Skip to main content

Modularity of Sequence Learning Systems in Humans

  • Chapter
Neural Representation of Temporal Patterns

Abstract

Humans excel at a variety of learned and highly skilled activities in which complex sequential behavior is distributed over time. The major theme of this chapter concerns the hypothesis that sequence learning and production of sequences of activities involves not a single function, but rather is made up of multiple components. For example, in playing a piano, pitch is mapped to key position and key position is mapped to the motor system for bringing the arms, hands, and fingers to the keys. In addition to this spatial mapping, the pianist must learn the sequence of notes or keys that correspond to a piece of music. The sequential representation must indicate not only which note or key is next in a series, but must also specify the intervals at which the keys should be hit and with what intensity. In other activities, dancing for example, trajectory through space, and not just the target of movement, must be specified. It is likely that some of these functions are independent of one another, both in the psychological sense that one function can be affected with minimal or no influence on another, and in a neurobiological sense in that they depend on different brain regions. This chapter will focus on a selected aspect of skill, the representation of learned sequences, and will consider only those representations that specify the succession of events. One of the issues to be addressed is the relationship between the representation of a sequence and the motor system that actually produces the sequence. Evidence will be presented that sequence representation is relatively abstract and independent of the implementation system. A second line of evidence to be presented suggests that the sequential representation itself has constituent parts or modules.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Alexander, G. E., Crutcher, M. D., and DeLong, M. R., 1990, Basal ganglia thalamo-cortical circuits: Parallel substrates for motor control, oculomotor, “prefrontal” and “limbic” functions, Prog. Brain Res., 85:119.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berkenblit, M. B., and Feldman, A. G., 1988, Some problems of motor control, J. Motor Behav., 20:369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, D.,1994, Implicit Learning: Twenty five years on. A tutorial, in: “Attention and Performance XV: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing”, C. Umiltë and M. Moscovitch, eds., MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cleeremans, A., 1993a, “Mechanisms of Implicit Learning: Connectionist Models of Sequence Processing”, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cleeremans, A., 1993b, Attention and awareness in sequence learning, in: “Proceedings of the 15th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society”, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cleeremans, A. and McClelland, J. L., 1991, Learning the structure of event sequences, J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen., 120:235.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A., and Curran, T., 1993, On tasks, knowledge, correlations, and dissociations: Comment on Perruchet and Amorim. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19:1431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A., Ivry, R. I., and Keele, S. W., 1990, Attention and structure in sequence learning, J. Exp. Psychol.: Learning, Memory, and, Cognition, 16:17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curran, T., and Keele, S. W., 1993, Attentional and nonattentional forms of sequence learning, J. Exp. Psychol.: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19:189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dienes, Z., Broadbent, D., and Berry, D., 1991, Implicit and explicit knowledge bases in artificial grammar learning, J. Exp. Psychol: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17:875.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J. L., 1990, Finding structure in time, Cognit. Sci., 14:179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferraro, F. R., Balota, D. A., and Connor, L. T., 1993, Implicit memory and the formation of new associations in nondemented Parkinson’s disease individuals and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type: A serial reaction time (SRT) investigation, Brain and Cognition, 21:163.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frensch, P. A., Buchner, A., and Lin, J., 1994, Implicit learning of unique and ambiguous serial transactions in the presence and absence of a distractor task, J. Exp. Psychol.: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20:567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frensch, P. A., and Miner, C. S., 1994,Effects of presentation rate and individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning, Memory and Cognition, 5:95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, J. M., 1993, Frontal lobes, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 3:160.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman-Rakic, P. S., 1990, Cellular and circuit basis of working memory in prefrontal cortex of nonhuman primates, Prog. Brain Res., 5:325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez, L. J., and Heilman, K. M., 1985, Ideomotor apraxia: Gestural discrimination, comprehension and memory, in: “Neuropsychological Studies of Aapraxia”, E. A. Roy, ed., North Holland Publishers, New York, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodale, M. A., 1993, Visual pathways supporting perception and action in the primate cerebral cortex, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 3:578.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, P. C., and Meyer, D. E., 1987, Control of serial order in rapidly spoken syllable sequences, J. Memory Language, 26:300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., and Ivry, R., 1995, Functional mapping of sequence learning in normal humans, J. Cognit. Neurosci., in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P. M., 1991, Language, tools and brain: The ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized behavior, Behav. Brain Sci., 14:531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J., and Valenstein, E., 1982, Two forms of ideomotor apraxia, Neurology, 32:342.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hillis, A. E., and Caramazza, A., 1988, The graphemic buffer and attentional mechanisms, Report no. 30, Cognitive Neuropsychology Laboratory, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ivry, R. I. and Gopal, H. S., 1992, Speech production and perception in patients with cerebellar lesions, in: “Attention and Performance XIII”, D. Meyer and S. Kornblum, eds., MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ivry, R. I. and Keele, S. W., 1989, Timing functions of the cerebellum, Cognit. Neurosci., 1:134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, G., and Jackson, S., 1992, Sequence structure and sequential learning: The evidence from aging reconsidered, Technical Report No. 92–9, Institute of Cognitive and Decision Sciences, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, S. R., Jackson, G. M., Harrison, J., Henderson, L., and Kennard, C., 1995, Serial reaction time learning and Parkinson’s disease: Evidence for a procedural learning deficit, Neuropsychologia, 33:577.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, M. I., 1986, Serial order: A parallel distributed processing approach, ICS Report 8604, Institute for Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, M. I., 1995, The organization of action sequences: Evidence from a relearning task. J. Motor Behav. 27, 179–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. W., Cohen, A., and Ivry, R. I., 1990, Motor programs: Concepts and issues, in: “Attention and Performance XIII”, M. Jeannerod, ed., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. W., and Ivry, R., 1990, Does the cerebellum provide a common computation for diverse tasks: A timing hypothesis, in: “The Development and Neural Bases of Higher Cognitive Function”, A. Diamond, ed., Ann. NY Acad. Sci., 608:179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. W., and Jennings, P., 1992, Attention in the representation of sequence: Experiment and theory, Human Movement Science, 11:125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. W., Jennings, P., Jones, S., Caulton, D., and Cohen, A., 1995, On the modularity of sequence representations, J. Motor Behav. 27:17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knopman, D., and Nissen, M. J., 1991, Procedural learning is impaired in Huntington’s disease: Evidence from the serial reaction time task, Neuropsychologia, 29:245.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lavond, D. G., Lincoln, J. S., McCormick, D. A., and Thompson, R. F., 1984, Effects of bilateral lesions of the dentate and interpositus nuclei on conditioning of heart-rate and nictitating membrane/eyelid responses in the rabbit, Brain Res., 305:323.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G., 1982, The problem of flexibility and fluency in skilled behavior, Psychol. Rev., 89:483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G., 1987, “The Organization of Perception and Action”, Springer-Verlag, New York, NY.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U., 1994, “Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences, Technical Report. 94–13, Institute of Cognitive and Decision Sciences, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesulam, M.-M., 1985, Patterns in behavioral neuroanatomy: Association areas, the limbic system, and hemispheric specialization, in: “Principles of Behavioral Neurology”, M.-M. Mesulam, ed., F. A. Davis Company, Philadelphia, PA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesulam, M.-M., 1990, Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory, Ann. Neurol., 28:597.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. J., and Bullemer, P., 1987, Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures, Cognit. Psychol., 19:1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. J., Knopman, D. S., and Schacter, D. L., 1987, Neurochemical dissociation of memory systems, Neurology, 37:789.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. J., Willingham, D., and Hartman, M., 1989, Explicit and implicit remembering: When is learning preserved in amnesia, Neuropsychologia, 27:341.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Clark, K., Stewart, M., Massaquoi, S., Lou, J.-S., and Hallett, M., 1993, Procedural learning in Parkinson’s disease and cerebellar degeneration, Ann. Neurol., 34:594.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Passingham, R. E., 1993, “The Frontal Lobes and Voluntary Action”, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Perrett, S. P., Ruiz, B. P., and Mauk, M. D., 1993, Cerebellar cortex lesions disrupt learning-dependent timing of conditioned eyelid responses, J.Neurosci., 13:1708.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Perruchet, P., and Amorim, M., 1992, Conscious knowledge and changes in performance in sequence learning: Evidence against dissociation, J. Exp. Psychol.: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18:785.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Povel, D. J., and Collard, R., 1982, Structural factors in patterned finger tapping, Acta Psychol., 52:107.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Raibert, M. H., 1977, Motor control and learning by the state space model, Technical Report AI-M-351, NTIS AD-A026-960, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S., 1989, Implicit learning and tacit knowledge, J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen., 118:219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Restle, F., and Burnside, B. L., 1972, Tracking of serial patterns, J. Exp. Psychol., 95:299.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Gentilucci, M., 1988, Motor and visual-motor functions of the premotor cortex, in: “Neurobiology of Neocortex”, P. Rakic and W. Singer, eds., Wiley, New York, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., 1987, Successive approximations to a model of human motor programming, in: “The Psychology of Learning and Motivation”, G. Bower, ed., Academic Press, New York, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., 1976, The evolution of intelligence and access to the cognitive unconscious, in: “Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology”, J. M. Sprague and A. N. Epstein, eds., Academic Press, New York, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanks, D. R., and St. John, M. F., 1994, Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems, Behav. Brain Sci., 17:367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stadler, M. A., 1989, On the learning of complex procedural knowledge, J. Exp. Psychol.: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15:1061.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stadler, M. A., 1992, Statistical structure and implicit serial learning, J. Exp. Psychol.: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18:318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stadler, M. A., 1993, Implicit serial learning: Questions inspired by Hebb (1961), Memory and Cognition, 21:819.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stadler, M. A., 1995, The role of attention in implicit learning, J. Exp. Psychol.: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21:674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R. F., 1986, The neurobiology of learning and memory, Science, 233:941.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R. F., 1990, Neural mechanisms of classical conditioning in mammals, Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B329:161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willingham, D. B., 1992, Systems of motor skill, in: “Neuropsychology of Memory”, L. R. Squire and N. Butters, eds., The Guilford Press, New York, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willingham, D. B., Greenley, D. B., and Bardona, A. M., 1993, Dissociation in a serial response time task using a recognition measure: Comment on Perruchet and Amorim (1992), J. Exp. Psychol.: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19:1424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willingham, D. B., and Koroshetz, W. J., 1993, Evidence for dissociable motor skills in Huntington’s disease patients, Psychobiol., 21:173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willingham, D. B., Nissen, M. J., and Bullemer, P., 1989, On the development of procedural knowledge, J. Exp.Psychol.: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15:1047.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, C. E., 1990, Generalized motor programs: Reexamining claims of effector independence in writing, in: “Attention and Performance XIII”, M. Jeannerod, ed., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, C. E. and Lindemann, P., 1993, Effector independence in hierarchically structured motor programs for handwriting. Presented at 34th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1995 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Keele, S.W., Curran, T. (1995). Modularity of Sequence Learning Systems in Humans. In: Covey, E., Hawkins, H.L., Port, R.F. (eds) Neural Representation of Temporal Patterns. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-1919-5_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-1919-5_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-5785-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-1919-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics