Skip to main content

Abstract

Typically developing children are well equipped to learn from their social environment from infancy. Early learning is supported by attentional preferences for novel events, others’ goal directed actions and communication cues, as well as early maturing abilities to share attention with their social partners, imitating them, understanding their actions and emotions, and developing close connections. Additionally, typically developing children experience social interactions as inherently rewarding, and infant learning experiences enabled by these processes contribute to further learning. Early learning is disrupted in young children with Autism Spectrum Disorder due to differences in the ‘built-in’ preferences and responses that support social learning in infants and toddlers. These early abnormalities, in turn, might result in a child who is not receiving the types of learning experiences needed to stimulate the organization and specialization of the neural networks that support the development of social communication and more advanced forms of social learning. Early intervention can play a major role in decreasing these early learning disruptions in children with autism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    Importantly, the emotion processing centers of the brain do not process any novel information as ‘exciting’—some information (like a dog barking) is typically experienced as scary. While the link between attention and emotion processing centers enables a quick emotional response to new stimuli, the specific emotion experienced by the child depends by the nature of the event and by the child’s observation of the caregivers’ and other children’s response to the event.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, Fifth Edition [DSM-5]. Arlington: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baranek, G. T., Watson, L. R., Boyd, B. A., Poe, M. D., David, F. J., & McGuire, L. (2013). Hyporesponsiveness to social and nonsocial sensory stimuli in children with autism, children with developmental delays, and typically developing children. Development and Psychopathology, 25(02), 307–320.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, M. E., Freuler, A., Baranek, G. T., Watson, L. R., Poe, M. D., & Sabatino, A. (2012). Temperament and sensory features of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(11), 2271–2284.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1975). From communication to language—A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3, 255–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brand, R. J., & Shallcross, W. L. (2008). Infants prefer motionese to adult-directed action. Developmental Science, 11, 853–861.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bunzeck, N., & Duzel, E. (2006). Absolute coding of stimulus novelty in the human substantia Nigra/VTA. Neuron, 51(3), 369–379.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2007). ‘Obsessed with goals’: Functions and mechanisms of teleological interpretation of actions in humans. Acta Psychologica, 124, 60–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, N., Barquero, L., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Gore, J. C., & Anderson, A. W. (2011). Functional correlates of children’s responsiveness to intervention. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(3), 288–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G. (2008). Early behavioral intervention, brain plasticity, and the prevention of autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 775–803.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., & Bernier, R. (2007). Social brain circuitry in autism. In D. Coch, G. Dawson, & K. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Jones, E. J., Merkle, K., Venema, K., Lowy, R., Faja, S., …, Webb, S. J. (2012). Early behavioral intervention is associated with normalized brain activity in young children with autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 1550–1559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gabrieli, J. D. (2009). Dyslexia: A new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience. Science, 325(5938), 280–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. H., Grossmann, T., & Farroni, T. (2008). The social cognitive neuroscience of infancy: Illuminating the early development of social brain functions. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 36, 331–372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Is speech learning “gated” by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10, 110–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McAlister, A., & Peterson, C. (2007). A longitudinal study of child siblings and theory of mind development. Cognitive Development, 22(2), 258–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N., Scherf, K. S., Behrmann, M., & Humphreys, K. (2011). Autism as a Developmental Neurobiological Disorder: New Insights from Functional Neuroimaging. In D. Amaral, G. Dawson, & D. Geschwind (Eds.), Autism Spectrum Disorders (pp. 630–647). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P. C. (2016). Autism and joint attention: Development, neuroscience, and clinical fundamentals. Guilford Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2000). The relation of child care to cognitive and language development. Child Development, 71(4), 960–980.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2003). Does amount of time spent in child care predict socioemotional adjustment during the transition to kindergarten? Child Development, 74(4), 976–1005.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nuske, H., Vivanti, G., & Dissanayake, C. (2016). Others’ emotions teach, but not in autism: An eye-tracking pupillometry study. Molecular Autism.

    Google Scholar 

  • Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012). Putting the social into social learning: Explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 182–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2008). Mirrors in the brain. How our minds share actions and emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J., & Dawson, G. (2010). Early start Denver model for young children with autism: Promoting language, learning, and engagement. Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanefuji, W., Ohgami, H., & Hashiya, K. (2006). Preference for peers in infancy. Infant Behavior & Development, 29, 584–593.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M. (1998). The Emanuel Miller memorial lecture 1997: Change and continuity in the development of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(6), 817–827.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thioux, M., & Keysers, C. (2010). Empathy: Shared circuits and their dysfunctions. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 12, 546–552.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454–1463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Hamann, K. (2012). Collaboration in young children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vivanti, G., & Dissanayake, C. (2014). Propensity to imitate in autism is not modulated by the model’s gaze direction: An eye-tracking study. Autism Research, 7(3), 392–399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vivanti, G., & Rogers, S. J. (2014). Autism and the mirror neuron system: Insights from learning and teaching. Philosophical Transactions of the Royale Society B, 369(1644), 20130184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vivanti, G., Hocking, D., Fanning, P., & Dissanayake, C. (2016, in press) Social affiliation motives modulate spontaneous learning in Williams Syndrome but not in Autism. Molecular Autism.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–2152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williamson, R. A., Meltzoff, A. N., & Markman, E. M. (2008). Prior experiences and perceived efficacy influence 3-year-olds’ imitation. Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 275.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9(1), 11–29.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Giacomo Vivanti .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Vivanti, G., Dawson, G., Rogers, S.J. (2017). Early Learning in Autism. In: Implementing the Group-Based Early Start Denver Model for Preschoolers with Autism. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-49691-7_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics