Skip to main content

Schools and Inequality: Implications from Seasonal Comparison Research

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Handbook of the Sociology of Education in the 21st Century

Abstract

The traditional narrative posits that differences in school quality are an important source of inequality in the stratification system. Improving the schools attended by disadvantaged children, therefore, is key to reducing inequality. But what if this view is wrong? We discuss the results of seasonal comparison studies that analyze how achievement gaps change when school is in versus out. Contrary to most education research, these studies suggest that the traditional narrative may be partly wrong in some cases and entirely misplaced in others. Indeed, when it comes to understanding socioeconomic-based gaps in math and reading skills, the evidence indicates that achievement gaps are mostly formed prior to formal schooling and that schools probably reduce the growth in gaps that we would observe in their absence. If this is correct, then the implications for battling inequality are profound. School reform efforts are likely to have limited influence; the primary source of the problem is the level of inequality in broader society.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 189.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    We are not persuaded by this conclusion because in their study children in the “school-only” condition enjoyed many benefits typically not available to children at school, such as free medical, dental, and mental health services.

  2. 2.

    Economist Eric Hanushek (1992, p. 106) explains the focus on schools: “While family inputs to education are indeed extremely important, the differential impacts of schools and teachers receive more attention when viewed from a policy viewpoint. This reflects simply that the characteristics of schools are generally more easily manipulated than what goes on in the family.”

  3. 3.

    Burkam et al. (2004) predicted summer learning (fall first grade score minus spring kindergarten score) with socioeconomic status, race, gender, age, repeat kindergarten status, family structure, home language (English or not), summer trips, summer literacy activities, computer for educational use, and summer school attendance. They explained 0.079%, 0.136%, and 0.131% of the variation in literacy, math, and general knowledge learning respectively. Clearly, the vast majority of why some children learn faster than others during the summer is not captured by the information typically available in large data sets.

  4. 4.

    It is important to recognize that with this kind of study design we do not look to the treatment period alone for our estimate of the treatment effect. We should not make the mistake, therefore, of simply observing the school-year patterns as a way of understanding how schools matter. If we just focus on the school year we would mistakenly conclude that high- and low-SES children learn at roughly the same rate, and so schools play a mostly neutral role. But the proper way to understand how schools matter is to compare the treatment (school year) period to the control period (summer). When we make that proper comparison, we learn that schools are compensatory with respect to SES-based gaps in math and reading because they reduce the magnitude of the gaps we would observe in their absence (Downey et al. 2004; Entwisle and Alexander 1992). See Downey and Condron (2016) for further discussion on this point.

  5. 5.

    We would note, however, that this issue is an awkward explanation for seasonal patterns because it needs to be applied selectively—the problem exists during the school year but not the summers.

  6. 6.

    Studying the kindergarten and elementary school years may offer an additional methodological advantage. Some evidence suggests that children learn more rapidly during these early years, about four times faster than during high school (LoGerfo et al. 2006). It is hard to know if young children actually learn faster or if this pattern is merely an artifact of the tests—early tests focus on more basic skills while later tests focus on the development of subject-specific course knowledge. Regardless of whether these patterns are real or an artifact, they have consequences for our ability to distinguish the learning “signal” from the “noise” produced by test measurement error. This issue becomes especially important when we estimate children’s summer learning rates that rely on test scores only a few months apart. Given the tests that are currently available, high school students only demonstrate modest learning gains, making it difficult to estimate learning accurately during the 9-month school year and even more difficult to confidently estimate summer patterns. In contrast, young children demonstrate much faster learning growth on currently available tests, producing a clearer picture of schools’ role.

  7. 7.

    One caveat to the general SES pattern is that NWEA extracts do not always produce consistent results. In the most extensive analysis of seasonal data sets to date, von Hippel and Hamrock (2016) compared patterns across the BSS, ECLS-K: 1998, and an NWEA extract covering 14 states and concluded that “The preschool years are the period of fastest gap growth; after school starts, it is hard to say unequivocally whether gaps grow faster during school or during summer.” This impressive analysis reinforces previous findings that most of the gap develops during the early childhood years, but raises questions about whether the SES gaps grow faster during the summers or school periods, once schooling begins. In von Hippel and Hamrock’s (2016) study, ECLS-K patterns were consistent with the notion that SES gaps grow fastest when school is out, but the patterns from the NWEA extract were at times contradictory. There are challenges interpreting the NWEA patterns, however. For example, the NWEA lacks an individual-level socioeconomic indicator, and so von Hippel and Hamrock (2016) had to compare school-level gaps across Title 1 and non-Title 1 schools. Another challenge interpreting the NWEA patterns is that various scholars typically analyze unique subsets of the larger Research Growth Database, making replication difficult.

  8. 8.

    For our purposes, it would be better if this study had estimated how the gaps increase from the beginning of kindergarten rather than first grade, but we know from other studies that the Black–White gap increases only slightly during kindergarten, and so these estimates would only increase slightly.

  9. 9.

    Confusingly, utilizing the same ECLS-K: 1998 data set, one study finds that Black students experience summer setbacks in math (Burkam et al. 2004). Nevertheless, Quinn (2015) clarifies that these contradictory findings result from variation in modeling strategy, test metric, and assumptions about measurement error. Burkam et al. (2004) explored conditional growth and found that Black students who had the same spring scores as White students made slower math gains during the summer, but overall, Black and White students learn at similar rates during the summer and there is little evidence to show that the summer period contributes to the growing Black–White gaps (Quinn 2015).

  10. 10.

    It is possible that the patterns we report here, emphasizing kindergarten and the next couple years, are unique and do not apply to later stages of the educational career. Some scholars have questioned whether seasonal patterns persist into high school, for example, where tracking mechanisms may produce greater school-based inequality (Gamoran 2016). It is worth noting, however, that prior to seasonal comparison analysis, most scholars assumed that schools increase achievement gaps, even among young children. Given that seasonal analysis reversed this view, we think it is important to refrain from making a similar mistake before we have seasonal analysis of high schoolers.

  11. 11.

    It is worth noting that when seasonal comparisons are applied to other dependent variables we also tend to come away with more favorable views of schools. For example, children’s body mass index tends to grow about twice as fast during the summer versus school year (von Hippel et al. 2007), and there does not seem to be any consistent pattern to how SES, racial/ethnic, and gender gaps in social-behavioral skills change when school is in versus out (Downey et al. 2016b).

  12. 12.

    The three demographic characteristics studied here (socioeconomic status, race/ethnicity, and gender) all produced different seasonal patterns. It is worth noting that socioeconomic status is an indicator of diverse home and neighborhood resources while gender is a socially constructed status largely uncorrelated with these non-school conditions and race/ethnicity has characteristics of both. This distinction may explain why we see the clearest seasonal patterns for socioeconomic status, the weakest for gender, and patterns somewhat in between for race/ethnicity.

  13. 13.

    We would worry, of course, about whether racial gaps would increase.

  14. 14.

    Growth models constructed with 9-month data remove summer noise and correlate only around 0.50 with traditional growth models using 12-month data, demonstrating that summer noise is a nontrivial problem (Atteberry 2011).

References

  • Alexander, K. L. (1997). Public schools and the public good. Social Forces, 76(1), 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Olson, L. S. (2007). Lasting consequences of the summer learning gap. American Sociological Review, 72(2), 167–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Olson, L. S. (2014). The long shadow: Family background, disadvantaged urban youth, and the transition to adulthood. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atteberry, A. (2011). Defining school value-added: Do schools that appear strong on one measure appear strong on another? Evanston: Society for Research on Educational Effectiveness.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borman, G., & Dowling, M. (2010). Schools and inequality: A multilevel analysis of Coleman’s equality of educational opportunity data. Teachers College Record, 112(5), 1201–1246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. (1977). Cultural reproduction and social reproduction. In J. Karabel & A. H. Halsey (Eds.), Power and ideology in education (pp. 487–511). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (1976). Schooling in capitalist America: Educational reform and the contradictions of economic life. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burkam, D. T., Ready, D. D., Lee, V. E., & LoGerfo, L. F. (2004). Social-class differences in summer learning between kindergarten and first grade: Model specification and estimation. Sociology of Education, 77(1), 1–31. Retrieved http://www.jstor.org.proxy.lib.ohio-state.edu/stable/3649401

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chatterji, M. (2006). Reading achievement gaps, correlates and moderators of early reading achievement: Evidence from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study (ECLS) kindergarten to first grade sample. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3), 489–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, J. S., et al. (1966). Equality of educational opportunity. Washington, DC: Department of Health, Education and Welfare.

    Google Scholar 

  • Condron, D. J. (2008). An early start: Skill grouping and unequal reading gains in the elementary years. The Sociological Quarterly, 49, 363–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Condron, D. J. (2009). Social class, school and non-school environments, and Black/White inequalities in children’s learning. American Sociological Review, 74(5), 685–708. Retrieved http://asr.sagepub.com/content/74/5/685

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiMaggio, P. (1982). Cultural capital and school success: The impact of status culture participation on the grades of U.S. high school students. American Sociological Review, 47(2), 189–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diprete, T. A., & Buchmann, C. (2013). The rise of women: The growing gender gap in education and what it means for American schools. CUP Services. Retrieved http://www.amazon.com/The-Rise-Women-Education-American/dp/0871540517/ref=sr_1_1?ie=UTF8&qid=1373561211&sr=8-1&keywords=the+rise+of+women

  • Dobbie, W., & Fryer, R. G. (2011). Are high quality schools enough to close the achievement gap? Evidence from a social experiment in Harlem. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 3(3), 158–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downey, D. B., & Pribesh, S. (2004). When race matters: Teachers’ evaluations of students’ classroom behavior. Sociology of Education, 77(4), 267–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Downey, D. B., & Condron, D. J. (2016). Fifty years since the Coleman report: Rethinking the relationship between schools and inequality. Sociology of Education, 89(3), 207–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Downey, D. B., von Hippel, P. T., & Broh, B. A. (2004). Are schools the great equalizer? Cognitive inequality during the summer months and the school year. American Sociological Review, 69(5), 613–635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Downey, D. B., von Hippel, P. T., & Hughes, M. (2008, July). Are “failing” schools really failing? Sociology of Education, 81, 242–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Downey, D. B., Workman, J., & von Hippel, P. (2017, August 15). Socioeconomic, racial, and gender gaps in children’s social/behavioral skills: Do they grow faster in school or out? Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3044923 or https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3044923

  • Downey, D. B., Quinn, D., & Alcaraz, M. (2017). The distribution of school quality (Working Paper).

    Google Scholar 

  • Duffett, A., Farkas, S., & Loveless, T. (2008). High-achieving students in the era of No Child Left Behind. Washington, DC. Retrieved http://www.edexcellence.net/detail/news.cfm?news_id=732&id=92

  • Duncan, G. J., & Magnuson, K. (2011). The nature and impact of early achievement skills, attention skills, and behavior problems. In G. J. Duncan & R. J. Murnane (Eds.), Whither opportunity: Rising inequality, schools, and children’s life chances (pp. 47–69). New York: The Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Entwisle, D. R., & Alexander, K. L. (1992). Summer setback: Race, poverty, school composition, and mathematics achievement in the first two years of school. American Sociological Review, 57(1), 72–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Entwisle, D. R., Alexander, K. L., & Olson, L. S. (1994). The gender gap in math: Its possible origins in neighborhood effects. American Sociological Review, 59(6), 822–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, C. S., et al. (1996). Inequality by design: Cracking the bell curve myth (1st ed.). Princeton University Press. Retrieved http://www.amazon.com/dp/0691028982

  • Fryer, R. G., & Levitt, S. D. (2004). Understanding the Black–White test score gap in the first two years of school. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 86(2), 447–464. Retrieved http://www.jstor.org/stable/3211640

  • Fryer, R. G., & Levitt, S. D. (2006). Testing for racial differences in the mental ability of young children. National Bureau of Economic Research (Working Paper). http://www. nber.org/papers/w12066

  • Gamoran, A. (2016). Gamoran comment on Downey and Condron. Sociology of Education, 89(3), 231–233. Retrieved December 21, 2016, http://soe.sagepub.com/cgi/doi/10.1177/0038040716651931

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gamoran, A., & Mare, R. D. (1989). Secondary school tracking and educational inequality: Compensation, reinforcement, or neutrality? American Journal of Sociology, 94(5), 1146–1183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gangl, M. (2010). Causal inference in sociological research. Annual Review of Sociology, 36, 21–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, B. (2010). Reversing fortunes or content change? Gender gaps in math-related skill throughout childhood. Social Science Research, 39(4), 540–569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Han, W.-J. (2008). The academic trajectories of children of immigrants and their school environments. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1572–1590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanushek, E. A. (1992). The trade-off between child quantity and quality. Journal of Political Economy, 100(1), 84–117. Retrieved January 31, 2013. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2138807.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, D. P., & Grether, J. (1983). The school year and vacations: When do students learn? Cornell Journal of Social Relations, 17, 56–71. New York City.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyns, B. (1978). Summer learning and the effects of schooling. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, A. D., & Reardon, S. F. (2011). Estimating achievement gaps from test scores reported in ordinal “proficiency” categories. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 37(4), 489–517. Retrieved April 19, 2014, http://jeb.sagepub.com/cgi/doi/10.3102/1076998611411918

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jencks, C. S. (1972). The Coleman report and the conventional wisdom. In Mosteller, F. & Moynihan, D. P. (Eds.), On equality of educational opportunity (pp. 69–115). New York: Vintage. Retrieved https://courses.utexas.edu/bbcswebdav/pid-2031893-dt-content-rid-2384509_1/xid-2384509_1

  • Jennings, J. L., Deming, D., Jencks, C., Lopuch, M., & Schueler, B. E. (2015). Do differences in school quality matter more than we thought? New evidence on educational opportunity in the twenty-first century. Sociology of Education, 88(1), 56–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kozol, J. (1991). Savage inequalities: Children in America’s schools (1st ptg). New York: Harper Perennial.

    Google Scholar 

  • LoGerfo, L. F., Nichols, A., & Reardon, S. F. (2006). Achievement gains in elementary and high school. Washington, DC: Urban Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, J. W. (2016). Meyer comment on Downey and Condron. Sociology of Education, 89(3), 227–228. Retrieved December 21, 2016, http://soe.sagepub.com/cgi/doi/10.1177/0038040716651679

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morsy, L., & Rothstein, R. (2016). Mass incarceration and children’s outcomes. Washington, DC: Economic Policy Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murnane, R. J. (1975). The impact of school resources on the learning of inner city children. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakes, J. (1985). Keeping track: How schools structure inequality (1st ed.). New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, D. (2015). Kindergarten Black–White test score gaps: Re-examining the roles of socioeconomic status and school quality with new data. Sociology of Education, 88(2), 120–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, D. M., Cooc, N., McIntyre, J., & Gomez, C. J. (2016). Seasonal dynamics of academic achievement inequality by socioeconomic status and race/ethnicity. Educational Researcher, 45(8), 443–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush, S. W., & Eschmann, R. D. (2015). Does schooling increase or reduce social inequality? Annual Review of Sociology, 41, 443–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ready, D. D., LoGerfo, L. F., Burkam, D. T., & Lee, V. E. (2005). Explaining girls’ advantage in kindergarten literacy learning: Do classroom behaviors make a difference? The Elementary School Journal, 106(1), 21–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reardon, S. F. (2011a). The widening academic achievement gap between the rich and the poor: New evidence and possible explanations (pp. 91–116). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reardon, S. F. (2011b). The widening socioeconomic status achievement gap: New evidence and possible explanations. In Whither opportunity: Rising inequality, schools, and children’s life chances (pp. 91–115). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reardon, S. F., & Galindo, C. (2009). The Hispanic–White achievement gap in math and reading in the elementary grades. American Educational Research Journal, 46(3), 853–891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reardon, S. F., Cheadle, J. E., & Robinson, J. P. (2009). The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading development in kindergarten through fifth grade. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 2(1), 45–87. Retrieved http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/19345740802539267

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothstein, R. (2004). Class and schools: Using social, economic, and educational reform to close the Black–White achievement gap. Washington, DC/New York: Economic Policy Institute/Teachers College.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thernstrom, A. M., & Thernstrom, S. (2003). No excuses: Closing the racial gap in learning. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Drehle, D. (2010). The case against summer vacation. Time.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Hippel, P. T., & Hamrock, C. (2016). Do test score gaps grow before, during, or between the school years? Measurement artifacts and what we can know in spite of them. Educational Researcher, 45(8), 443–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Hippel, P. T., Powell, B., Downey, D. B., & Rowland, N. J. (2007). The effect of school on overweight in childhood: Gain in body mass index during the school year and during summer vacation. American Journal of Public Health, 97(4), 696–702. Retrieved November 30, 2014, http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1829359&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract

  • Whitehurst, G. J. (2016). Family support of school readiness? Contrasting models of public spending on children’s early care and learning. Evidence Speaks Reports, Vol 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, A., & Merry, J. J. (2015). Understanding the role of schools in the Asian–White gap: A seasonal comparison approach. In American Sociological Association, Chicago, IL.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Douglas B. Downey .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Downey, D.B., Yoon, A., Martin, E. (2018). Schools and Inequality: Implications from Seasonal Comparison Research. In: Schneider, B. (eds) Handbook of the Sociology of Education in the 21st Century. Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-76694-2_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-76694-2_3

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-76692-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-76694-2

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics