Skip to main content

Shifting Focus

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Experimental Psychology and Human Agency
  • 668 Accesses

Abstract

Experimental psychology tends to lose contact with the broader context of human concerns. This loss of contact is not accidental. It is an outcome of a rhetoric that preserves the status of the discipline as a natural science concerned with laws and universal regularities. What can connect the work of experimenters to a broader context is in the space of activity shared by the researchers and the research participants. This space of activity corresponds to aspects of the research method that are generally downplayed and relegated to the background. Bringing those activities into view results in (1) appreciating the place of research findings within the domain of everyday experience and (2) seeing the ambiguities of the findings and their openness to alternative interpretations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 69.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1996). ACT: A simple theory of complex cognition. American Psychologist, 51(4), 355–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Billig, M. (2013). Learn to write badly: How to succeed in the social sciences. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Blackman, L., Cromby, J., Hook, D., Papadopoulos, D., & Walkerdine, V. (2008). Creating subjectivities. Subjectivity, 22(1), 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cardoso-Leite, P., Mamassian, P., Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Waszak, F. (2010). A new look at sensory attenuation: Action-effect anticipation affects sensitivity, not response bias. Psychological Science, 21(12), 1740–1745.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen-Cole, J. (2014). The open mind: Cold War politics and the sciences of human nature. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colzato, L. S., Hommel, B., & Shapiro, K. (2010). Religion and the attentional blink: Depth of faith predicts depth of the blink. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 147.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Colzato, L. S., van Beest, I., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Scorolli, C., Dorchin, S., Meiran, N., … Hommel, B. (2010). God: Do I have your attention? Cognition, 117, 87–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1997). Naming the mind. London, UK: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, D. (2001). The second person. In Subjective, intersubjective, objective: Collected essays by Donald Davidson (Vol. 3). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2011). Why the cognitive approach in psychology would profit from a functional approach and vice versa. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(2), 202–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. New York, NY: Little Brown & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagleman, D. M. (2008). Human time perception and its illusions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(2), 131–136.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Emrich, S. M., & Ferber, S. (2012). Competition increases binding errors in visual working memory. Journal of Vision, 12(4), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eskine, K. J., Kacinik, N. A., & Prinz, J. J. (2011). A bad taste in the mouth: Gustatory disgust influences moral judgment. Psychological Science, 22, 295–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Falk, R., & Greenbaum, C. W. (1995). Significance tests die hard: The amazing persistence of a probabilistic misconception. Theory & Psychology, 5, 75–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzgerald, D., & Callard, F. (2015). Social science and neuroscience beyond interdisciplinarity: Experimental entanglements. Theory, Culture & Society, 32(1), 3–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 127–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Zahavi, D. (2007). The phenomenological mind: An introduction to philosophy of mind and cognitive science. New York, NY: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gozli, D. G. (2017). Behaviour versus performance: The veiled commitment of experimental psychology. Theory & Psychology, 27, 741–758.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gozli, D. G., Aslam, H., & Pratt, J. (2016). Visuospatial cueing by self-caused features: Orienting of attention and action-outcome associative learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 459–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gozli, D. G., & Deng, W. (2018). Building blocks of psychology: On remaking the unkept promises of early schools. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 52, 1–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasson, U., Chen, J., & Honey, C. J. (2015). Hierarchical process memory: Memory as an integral component of information processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 304–313.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hibberd, F. (2014). The metaphysical basis of a process psychology. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 34(3), 161–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hibberd, F. J., & Gozli, D. G. (2017). Psychology’s fragmentation and neglect of foundational assumptions: An interview with Fiona J. Hibberd. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 13, 366–374.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Scorolli, C., Borghi, A. M., & van den Wildenberg, W. P. M. (2011). Religion and action control: Faith-specific modulation of the Simon effect but not stop-signal performance. Cognition, 120, 177–185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kominsky, J. F., Strickland, B., Wertz, A. E., Elsner, C., Wynn, K., & Keil, F. C. (2017). Categories and constraints in causal perception. Psychological Science, 28, 1649–1662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kukla, A. (2001). Methods of theoretical psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Marken, R. S., & Carey, T. A. (2015). Controlling people: The paradoxical nature of being human. Samford Valley, Australia: Australian Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Memelink, J., & Hommel, B. (2013). Intentional weighting: A basic principle in cognitive control. Psychological Research, 77, 249–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality: Principles and implication of cognitive psychology. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1992). Précis of unified theories of cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 425–492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nie, Q. Y., Müller, H. J., & Conci, M. (2017). Hierarchical organization in visual working memory: From global ensemble to individual object structure. Cognition, 159, 85–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), aac4716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paglieri, F., Borghi, A. M., Colzato, L. S., Hommel, B., & Scorolli, C. (2013). Heaven can wait: How religion modulates temporal discounting. Psychological Research, 77, 738–747.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child (M. Cook, Trans.). New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, J. R., & Portillo, M. C. (2011). Grouping and emergent features in vision: Toward a theory of basic Gestalts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1331–1349.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (1995). The construction of social reality. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2016). On the necessity of distinguishing between unintentional and intentional mind wandering. Psychological Science, 27(5), 685–691.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sellars, W. (1963). Science, perception, and reality. Austin, TX: Ridgeview Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smaldino, P. E., & McElreath, R. (2016). The natural selection of bad science. Royal Society Open Science, 3(9), 160384.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (1997). The structure of psychological common sense. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Summerfield, C., & Egner, T. (2009). Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 403–409.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teo, T. (2018). Outline of theoretical psychology: Critical investigations. London, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Thomson, J. J. (2008). Normativity. Chicago, IL: Open Court.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willett, A. B., Marken, R. S., Parker, M. G., & Mansell, W. (2017). Control blindness: Why people can make incorrect inferences about the intentions of others. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79, 841–849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, J., Al-Aidroos, N., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2013). Attention is spontaneously biased toward regularities. Psychological Science, 24(5), 667–677.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313, 1451–1452.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Gozli, D. (2019). Shifting Focus. In: Experimental Psychology and Human Agency. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-20422-8_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics