Skip to main content

Correlated Hypothesizing and the Distinction between Contingency-Shaped and Rule-Governed Behavior

  • Chapter
Rule-Governed Behavior

Abstract

Within our culture, references to consciousness, rational thought, and the use of knowledge are taken as straightforward descriptions of basic human functioning. Within psychology, there has been an enduring controversy over whether such terms qualify as basic explanatory terms, or whether they are misleading intrusions from ordinary language. On one hand, cognitivists have conformed to common usage, encorporating “knowledge,” “awareness,” and other terms of mentalistic origin into the groundwork of their theories. These concepts are said to explain why humans (and sometimes animals as well) behave as they do. On the other hand, behavioral traditions have run counter to these patterns. Many behaviorists have asserted that terms of mentalistic origin are inappropriate to a scientific account.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Adams, J. (1957). Laboratory studies of behavior without awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 54, 383–405.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1980). Cognitive psychology and its implications. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman

    Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (1974). On two types of deviation from the matching law: Bias and undermatching. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 231–242.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bern, S. L. (1967). Verbal self-control: The establishment of effective self-instruction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 485–491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentall, R. P., & Lowe, C. F. (1987). The role of verbal behavior in human learning: III. Instructional effects in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 177–190.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bentall, R. P., Lowe, C. F., & Beasty, A. (1985). The role of verbal behavior in human learning: II. Developmental differences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 165–181.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Black, A. H. (1971). The direct control of neural processes by reward and punishment. The American Scientist, 59, 236–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boren, M. C. P. (1973). Fixed-ratio and variable-ratio schedules of brief stimuli in second-order schedules of matching to sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 20, 219–234.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G., & Trobasso, T. (1964). Concept identification. In R. C. Atkinson (Ed.), Studies in mathematical psychology (pp. 32–96). Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, W. F. (1975). There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in adult humans. In W. B. Weimer & D. S. Palermo (Eds.), Cognitive and symbolic processes (pp. 1–56). Hillsdale, NJ.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P. L., & Jenkins, H. M. (1968). Auto-shaping of the pigeon’s key-peck. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 1–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, A., & Revusky, S. H. (1961). Collateral behavior in humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 349–350.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1973). The concept of the operant in the analysis of behavior. Behaviorism, 1, 103–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C., Matthews, B. A., & Shimoff, E. (1982). Instructed versus shaped human verbal behavior: Interactions with nonverbal responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 38, 233–248.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Centers, R. (1963). A laboratory adaptation of the conversational procedure for the conditioning of verbal operants. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 334–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cumming, W. W., & Berryman, R. (1965). The complex discriminated operant: Studies of matching-to-sample and related problems. In D. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization (pp. 284–330). Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1959). Verbal Behavior, by B. F. Skinner. Language, 35, 26–58. Reprinted as item A-34 in the Bobbs-Merrill Reprint Series in the Social Sciences,

    Google Scholar 

  • and in J. A. Fodor and J. J. Katz, (1964) The Structures of language: readings in the philosophy of Language. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, N. A., & Osborne, J. G. (1974). Fixed-ratio and fixed-interval schedule control of matching-to-sample errors by children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 27–36.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1981). Selfish genes and selfish memes. In D. R. Hofstadter & D. C. Dennett (Eds.), The mind’s I: Fantasies and reflections on self and soul (pp. 124–144). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1982). The extended phenotype. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeNike, L. D. (1964). The temporal relationship between awareness and performance in verbal conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 521–529.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dember, W. N. (1974). Motivation and the cognitive revolution. American Psychologist, 29, 161–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devany, J. M., Hayes, S. C., & Nelson, R. O. (1986). Equivalence class formation in language-able and language-disabled children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243–257.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dulany, D. E. (1961). Hypotheses and habits in verbal “operant conditioning.” Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 63 (2), 251–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review, 87, 215–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K., & Skinner, B. F. (1941). Some quantitative properties of anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 390–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falk, J. L. (1966). Schedule-induced polydipsia as a function of fixed-interval length. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 37–39.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B. (1960). Intermittent reinforcement of matching to sample in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 3, 259–272.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1971). First discussant’s comments: What is memory development the development of? Human Development, 14, 272–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1981). The mind-body problem. Scientific American, 244, 114–123.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenspoon, J. (1955). The reinforcing effect of two spoken sounds on the frequency of two responses. American Journal of Psychology, 68, 409–416.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenspoon, J. (1963). Reply to Spielberger and DeNike: “Operant conditioning of plural nouns: A failure to replicate the Greenspoon effect.” Psychological Reports, 12, 29–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guevremont, D. C., Osnes, P. G., & Stokes, T. F. (1988). The functional role of preschoolers’ verbalizations in the generalization of self-instructional training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, 45–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. (1959). Learning set and error factor theory. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science. Vol. 2. General systematic formulations; learning and special processes (pp. 492–537). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, L., & Shimp, C. P. (1974). Choice between response rates. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 109–115.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C. (1986). The case of the silent dog—verbal reports and the analysis of rules: A review of Ericsson and Simon’s Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 351–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Brownstein, A. J., Haas, J. R., & Greenway, D. E. (1986). Instructions, multiple schedules, and extinction: Distinguishing rule-governed from schedule-controlled behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 137–147.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hefferline, R. F., Keenan, B., & Harford, R. A. (1959). Escape and avoidance conditioning in human subjects without their observation of the response. Science, 130, 1338–1339.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hefferline, R. F., & Pererra, T. B. (1963). Proprioceptive discrimination of a covert operant without its observation by the subject. Science, 139, 834–835.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J. (1979). Acquisition, generalization, and discrimination reversal of a natural concept. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 5, 116–129.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hineline, P. N. (1983). When we speak of knowing. The Behavior Analyst, 6, 183–186.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hineline, P. N. (1984). What, then, is Skinner’s operationism? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hineline, P. N. (1986). Re-tuning the operant-respondent distinction. In Travis Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 55–79). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, H. H., & Spence, J. T. (Eds.). (1971). Essays in neobehaviorism: A memorial volume to Kenneth W. Spence. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237, 1445–1452.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kluwe, R. H. (1982). Cognitive knowledge and executive control: Metacognition. In D. F. Griffin (Ed.), Animal mind-human mind (pp. 201–224). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krechevsky, I. (1932). “Hypotheses” in rats. Psychological Review, 39, 516–532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lacey, H. M., & Rachlin, H. (1978). Behavior, cognition and theories of choice. Behaviorism, 6, 177–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamarre, J., & Holland, J. G. (1985). The functional independence of mands and tacts. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 5–19.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laties, V. G., & Weiss, B. (1963). Effects of a concurrent task on fixed-interval responding in humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 431–436.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laties, V. G., Weiss, B., Clark, R. L., & Reynolds, M. D. (1965). Overt “mediating” behavior during temporally spaced responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 8, 107–116.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A. (1975). Reinforcement contingency as discriminative stimulus. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 241–246.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laurenti-Lions, L., Gallego, J., Chambille, B., Vardon, G., & Jacquemin, C. (1985). Control of myoelectrical responses through reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 44, 185–193.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, M. (1966). Hypothesis behavior by humans during discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 331–338.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, M. (1975). A cognitive theory of learning. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action (and related commentary). Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 529–566.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd, K. E. (1980). Do as I say; not as I do. New Zealand Psychologist, 9, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, C. F. (1979). Determinants of human operant behavior. In M. D. Zeiler and P. Harzern (Eds.), Advances in analysis of behaviour (Vol. 1): Reinforcement and the organization of behaviour (pp. 159–192). Chichester and New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, C. F., Beasty, A., & Bentall, R. P. (1983). The role of verbal behavior in human learning: Infant performance on fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 39, 157–164.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacCorquodale, K. (1970). On Chomsky’s review of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 83–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacCorquodale, K., & Meehl, P. E. (1948). On a distinction between hypothetical constructs and intervening variables. Psychological Review, 55, 95–107.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, B. A., Catania, A. C., & Shimoff, E. (1985). Effects of uninstructed verbal behavior on nonverbal responding: Contingency descriptions versus performance descriptions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 155–164.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Millenson, J. R., & Leslie, J. (1979). Principles of behavioral analysis (2nd ed.). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, E. K., Higgins, S. T., & Bickel, W. K. (1982). Comments on cognitive science in the experimental analysis of behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 5, 109–125.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neuringer, A. (1986). Can people behave “randomly?”: The role of feedback. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 62–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nevin, J. A., & Liebold, K. (1966). Stimulus control of matching and oddity in a pigeon. Psychonomic Science, 5, 351–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. C. (1977). Telling more than we know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1969). Memory and attention: An introduction to human information processing. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orne, M. T. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. American Psychologist, 17, 776–783.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (1973). Reductionism and the nature of psychology. Cognition, 2, 131–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, W. K., & Clark, D. B. (1976). A comparison of the key-peck and treadle-press operants in the pigeon: Differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate schedule of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 26, 237–256.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers-Warren, A., & Baer, D. M. (1976). Correspondence between saying and doing: Teaching children to share and praise. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9, 335–354.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld, J. P., Rudell, A. P., & Fox, S. S. (1969). Operant control of neural events in humans. Science, 165, 821–823.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salzinger, K. (1986). Cognitive problems, behavioral solutions. Paper presented at the Behaviorists’ European Summer Academy, Bad Kreuznach, Germany, June 1986.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salzinger, K., & Pisoni, S. (1960). Reinforcement of verbal affect responses of normal subjects during the interview. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 127–130.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schnaitter, R. (1978). Private causes. Behaviorism, 6, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, B. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–703.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shimoff, E., Matthews, B. A., & Catania, A. C. (1986). Human operant performance: Sensitivity and pseudosensitivity to contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 149–157.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shimoff, E., Catania, A. C., & Matthews, B. A. (1981). Uninstructed human responding: Sensitivity of low-rate performance to schedule contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 207–218.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shimp, C. P. (1970). The concurrent reinforcement of two interresponae times: Absolute rate of reinforcment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 1–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shimp, C. P. (1983). The local organization of behavior: Dissociations between a pigeon’s behavior and self-reports of that behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 39, 61–68

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shinkman, P. G., Bruce, C. J., & Pfingst, B. E. (1974). Operant conditioning of single-unit response patterns in visual cortex. Science, 184, 1194–1196.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 213–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., Rauzin, R., Lazar, R., Cunningham, S., Tailby, W., & Carrigan, P. (1982). A search for symmetry in the conditional discriminations of rhesus monkeys, baboons, and children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 23–44.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1983). How knowledge influences learning. American Scientist, 71, 631–638.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1931). The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. Journal of General Psychology, 5, 427–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1935). The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. Journal of General Psychology, 12, 40–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1945). The operational analysis of psychological terms. Psychological Review, 52, 270–277, 291–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review, 57, 193–216.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1961). Why we need teaching machines. Harvard Educational Review, 31, 377–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1963). Behaviorism at fifty. Science, 140, 951–958.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1968). The technology of teaching. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1977). Why I am not a cognitive psychologist. Behaviorism, 5, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213, 501–504.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T. L. (1986). Biology as allegory: A review of Elliott Sober’s The nature of selection. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 105–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sober, E. (1984). The nature of selection: Evolutionary theory in philosophical focus. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D., & DeNike, L. D. (1966). Descriptive behaviorism versus cognitive theory in verbal operant conditioning. Psychological Review, 73, 306–326.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stubbs, A. (1968). The discrimination of stimulus duration by pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 223–238.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Touchette, P. E. (1971). Transfer of stimulus control: Measuring the moment of transfer. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 15, 347–354.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan, W., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1987). Choosing among natural stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 5–16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weisberg, R. W. (1980). Memory, thought, and behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. P., & Keller, F. S. (1953). On the selective reinforcement of spaced responses. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 46, 190–193.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zettle, R. D., & Hayes, S. C. (1982). Rule-governed behavior: A potential theoretical framework for cognitive-behavioral therapy. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy, volume I (pp. 73–118). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hineline, P.N., Wanchisen, B.A. (1989). Correlated Hypothesizing and the Distinction between Contingency-Shaped and Rule-Governed Behavior. In: Hayes, S.C. (eds) Rule-Governed Behavior. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-0447-1_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-0447-1_7

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4757-0449-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4757-0447-1

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics