Skip to main content

Conclusion

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Bilingualism and Cognitive Control

Part of the book series: The Bilingual Mind and Brain Book Series ((BMBBS,volume 6))

  • 1412 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter offers a summary of the ideas covered in the book and a perspective on future research. The chapter summarises the various theoretical proposals reviewed in the earlier chapters. Despite several years of intensive research on the cognitive consequences of bilingualism, some of the core issues remain unsettled. I have highlighted that individual differences in executive control should be taken up more seriously, including understanding the integration between bilingualism and cognition in a more diversified international context. Newer methodologies and analysis methods might be helpful in clearing up some of the confusion. I also touch upon some of the current global issues such as migration and the refugee crisis in this chapter. Since bilingualism is a social phenomenon, it is necessary to take into consideration such social changes and examine their impact. It is time bilingualism research moves out of the laboratory and that factors previously ignored such as the social and linguistic context of the bilingualism are considered more specifically.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 89.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., … Costa, A. (2011). Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 22(9), 2076–2086.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. (2016). Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: Neural adaptation and reserve. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(4), 689–698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., ... & Kaul, S. (2013). Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81(22), 1938–1944.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amara, M. (2017). Arabic in Israel: Language, identity and conflict. Abingdon, UK: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., … Carreiras, M. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 398.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Arar, K., & Massry-Herzalah, A. (2017). Progressive education and the case of a bilingual Palestinian-Arab and Jewish co-existence school in Israel. School Leadership & Management, 37(1–2), 38–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arredondo, M. M., Hu, X. S., Satterfield, T., & Kovelman, I. (2017). Bilingualism alters children’s frontal lobe functioning for attentional control. Developmental Science, 20(3). https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12377

  • Babiloni, C., Marzano, N., Infarinato, F., Iacoboni, M., Rizza, G., Aschieri, P., … Del Percio, C. (2010). “Neural efficiency” of experts’ brain during judgment of actions: a high-resolution EEG study in elite and amateur karate athletes. Behavioural Brain Research, 207(2), 466–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2014). Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 35(5), 857–894.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bekerman, Z. (2016). The promise of integrated multicultural and bilingual education: Inclusive Palestinian-Arab and Jewish schools in Israel. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bhatia, D., Prasad, S. G., Sake, K., & Mishra, R. K. (2017). Task Irrelevant External Cues Can Influence Language Selection in Voluntary Object Naming: Evidence from Hindi-English Bilinguals. PloS one, 12(1), e0169284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2010). Bilingualism. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(4), 559–572.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2013). Parallel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 547–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual mechanisms framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 106–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung-Fat-Yim, A., Sorge, G. B., & Bialystok, E. (2017). The relationship between bilingualism and selective attention in young adults: Evidence from an ambiguous figures task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(3), 366–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diab, M., Peltonen, K., Qouta, S. R., Palosaari, E., & Punamäki, R. L. (2017). Can functional emotion regulation protect children’s mental health from war trauma? A Palestinian study. International Journal of Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12427

  • Dijkstra, T., Van Heuven, W. J., & Grainger, J. (1998). Simulating cross-language competition with the bilingual interactive activation model. Psychologica Belgica, 38, 177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, A., & Mavisakalyan, A. (2015). Russian language skills and employment in the Former Soviet Union. Economics of Transition, 23(3), 625–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eichert, N., Peeters, D., & Hagoort, P. (2017). Language-driven anticipatory eye movements in virtual reality. Behavior Research Methods, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0929-z

  • Engen, H. G., Bernhardt, B. C., Skottnik, L., Ricard, M., & Singer, T. (2017). Structural changes in socio-affective networks: Multi-modal MRI findings in long-term meditation practitioners. Neuropsychologia. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.08.024

  • Extra, G., & Verhoeven, L. (Eds.). (1999). Bilingualism and migration (Vol. 14). Berlin, Germany: Walter de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P. (2016). Research on individual differences in executive functions. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(5), 535–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, 186–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friesen, D. C., Latman, V., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2015). Attention during visual search: The benefit of bilingualism. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19(6), 693–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(27), 9240–9245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaley, A., & Nobre, A. C. (2012). Top-down modulation: Bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 129–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gozli, D. G., & Ansorge, U. (2016). Action selection as a guide for visual attention. Visual Cognition, 24(1), 38–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grosjean, F. (2001). The bilingual’s language modes. In J. Nicol (Ed.), One mind, two languages: Bilingual language processing (p. 122). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grundy, J. G., Chung-Fat-Yim, A., Friesen, D. C., Mak, L., & Bialystok, E. (2017). Sequential congruency effects reveal differences in disengagement of attention for monolingual and bilingual young adults. Cognition, 163, 42–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haman, E., Wodniecka, Z., Marecka, M., Szewczyk, J., Białecka-Pikul, M., Otwinowska, A., … & Kacprzak, A. (2017). How does L1 and L2 exposure impact L1 performance in bilingual children? Evidence from Polish-English migrants to the United Kingdom. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 1444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedge, C., Powell, G., & Sumner, P. (2017). The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1

  • Hedger, N., Adams, W. J., & Garner, M. (2015). Autonomic arousal and attentional orienting to visual threat are predicted by awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(3), 798–806.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 625–658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hon, N. (2017). Attention and the sense of agency: A review and some thoughts on the matter. Consciousness and cognition, 56, 30–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huettig, F., & Janse, E. (2016). Individual differences in working memory and processing speed predict anticipatory spoken language processing in the visual world. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 80–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huettig, F., Mishra, R. K., & Olivers, C. N. (2012). Mechanisms and representations of language-mediated visual attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janssens, R. (2015). Language conflict in Brussels: Political mind-set versus linguistic practice. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 2015(235), 53–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kapiley, K., & Mishra, R. K. (in press) What do I choose? Influence of interlocutor on bilingual language choice during object naming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleinman, D., & Gollan, T. H. (2016). Speaking two languages for the price of one: By passing language control mechanisms via accessibility-driven switches. Psychological Science, 27(5), 700–714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(2), 149–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Medvedeva, M., & Portes, A. (2017). Immigrant Bilingualism in Spain: An asset or a liability? International Migration Review, 51(3), 632–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, R. K. (2015). Interaction between attention and language systems in humans. New Delhi, India: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, R. K., Hilchey, M. D., Singh, N., & Klein, R. M. (2012). On the time course of exogenous cueing effects in bilinguals: Higher proficiency in a second language is associated with more rapid endogenous disengagement. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(8), 1502–1510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, R. K., & Singh, N. (2014). Language non-selective activation of orthography during spoken word processing in Hindi–English sequential bilinguals: An eye tracking visual world study. Reading and Writing, 27(1), 129–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, R. K., & Singh, N. (2016). The influence of second language proficiency on bilingual parallel language activation in Hindi–English bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28(4), 396–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, R., Myles, F., & Marsden, E. (2013). Second language learning theories. New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molnar, M., Ibáñez-Molina, A., & Carreiras, M. (2015). Interlocutor identity affects language activation in bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 81, 91–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morawetz, C., Oganian, Y., Schlickeiser, U., Jacobs, A. M., & Heekeren, H. R. (2017). Second language use facilitates implicit emotion regulation via content labeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olivers, C. N. L., Huettig, F., Singh, J. P., & Mishra, R. K. (2014). The influence of literacy on visual search. Visual Cognition, 22(1), 74–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peeters, D., & Dijkstra, T. (2017). Sustained inhibition of the native language in bilingual language production: A virtual reality approach. Bilingualism: Language and Cognitions. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000396

  • Pessoa, L. (2005). To what extent are emotional visual stimuli processed without attention and awareness? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(2), 188–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ren, Y., Wyver, S., Xu Rattanasone, N., & Demuth, K. (2016). Social competence and language skills in Mandarin–English bilingual preschoolers: The moderation effect of emotion regulation. Early Education and Development, 27(3), 303–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E., & Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(4), 763–797.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saint-Aubin, J., Hilchey, M., Mishra, R. K., Singh, N., Savoie, D., Guitard, D., & Klein, R. (in press). Does the relation between the control of attention and second language proficiency generalize from India to Canada? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skeide, M. A., Kumar, U., Mishra, R. K., Tripathi, V. N., Guleria, A., Singh, J. P., … Huettig, F. (2017). Learning to read alters cortico-subcortical cross-talk in the visual system of illiterates. Science Advances, 3(5), e1602612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus, M. K., Magnuson, J. S., Dahan, D., & Chambers, C. (2000). Eye movements and lexical access in spoken-language comprehension: Evaluating a linking hypothesis between fixations and linguistic processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(6), 557–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632–1634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Fan, Y., Feng, H., Wang, J., Feng, S., … Zhang, Y. (2009). Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is altered by short-term meditation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(22), 8865–8870.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tarr, M. J., & Warren, W. H. (2002). Virtual reality in behavioral neuroscience and beyond. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 1089–1092.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thierry, G., & Wu, Y. J. (2007). Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign-language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(30), 12530–12535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas-Sunesson, D., Hakuta, K., & Bialystok, E. (2018). Degree of bilingualism modifies executive control in Hispanic children in the USA. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 21(2), 197–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woumans, E., Martin, C. D., Vanden Bulcke, C., Van Assche, E., Costa, A., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Duyck, W. (2015). Can faces prime a language? Psychological Science, 26(9), 1343–1352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahodne, L. B., Schofield, P. W., Farrell, M. T., Stern, Y., & Manly, J. J. (2014). Bilingualism does not alter cognitive decline or dementia risk among Spanish-speaking immigrants. Neuropsychology, 28(2), 238–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Mishra, R.K. (2018). Conclusion. In: Bilingualism and Cognitive Control. The Bilingual Mind and Brain Book Series, vol 6. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-92513-4_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics