Skip to main content

The Partial Noncommunication of Culture to Autistic Children—An Application of Human Ethology

  • Chapter
Autism

Abstract

Autistic children’s incompetence in the use of language and symbols has attracted much attention in recent years. Fifty percent of autistic children are mute, and others show characteristic peculiarities of language such as reversal of the pronouns “I” and “you,” echolalia, repetitive and out of context speech, few questions or spontaneous utterances, and the idiosyncratic use of words. As Kanner (1943) remarked, they rarely use language to communicate. Rutter (1974) has found that nearly all autistic children show language peculiarities and retardation and in fact he suggests this as a diagnostic criterion.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Brazelton, T. B., Kaslowski, B., & Main, M. The origins of reciprocity: The early mother infant interaction. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.),The effect of the infant on its caregiver. New York: Wiley Interscience, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. From communication to language—a psychological perspective. Cognition, 1975, 3(3), 255–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Churchill, D. W. The effects of success and failure in psychotic children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1971,25, 208–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clancy, H., & McBride, G. The autistic process and its treatment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1969,10, 233–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, R. Encounter with anthropology. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovitch, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. The impact of the concept of culture on the concept of man. In J. R. Piatt (Ed.), New views on the nature of man. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1965.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. The rise of anthropological theory—a history of theories of culture. New York: Cowell, 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hütt, C., & Vaizey, M. J. Differential effects of group density on social behaviour. Nature, 1966,209, 1371–1372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 1943,2, 217–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. Follow up study of eleven autistic children originally reported in 1943. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 1911,1, 119–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. Some functions of gaze-direction in social interaction. Psychologica, 1967, 26, 22–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newson, J., & Newson, E. Intersubjectivity and the transmission of culture: On the social origins of symbolic functioning. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 1975,28, 437–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richer, J. M. The social avoidance behaviour of autistic children. Animal Behaviour, 1976, 24, 898–906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richer, J. M. The self perpetuation of autism in a normal social environment. Submitted for publication, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richer, J. M., & Coss, R. G. Gaze aversion in autistic and normal children.Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 1976,55, 193–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richer, J. M., & Richards, B. Reacting to autistic children: The danger of trying too hard. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1915,127, 526–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ricks, D., & Wing, L. Language, communication, and the use of symbols in normal and autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 1975,5, 191–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruttenberg, B. A. A psychoanalytic understanding of infantile autism and its treatment. In D. W. Churchill, G. D. Alpern, & M. K. DeMeyer (Eds.), Infantile autism. Springfield, Ill.: Charles C Thomas, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. The development of infantile autism. Psychological Medicine, 1974,4, 147–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bartak, L., & Newman, S. Autism—A central disorder of cognition and language. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Infantile autism: Concepts, characteristics and treatment. London: Churchill-Livingstone, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. A micro-analysis of mother-infant interaction: Behavior regulating social contact between a mother and her 31/2 month old twins. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 1911,10, 501–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. Mother and infant at play: The dyadic interaction involving facial, vocal, and gaze behaviours. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregivers. New York: Wiley Interscience, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L. What is an autistic child? London: National Society for Autistic Children, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1978 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Richer, J. (1978). The Partial Noncommunication of Culture to Autistic Children—An Application of Human Ethology. In: Rutter, M., Schopler, E. (eds) Autism. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-0787-7_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-0787-7_3

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4684-0789-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4684-0787-7

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics