Skip to main content

Completing the Psychobiological Architecture of Human Personality Development: Temperament, Character, and Coherence

  • Chapter
Understanding Human Development

Abstract

Three models of personality and its development are described in terms of their psychobiological mechanisms. Personality is the organization within the individual of the psychobiological processes by which we adapt to experience. Personality as temperament involves individual differences in heritable traits that influence the salience of stimuli to which we attend selectively. Temperament is superseded by a model of self that includes temperament and character. The organization of self is described by three character dimensions, which correspond to executive, legislative, and judicial functions. The development of these higher cortical functions is experience-dependent and influenced by social and cultural learning. Temperament and character together can account for some of the self-organizing characteristics of personality development but are incomplete models of human personality and intellectual development. They cannot explain uniquely human characteristics, such as creativity, freedom of will, or spirituality, which involve individual differences in personality coherence. Personality coherence refers to mind as a complex adaptive system functioning as a unified whole, much like the quantum coherence of superfluids. This hierarchy of models is discussed in relation to Baltes’s (1997) hypothesis about the incompleteness of human ontogeny when limited to its first two levels.

Paul Baltes (1997) has observed that genetic and social-cultural influences on human intellectual development do not complete the task of maintaining a positive balance of gains over losses across the lifespan. As we age, gene-environment interactions cause decreased flexibility, plasticity, and learning efficiency, as well as problems in the brain mechanics of basic information processing that cannot be effectively overcome by greater social-cultural compensation. Many people become intellectually impaired, forgetful, or even demented, and everyone eventually dies.

Baltes has carefully distinguished between two main components of intellectual function—the fluid mechanics and the crystallized pragmatics. The fluid cognitive mechanics refers to the basic “hardware” or brain mechanisms that are largely content-free, universal, biological, and susceptible to genetic differences in inheritance and development. The crystallized pragmatics are largely content-rich, culture-dependent, and experience-based “software” by which the hardware is directed and organized.

Accordingly, Baltes’s dual-process model of intellectual development is directly comparable to my own work on personality development in which I have distinguished between temperament and character. I have defined temperament in terms of heritable individual differences in basic information processing of emotional responses to simple stimuli, which is comparable to fluid mechanics of intelligence. Differences in temperament are heritable, developmentally stable throughout life, and little influenced by social and cultural influences. In contrast, character is what individuals make of themselves intentionally in terms of goals and values. Character development is experience-dependent, influenced by social and cultural influences, and can initially increase with age, particularly from puberty to early adulthood and little thereafter under commonplace conditions (Cloninger et al., 1993, 1997). Hence temperament and character are comparable to fluid and crystallized intelligence, respectively. This suggestion is supported by the work of Staudinger and Pasupathi (2000) to extend the dual-process model of intelligence to understand wisdom by incorporating information about personality, emotion, and motivation as well as experiential contexts.

The incompleteness of character is revealed in the difficulty of treating patients with character disorders. Despite strenuous efforts with pharmacological and psychotherapeutic interventions, the results of therapy for character disorders often show little benefit. This underscores the incompleteness of the theory underlying cognitive-behavioral therapy because it shows that cognitive schemas are rarely changed in therapy in a fundamental way. Individual differences in character can also be described as individual differences in the schemas or initial perspectives by which we organize our responses to experience. Most therapy for personality disorders does not efficiently change measures of character traits or cognitive schemas. Patients are usually left to struggle throughout their lifespan in an effort to balance emotional conflicts in such a way that personal and social gains will exceed losses (Beck, 1996).

Recent experimental and clinical findings regarding the importance of intuitive aspects of cognitive function suggest that this incompleteness is a consequence of limiting awareness only to temperament and character. Specifically, in the domain in which intellect and personality merge, Baltes and his colleagues (Baltes, 1997; Baltes & Smith, 1990) have studied measures of wisdom and have found that Baltes’s measures do not increase with age. However, Baltes has measured wisdom as an advanced form of crystallized intelligence within his dual-process model. As a consequence, wisdom involves a form of intellectual expertise for Baltes (Chandler & Holliday, 1990), whereas others have noted that wisdom has an intuitive, holistic, integrative mechanism that is distinct from the algorithmic logic of crystallized intelligence (Labouvie-Vief, 1990; Sternberg, 1990; see also Labouvie-Vief, this volume). Furthermore, experimental studies have shown that algorithmic models of crystallized intelligence, which involve verbatim statements in memory, provide an inadequate description of cognitive growth and development (Bjorklund, 1997). They do not account well for the facts of cognitive growth, such as how children learn intuitively in play, how forgetting and reminiscence occur, or how true memories are distinguished from false memories (see also Wellman, this volume). This has led to the dissociation of intuitive learning of the gist of things from the rational learning of verbatim memories (Reyna & Brainerd, 1998).

Likewise, in the personality domain, there is a third level of personality integrative function that is uniquely human and involves the nonalgorithmic, intuitive processing that underlies human creativity, inventiveness, and integrative spirituality. It is my suggestion that this integrative function, which I will call personality coherence, completes the architecture of human ontogeny. Personality coherence refers to mind as a complex adaptive system that functions as a unified whole without separate or conflicting parts. The importance of personality coherence in adaptation across the lifespan has been recognized in social cognitive research on personality for many years (Cervone & Shoda, 1999). Well-adapted people can maintain an integrated sense of continuity throughout their life despite changing demands in different contexts across the lifespan. In fact, the unique power and direction of personality coherence emerges especially in the face of adversity, illness, and imminent death, which are so prominent in old age. Furthermore, such suffering serves the crucial function of bringing the limitations of algorithmic processes and the incompleteness of the individual self into conscious awareness. This point of awareness can provide a useful link between Baltes’s findings about the incompleteness of genetic and cultural determinants of human ontogeny and other research regarding the role of intuitive learning in the cognitive development of personality coherence.

My own research on the understanding of personality development has gone through three major phases. I will refer to these three phases in terms of models of personality as temperament, as self, and as coherence. Nevertheless, these phases serve to illustrate a hierarchy of paradigms, each of which exists to some extent in other current and past theories in the philosophy of mind and in the psychobiology of cognition and personality. They are truly a hierarchy of nested hypotheses about personality in which the model of coherence includes that of self as a special case and the model of self includes temperament as a special case. I have been forced to develop more comprehensive models by experimental observations that could not be satisfied by the more restricted cases. Here I will only sketch this succession of paradigms in sufficient detail to reveal the major conceptual and experimental issues that I have confronted in order to spare the reader from adopting inadequate paradigms, which are unfortunately all too commonplace. The third phase is still under development and consequently is less well supported by empirical data. Interested readers can consult other cited sources for additional details and documentation, as well as comparisons to other models of human personality (Cloninger, Przybeck, Svrakic, & Wetzel, 1994). Alternative models of personality, such as those derived by linear factor analysis, do not distinguish temperament and character and so confound relevant developmental phenomena in which these domains differ (see also Roberts & Caspi, this volume).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ackerman, B. (1998). Children’s false memories: A test of the dissociability of cognitive and social processes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 178–183.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Augustine. (1991). The Trinity (E. Hill, Trans.). Brooklyn, NY: New City Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny. American Psychologist, 52, 366–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (1990). Toward a psychology of wisdom and its ontogenesis. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 87–120). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bayon, C, Hill, K., Svrakic, D. M., Przybeck, T. R., & Cloninger, C. R. (1996). Dimensional assessment of personality in an outpatient sample: Relations of the systems of Millon and Cloninger. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 30, 341–352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bayrak, T. (1993). Ibn ‘Arab’s divine governance of the human kingdom (pp. 411–430). Louisville, KY: Fons Vitae.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, J. S. (1996). Cognitive therapy of personality disorders. In P. M. Salkovskis (Ed.), Frontiers of cognitive therapy (pp. 165–181). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, J. S. (1993). Speakable and unspeakable in quantum mechanics (pp. 40–43). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). In search of a metatheory for cognitive development (or, Piaget is dead and I don’t feel so good myself). Child Development, 68, 144–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bohm, D. (1980). Wholeness and the implicate order. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouwmeester, D., Pan, J. W., Mattle, K., Eibl, M, Weinfurter, H., & Zeilinger, A. (1997). Experimental quantum teleportation. Nature, 390, 575–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brainer, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (1993). Memory independence and memory interference in cognitive development. Psychological Review, 100, 42–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (1998). Fuzzy-trace theory and children’s false memories. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 81–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., Howe, M. L., & Kingma, J. (1990). The development of forgetting and reminiscence. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 5, 1–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F, & Mojardin, A. H. (1999). Conjoint recognition. Psychological Review, 106, 160–179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cervone, D., & Shoda, Y. (1999). The coherence of personality: Social-cognitive bases of consistency, variability, and organization. New York: Guildford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D. J. (1996). The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, M. J. with Holliday, D. (1990). Wisdom in a postapocalyptic age. In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.): Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 121–141.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R. (1986). A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states. Psychiatric Developments, 3, 167–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants: A proposal. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 573–587.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R. (1994). Temperament and personality. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 4, 266–273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R. (1998). The genetics and psychobiology of the seven-factor model of personality. In K. R. Silk (Ed.), The biology of personality disorders (pp. 63–84). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R. (1999). Personality and psychopathology. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R. (2000). Biology of personality dimensions. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 13, 611–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R. (2002). Implications of comorbidity for the classification of mental disorders: The need for a psychobiology of coherence. In M. Maj (Ed.), psychiatric diagnosis and classification. Chichester, West Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 79–106.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R., Przybeck, T. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Wetzel, R. D. (1994). The temperament and character inventory (TCI): A guide to its development and use. St. Louis, MO: Washington University Center for Psychobiology of Personality.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R., & Svrakic, D. M. (2000). Personality disorders. In B. J. Sadock & V. A. Sadock (Eds.), Comprehensive textbook of psychiatry (7th ed., pp. 1723–1764). New York: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975–990.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, N. M., & Svrakic, D. M. (1997). Role of personality self-organization in development of mental order and disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 681–906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Comings, D. E., Gade-Andavolu, R., Gonzalez, N., Wu, S., Muhleman, D., Blake, H., et al. (2000). A multivariate analysis of 59 candidate genes in personality traits: The Temperament and Character Inventory. Clinical Genetics 58, 375–385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coward, D. D. (1995). The lived experience of Self-transcendence in women with AIDS. Journal of Obstetrics, Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing, 24, 314–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. (1989). Evolution of the brain: Creation of the self London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, W., & Watts, J. W. (1942). Psychosurgery: Intelligence, emotion, and social behavior following prefrontal lobotomyfor mental disorders. Springfield, IL: Thomas.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, A. C, Cloninger, C. R., & Martin, N. G. (1994). Testing a model for the genetic structure of personality: A comparison of the personality systems of Cloninger and Eysenck. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 762–775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krishnamurti, J. (1973). The awakening of intelligence. San Francisco: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krishnamurti, J., & Bohm, D. (1985). The ending of time. San Francisco: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1990). Wisdom as integrated thought: Historical and developmental perspectives. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins and development (pp. 52–86). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Linehan, M. M., Armstrong, H. E., & Suarez, A. (1991). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of chronically pararsuicidal borderline patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 795–799.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. H., & Bjorklund, D. F. (1998). Contemplating fuzzy-trace theory: The gist of it. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 184–193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind: The cognitive origins of art, religion, and science. London: Thames and Hudson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulder, R. T., Joyce, P. R., Sellman, J. F, & Cloninger, C. R. (1996). Towards an understanding of defense style in terms of temperament and character. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 93, 99–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Penrose, R. (1989). The emperor’s new mind: Concerning computers, minds, and the laws of physics. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rey, G. (1997). Contemporary philosophy of mind. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, V. F, & Brainerd, C. J. (1998). Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory: New frontiers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 194–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schrodinger, E. (1967). What is lifel New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stallings, M. C, Hewitt, J. K., Cloninger, C. R., Heath, A. C, & Eaves, L. J. (1996). Genetic and environmental structure of the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire: Three or four temperament dimensions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 127–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stapp, H. (1999). Attention, intention, and will in quantum physics. Journal of Conscious Studies: The Volitional Brain, 6, 143–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staudinger, U. M., & Pasupathi, M. (2000). Life-span perspectives on self, personality, and social cognition. In T. Salthouse & F. Craik (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and aging (pp. 633–688). Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1988). The nature of creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1986). Personality and emotion. In D. T. Stuss & D. F. Benson (Eds), The frontal lobes (pp. 121–140). New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sugiura, M., Kawashima, R., Nakagawa, M., Okada, K., Sato, T., & Goto, R. (2000). Correlation between human personality and neural activity in cerebral cortex. Neurolmage, 11, 541–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Svrakic, D. M., Whitehead, C, Przybeck, T. R., & Cloninger, C. R. (1993). Differential diagnosis of personality disorders by the seven factor model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 991–999.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teilhard de Chardin (1959). The Phenomenon of Man. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tittel, W, Brendel, J., Zbinden, H., & Gisen, N. (1998). Violation of Bell inequalities by photons more than 10 km apart. Physical Review Letters, 81, 3563–3566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vedeniapin, A. B., Anokhin, A. A., Sirevaag, E., Rohrbaugh, J. W, & Cloninger, C. R. (2001). Visual P300 and the self-directedness scale of the temperament-character inventory. Psychiatry Research, 101, 145–156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P. E. (1970). Creativity: Selected readings. Baltimore, MD: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, E. H. (2000). The physics of consciousness: Quantum minds and the meaning of life (pp. 257–266). Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weihs, G., Jennewein, T., Simon, C, Weinfurter, H., & Zeilinger, A. (1998). Violation of Bell’s inequality under strict Einstein locality conditions. Physical Review Letters, 81, 5039–5043.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winner, E. (1996). Gifted children: Myths and realities. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeilinger, A. (2000). Quantum teleportation. Scientific American, 282, 50–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2003 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Cloninger, C.R. (2003). Completing the Psychobiological Architecture of Human Personality Development: Temperament, Character, and Coherence. In: Staudinger, U.M., Lindenberger, U. (eds) Understanding Human Development. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0357-6_8

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0357-6_8

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4020-7383-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-0357-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics