with the idea of perspectival knowing. We would like to thank Najam Tirimizi for his help with the idea of rationally self-transcending rationality. Finally, we would like to thank David Kim for his help in discussions of the role of wisdom in resolving internal conflict.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John Vervaeke .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Vervaeke, J., Ferraro, L. (2013). Relevance, Meaning and the Cognitive Science of Wisdom. In: Ferrari, M., Weststrate, N. (eds) The Scientific Study of Personal Wisdom. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-9231-1_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-9231-1_2

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-481-9230-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-90-481-9231-1

Publish with us

Policies and ethics