Skip to main content

Parkinson’s Disease Without Dementia

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Research in Clinical Pragmatics

Part of the book series: Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology ((PEPRPHPS,volume 11))

Abstract

Although viewed primarily as a motor disorder, Parkinson’s disease (PD) is also associated with a variety of communication and cognitive deficits. In this chapter, we review research on pragmatic deficits in PD, as well as related cognitive processes that can contribute to those deficits. A variety of comprehension deficits have been demonstrated in PD, including deficits in the speed and accuracy with which non-literal meanings and speech acts are recognized, as well as an impaired ability to recognize emotions. These deficits overlap somewhat with various executive functions (e.g. working memory) and theory of mind abilities. Individuals with PD are also impaired in terms of language production, possibly in part because of their comprehension deficits. Major production deficits include reduced informational content, longer and more frequent pauses and associated turn-taking disruption, inappropriate levels of politeness, and deficits in various nonverbal accompaniments. The extent to which these production deficits are associated with general cognitive decline remains somewhat unclear. There is evidence that the severity of pragmatic deficits in PD is associated with greater disease severity, and that dopaminergic medication can reduce some of these deficits.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abu-Akel, A., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. (2011). Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind. Neuropsychologia, 49(11), 2971–2984.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altmann, L., & Troche, M. (2011). High-level language production in Parkinson’s disease: A review. Parkinson’s Disease, vol. 2011, Article ID 238956. doi: 10.4061/2011/238956.

  • Angwin, A., Chenery, H., Copland, D., Murdoch, B., & Silburn, P. (2005). Summation of semantic priming and complex sentence comprehension in Parkinson’s disease. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 78–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., Chiavarino, C., & Humphreys, G. W. (2004). Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: Neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(10), 1773–1784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariatti, A., Benuzzi, F., & Nichelli, P. (2008). Recognition of emotions from visual and prosodic cues in Parkinson’s disease. Neurological Sciences, 29(4), 219–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnott, W., Chenery, H., Murdoch, B., & Silburn, P. (2001). Semantic priming in Parkinson’s disease: Evidence for delayed spreading activation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(4), 502–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(7), 813–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The ‘Reading the mind in the eyes’ test revised version: A study with normal adults, adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayles, K. (1990). Language and Parkinson disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 4(3), 171–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benton, A. L. (1968). Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe disease. Neuropsychologia, 6(1), 53–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berg, E., Bjornram, C., Hartelius, L., Laakso, K., & Johnels, B. (2003). High-level language difficulties in Parkinson’s disease. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 17(1), 63–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertella, L., Albani, G., Greco, E., Priano, L., Mauro, A., Marchi, S., Bulla, D., & Semenza, C. (2002). Noun verb dissociation in Parkinson’s disease. Brain & Cognition, 48(2-3), 277–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodden, M. E., Dodel, R., & Kalle, E. (2010a). Theory of mind in Parkinson’s disease and related basal ganglia disorders: A systematic review. Movement Disorders, 25(1), 13–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bodden, M. E., Mollenhauer, B., Trenkwalder, C., Cabanel, N., Eggert, K. M., Unger, M. M., Oertel, W. H., Kessler, J., Dodel, R., & Kalbe, E. (2010b). Affective and cognitive theory of mind in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, 16(7), 466–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brand, M., Labudda, K., Kalbe, E., Hilker, R., Emmans, D., Fuchs, G., Kessler, J., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2004). Decision-making impairments in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Behavioral Neurology, 15(3-4), 77–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brookshire, R. H., & Nicholas, L. E. (1993). The discourse comprehension test. Tucson: Communication Skill Builders.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. G., & Marsden, C. D. (1988a). An investigation of the phenomenon of ‘set’ in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders, 3(2), 152–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. G., & Marsden, C. D. (1988b). Internal versus external cues and the control of attention in Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 111(2), 323–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Canter, G. J. (1963). Speech characteristics of patients with Parkinson’s disease, 1: Intensity, pitch, and duration. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 28, 221–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheang, H. S., & Pell, M. D. (2007). An acoustic investigation of Parkinsonian speech in linguistic and emotional contexts. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20(3), 221–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, U. S., Neargarder, S., & Cronin-Golomb, A. (2008). Specific impairments in the recognition of emotional facial expressions in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 46(9), 2300–2309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, M. R., Davis, M. H., Rodd, J. M., Robson, T., Ali, A., Owen, A. M., & Pickard, J. D. (2009). Towards the routine use of brain imaging to aid the clinical diagnosis of disorders of consciousness. Brain, 132(9), 2541–2552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cools, R., Barker, R., Sahakian, B., & Robbins, T. (2001). Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. Cerebral Cortex, 11(12), 1136–1143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, J. L. (1988). Intellectual impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Clinical, pathologic, and biochemical correlates. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 1(1), 24–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(4), 450–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Lau, L. M. L., Giesbergen, P. C. L. M., de Rijk, M. C., Hofman, A., Koudstaal, P. J., & Breteler, M. M. (2004). Incidence of parkinsonism and Parkinson disease in a general population: The Rotterdam study. Neurology, 63(7), 1240–1244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). The Delis-Kaplan executive function system. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1971). Intentional systems. Journal of Philosophy, 68(4), 87–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dirnberger, G., & Jahanshahi, M. (2013). Executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: A review. Journal of Neuropsychology, 7(2), 193–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Driver, J. A., Logroscino, G., Gaziano, J. M., & Kurth, T. (2009). Incidence and remaining lifetime risk of Parkinson disease in advanced age. Neurology, 72(5), 432–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dubois, B., Slachevsky, A., Litvan, I., & Pillon, B. (2000). The FAB: A frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology, 55(11), 1621–1626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test—Revised. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eddy, C. M., Beck, S. R., Mitchell, I. J., Praamstra, P., & Pall, H. S. (2013). Theory of mind deficits in Parkinson’s disease: A product of executive dysfunction? Neuropsychology, 27(1), 37–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engen, E., & Engen, T. (1983). Rhode Island test of language structure. Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filoteo, J., Friedrich, F., Rilling, L., Davis, J., Stricker, J., & Prenovitz, M. (2003). Semantic and cross-case identity priming in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(4), 441–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state.” A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, M., & Stuss, D. T. (2011). Theory of mind in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurological Sciences, 310(1–2), 225–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A., Kotz, S., Werheid, K., Hein, G., & Cramon, D. (2003). Syntactic comprehension in Parkinson’s disease: Investigating early automatic and late integrational processes using event-related brain potentials. Neuropsychology, 17(1), 133–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron, 50(4), 531–534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Ruiz, P. J., Chaudhuri, K. R., & Martinez-Martin, P. (2014). Non-motor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease: A review…from the past. Journal of Neurological Sciences, 338(1–2), 30–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A., Keysar, B., Robertson, R. R. W., & Werner, N. K. (2001). The role of suppression and enhancement in understanding metaphors. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(3), 433–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, H. M., & Tickle-Degnen, L. (2010). A meta-analysis of performance on emotion recognition tasks in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology, 24(2), 176–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Speech acts (pp. 41–58). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, S., Barnes, R., Britten, N., & Wilkinson, R. (2011). Investigating interactional competencies in Parkinson’s disease: The potential benefits of a conversation analytic approach. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 46(5), 497–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, S., Barnes, R., Britten, N., & Wilkinson, R. (2012). Potential causes and consequences of overlap in talk between speakers with Parkinson’s disease and their familiar conversation partners. Seminars in Speech and Language, 33(1), 27–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Stern, M. B., Gollomp, S., & Hurtig, H. I. (1992). Sentence comprehension in Parkinson’s disease: The role of attention and memory. Brain and Language, 42(4), 347–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, M., Kalmanson, J., Bernhardt, N., Morris, J., Stern, M. B., & Hurtig, H. I. (2000). Cognitive resource limitations during sentence comprehension in Parkinson’s disease. Brain and Language, 73(1), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, M., Zurif, E., Lee, C., Prather, P., Kalmanson, J., Stern, M., & Hurtig, H. (2002). Information processing speed and sentence comprehension in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology, 16(2), 174–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, D., Ouyand, B., Lonnquist, E., & Newcombe, J. (2011). Pragmatic communication is impaired in Parkinson’s disease. International Journal of Neuroscience, 21(5), 254–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halper, A. S., Cherney, L. R., Burns, M. S., & Mogil, S. I. (1996). Clinical management of right hemisphere dysfunction (2nd ed.). Gaithersburg: Aspen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, V. L., & Yorkston, K. M. (1996). Speech and pause characteristics following speech rate reduction in hypokinetic dysarthria. Journal of Communication Disorders, 29(6), 429–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamon, M., & Blier, P. (2013). Monoamine neurocircuitry in depression and strategies for new treatments. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 45, 54–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding the story characters thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test manual: Revised and expanded. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henry, J. D., & Crawford, J. R. (2004). Verbal fluency deficits in Parkinson’s disease: A meta-analysis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(4), 608–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, A., Iansek, R., & Bradshaw, J. (2001). Motor instability in parkinsonian speech intensity. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology, 14(2), 109–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holtgraves, T. (1998). Interpreting indirect replies. Cognitive Psychology, 37(1), 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holtgraves, T. (2010). Social psychology and language: Words, utterances and conversations. In S. Fiske, D. Gilbert & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, 5th ed., pp. 1386–1422). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holtgraves, T., & McNamara, P. (2010a). Pragmatic comprehension deficit in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(4), 388–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holtgraves, T., & McNamara, P. (2010b). Parkinson’s disease and politeness. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 29(2), 178–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holtgraves, T., Fogle, K., & Marsh, L. (2013). Pragmatic language production in Parkinson’s disease. Advances in Parkinson’s Disease, 2(1), 31–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Illes, J. (1989). Neurolinguistic features of spontaneous language production dissociate three forms of neurodegenerative disease: Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s. Brain and Language, 37(4), 628–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Illes, J., Metter, E. J., Hanson, W. R., & Iritani, S. (1988). Language production in Parkinson’s disease: Acoustic and linguistic considerations. Brain and Language, 33(1), 146–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaywant, A., & Pell, M. D. (2010). Listener impressions of speakers with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(1), 49–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jorgensen, C., Barrett, M., Huisingh, R., & Zachman, L. (1981). The WORD test: A test of expressive vocabulary and semantics. Moline: LinguiSystems, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalbe, E., Schlegel, M., Sack, A. T., Nowak, D. A., Dafotakis, M., Bangard, C., Brand, M., Shamay-Tsoory, S., Onur, O. A., & Kessler, J. (2010). Dissociating cognitive from affective theory of mind: A TMS study. Cortex, 46, 769–780.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, E., & Goodglass, H. (1983). Boston naming test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kertesz, A. (1982). The western aphasia battery. New York: Grune & Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kudlicka, A., Clare, L., & Hindle, J. (2011). Executive functions in Parkinson’s disease: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Movement Disorders, 26(13), 2305–2315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawrence, A. D., Goerendt, I. K., & Brooks, D. J. (2007). Impaired recognition of facial expressions of anger in Parkinson’s disease patients acutely withdrawn from dopamine replacement therapy. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 65–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lees, A. J., & Smith, E. (1983). Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 106(2), 257–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (2000). Presumptive meanings: The theory of generalized conversational implicature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, F. M., Lapointe, L. L., Murdoch, B. E., & Chenery, H. J. (1998). Language impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Aphasiology, 12(3), 193–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, S. J., Slabosz, A., Robbins, T. W., Barker, R. A., & Owen, A. M. (2005). Dopaminergic basis for deficits in working memory but not attentional set-shifting in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 43(6), 823–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P., Friedman, J., & Feldman, L. (1990). Syntax comprehension in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 178(6), 360–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P., Kako, E., Friedman, J., Tajchman, G., Feldman, L. S., & Jiminez, E. B. (1992). Speech production, syntax comprehension, and cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Brain and Language, 43(2), 169–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd, A. J. (1999). Comprehension of prosody in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex, 35(3), 389–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marin, R. S., Biedrzycki, R. C., & Firinciogullari, S. (1991). Reliability and validity of the apathy evaluation scale. Psychiatry Research, 38(2), 143–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mattis, S. (1988). Dementia rating scale (DRS). Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonald, C., Brown, G., & Gorell, J. (1996). Impaired set-shifting in Parkinson’s disease: New evidence from a lexical decision task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8(16), 793–809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKinlay, A., Grace, R. C., Halrymple-Alford, J. C., & Roger, D. (2009). Cognitive characteristics associated with mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 28(2), 121–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, P., & Durso, R. (2003). Pragmatic communication skills in Parkinson’s disease. Brain and Language, 84(3), 414–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, P., Holtgraves, T., Durso, R., & Harris, E. (2010). Social cognition of indirect speech: Evidence from Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23(2), 162–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N., Allcock, L., Jones, D., Noble, E., Hildreth, A. J., & Burn, D. J. (2007). Prevalence and pattern of perceived intelligibility changes in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 78(11), 1188–1190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N., Noble, E., Jones, D., Allcock, L., & Burn, D. J. (2008). How do I sound to me? Perceived changes in communication in Parkinson’s disease. Clinical Rehabilitation, 22(1), 14–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monetta, L., & Pell, M. (2007). Effects of verbal working memory deficits on metaphor comprehension in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Brain and Language, 101(1), 80–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monetta, L., Grinrod, C. M., & Pell, M. D. (2009). Irony comprehension and theory of mind deficits in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Cortex, 45(8), 972–981.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L. (2000). Spoken language production in Huntington’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(6), 1350–1366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L., & Lenz, L. (2001). Productive syntax abilities in Huntington’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Brain and Cognition, 46(1–2), 213–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L., & Rutledge, S. (2014). Reading comprehension in Parkinson’s disease. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23(2), 246–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L., & Stout, J. (1999). Discourse comprehension in Huntington’s and Parkinson’s diseases. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8(2), 137–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narme, P., Roussel, M., Mouras, H., & Duren, C. (2013). Emotional and cognitive social processes are impaired in Parkinson’s disease and are related to behavioral disorders. Neuropsychology, 27(2), 182–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owen, A. M., Roberts, A. C., Polkey, C. E., Sahi, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (1991). Extra-dimensional versus intra-dimensional set shifting performance following frontal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man. Neuropsychologia, 29(10), 993–1006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owen, A. M., James, M., Leigh, P. N., Summers, B. A., Marsden, C. D., Quinn, N. P., Lange, K. W., & Robbins, T. W. (1992). Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 115(6), 1727–1751.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papagno, C., Mattavelli, G., Cattaneo, Z., Romito, L., & Albanese, A. (2013). Ambiguous idiom processing in Parkinson’s disease patients. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 30(7–8), 495–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pell, M. D., & Leonard, C. L. (2005). Facial expression decoding in early Parkinson’s disease. Cognitive Brain Research, 23(2-3), 327–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pell, M. D., Cheang, H. S., & Leonard, C. L. (2006). The impact of Parkinson’s disease on vocal- prosodic communication from the perspective of listeners. Brain and Language, 97(2), 123–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, M. (2007). Pragmatic impairment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Péron, J., Vicente, S., Leray, E., Drapier, S., Drapier, D., Cohen, R., Biseul, I., Rouand, T., Le Jeune, F., Sauleau, P., & Vérin, M. (2009). Are dopaminergic pathways involved in theory of mind? A study in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 406–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2014). Self-, other-, and joint monitoring using forward models. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 132. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pitcairn, T. K., Clemie, S., Gray, J. M., & Pentland, B. (1990). Nonverbal cues in the self-presentation of Parkinsonism patients. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 29(2), 177–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poletti, M., Enrici, I., & Adenzato, M. (2012). Cognitive and affective theory of mind in neurodegenerative diseases: Neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neurochemical levels. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(9), 2147–2164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prutting, C. A., & Kirchner, D. M. (1987). A clinical appraisal of the pragmatic aspects of language. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 52(2), 105–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead–Reitan neuropsychological test battery: Therapy and clinical interpretation. Tucson: Neuropsychological Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roca, M., Torralva, T., Gleichgerrcht, E., Chade, A., Arevalo, G., Gershanik, O., & Manes, F. (2010). Impairments in social cognition in early medicated and unmedicated Parkinson disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 23(3), 152–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saldert, C., Ferm, U., & Bloch, S. (2014). Semantic trouble sources and their repair in conversations affected by Parkinson’s disease. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 49(6), 710–721.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saltzman, J., Strauss, E., Hunter, M., & Archibald, S. (2000). Theory of mind and executive functions in normal human aging and Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(7), 781–788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Santangelo, G., Vitale, C., Trojano, L., Errico, D., Amboni, M., Barbarulo, A., Grossi, D., & Barone, P. (2012). Neuropsychological correlates of theory of mind in patients with early Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders, 27(1), 98–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A., Jefferson, G., & Sacks, H. (1977). The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language, 53(2), 361–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 298(1089), 199–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: A lesion study. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 3054–3067.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Small, J. A., Lyons, K., & Kemper, S. (1997). Grammatical abilities in Parkinson’s disease: Evidence from written sentences. Neuropsychologia, 35(12), 1571–1576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spicer, K., Brown, G., & Gorell, J. (1994). Lexical decision in Parkinson disease: Lack of evidence for generalized bradyphrenia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16(3), 457–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(5), 640–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swainson, R., Rogers, R., Sahakian, B., Summers, B., Polkey, C., & Robbins, T. (2000). Probabilistic learning and reversal deficits in patients with Parkinson’s disease or frontal or temporal lobe lesions: Possible adverse effects of dopaminergic medication. Neuropsychologia, 38(5), 596–612.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tagliati, M., Chaudhuri, K., & Pagano, G. (2014). Prevalence of non-motor symptoms in Parkinson’s disease: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Neurology, 82(10), Supplement P2.053.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, A., Mirelman, A., Gurevich, T., Simon, E., Orr-Urtreger, A., Marder, K., Bressman, S., Giladi, N. and the LRRK2 Ashkenazi Jewish Consortium (2012). Lower cognitive performance in healthy G2019DS LRRK2 mutation carriers. Neurology, 79(10), 1027–1032.

    Google Scholar 

  • Troche, M. S., & Altmann, L. J. P. (2012). Sentence production in Parkinson’s disease: Effects of conceptual and task complexity. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33(2), 225–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vachon-Joanette, J., Tremblay, C., Langlois, M., & Monetta, L. (2013). Are non-literal language comprehension deficits related to a theory of mind deficit in Parkinson’s disease? Gériatrie et Psychologie Neuropsychiatrie du Vieillissement, 11(2), 208–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1955). Manual for the Wechsler adult intelligence scale. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiederholt, J., & Bryant, R. (2001). Gray oral reading test (4th ed.). Austin: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (1989). Test of language competence-expanded edition. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zanini, S., Tavano, A., & Fabbro, F. (2010). Spontaneous language production in bilingual Parkinson’s disease patients: Evidence of greater phonological, morphological and syntactic impairments in native language. Brain and Language, 113(2), 84–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

The writing of this chapter was partially supported by a grant (BCS-1224553) from the National Science Foundation awarded to the first author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas Holtgraves .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Holtgraves, T., Giordano, M. (2017). Parkinson’s Disease Without Dementia. In: Cummings, L. (eds) Research in Clinical Pragmatics. Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology, vol 11. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47489-2_15

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47489-2_15

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-47487-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-47489-2

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics