Skip to main content

Enablement and Constraint

  • Chapter
Understanding Human Development

Abstract

A general issue for understanding development concerns the interplay between enablement and constraint, gains and losses. Notions of expertise provide one framework commonly used to consider such questions. For example, notions of expertise often underpin Baltes’s lifespan metatheory with its emphasis on gains and losses and on the tradeoffs between selection and optimization. Three features often come to the foreground, if one thinks of cognitive development in terms of acquiring expertise: (1) the domain-general character of initial learning processes, (2) a presumed developmental shift from an amassed set of facts and experiences to a derived set of abstract principles, and (3) the expectation that more practice and more experience lead to more knowledge and that more knowledge leads to more powerful learning. All of these expectations are challenged by features of early cognitive development. While early cognitive development does encompass the accumulation of knowledge and skill in ways that meet these expectations, it also profoundly manifests (1) domain-specific inital learning states and mechanisms, (2) abstract frameworks of understanding that precede and shape the accumulation of bodies of more specific knowledge rather than being derived from them, and (3) learning mechanisms and trajectories where less is more. In this chapter, I discuss these features of early development as a way to contribute to a lifespan metatheory that comprehensively encompasses the full scope of cognitive development. I argue, among other things, that a comprehensive understanding of cognitive development may need to be especially informed by developments at both extremes of the life courseearly childhood and late life.

In limitations he first shows himself the master. (Goethe, as quoted in Bartlett 1992) —Goethe (1802)

A general issue for understanding development concerns the interplay between enablement and constraint, plasticity and fixedness. Conceivably, development could move simply from plasticity to fixity, from potential to accomplishment. At times, scholars and researchers interested in early periods of the lifespan—infancy and childhood—have written and acted as if development was unidirectional in this fashion. But this is simplistic, as Baltes’s lifespan proposals make clear. In particular, Baltes’s metatheoretical perspectives—especially those on gains and losses and on selective optimization with compensation—emphasize some of the intricate interrelations and balances between enablement and constraint. At the very least, to optimize some performance, capacity, or skill requires selection of a focus for resources and efforts, and since resources are inevitably limited, such selection breeds losses as well as gains. To use an economic metaphor, each developmental advance entails opportunity costs (alternatives not invested in; see Behrman, this volume); to use a poetic metaphor, each developmental path necessitates “a road not taken.”

A third perspective that underpins Baltes’s writings, including the target article, is that of expertise (see also Salthouse, this volume). Humans acquire knowledge and skill over the lifespan: most contemporary Western adults, for example, acquire considerable, albeit commonplace, expertise in reading or in driving cars. Scientists and engineers, skilled traditional wayfarers or master herbalists, acquire more individualized, specialized expertises. The achievement and maintenance of commonplace and specialized expertise is an ever-present task. Thus, Baltes’s example of Arthur Rubenstein focuses not only on selection, optimization, and compensation but on expertise, in this case maintaining expert piano playing. Notions of expertise help frame and articulate relationships between gains, losses, selection, and optimization. One relation between gains, losses, and expertise is alluded to in the epigram by Goethe that begins this chapter; another is parodied in the saying that “an expert is someone who knows more and more about less and less.”

How can we conceptualize the interplay between gains, losses, and expertise as knowledge and skill wax and wane over the life course? Baltes’s examples and analyses focus on adult cognitive development. Over the last 15 years, researchers interested in early cognitive development have wrestled with parallel issues. One of my aims in this chapter is to focus on early development by way of endorsing a comprehensively lifespan perspective on cognitive development; every person who lives to late life was once a young child. Another aim is to consider more closely the notion of expertise as a metatheoretical construct for thinking about gains and loses, about enablement and constraint. I believe that background assumptions about the acquisition of expertise often shape our theories and discussions of cognitive development, but these expectations can prove misleading for understanding certain crucial aspects of early development. They may also mislead us for understanding development more generally.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M. J., Treiman, R., & Pressley, M. (1998). Reading, writing, and literacy. In W. Damon, I. Sigel, & K. Renninger (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 4, Child psychology in practice. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adelson, B. (1984). When novices surpass experts: The difficulty of the task may increase with expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10,483–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, P. A. (1992). Domain knowledge: Evolving themes and emerging concerns. Educational Psychology, 27, 33–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. W. (1993). The child’s discovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R. (1986). Representing the existence and the location of hidden objects: Object permanence in six- and eight-month-old infants. Cognition, 23, 21–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R., & DeVos, J. (1991). Object permanence in young infants: Further evidence. Child Development, 62, 1227–1246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R., Kotovsky, L., & Needham, A. (1995). The acquisition of physical knowledge in infancy. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multi-disciplinary debate (pp. 79–116). New York: Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny: Selection, optimization, and compensation as foundation of developmental theory. American Psychologist, 52, 366–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, J. (1992). Familiar quotations (16th edition). London: Little, Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1989). Young children’s attribution of action to beliefs and desires. Child Development, 60, 946–964.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beddard, J., & Chi, M. T. H. (1992). Expertise. Current Directions in Psychological Research, 1, 135–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1987). How age changes in knowledge base contibute to the development of children’s memory. Developmental Review, 1, 93–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., & Green, B. L. (1992). The adaptive nature of cognitive immaturity. American Psychologist, 47, 46–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L., & DeLoache, J. S. (1978). Skills, plans, and self-regulation. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? (pp. 3–35). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R. & Farr, M. (Eds.), (1988). The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., Hutchinson, J., & Robin, A. (1989). How inferences about novel domain-related concepts can be constrained by structured knowledge. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 35, 27–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1975). Reflections on language. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J. L. (1993). Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small. Cognition, 48, 71–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ericcson, K. A., & Lehman, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericcson, K. A., & Smith, J. (1991). Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes, D., Wellman, H. M., & Woolley, J. D. (1989). Children’s understanding of mental phenomena. In H. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 41–87). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). Modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R. (1987). Cognitive development: Principles guide learning and contribute to conceptual coherence. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association Meetings, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R. (1990). First principles organize attention to and learning about relevant data: Number and the animate-inanimate distinction as examples. Cognitive Science, 14, 79–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A., Coley, J. D., & Gottfried, G. M. (1994). Essentialist beliefs in children: The acquisition of concepts and theories. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 341–365). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (1991). Insides and essences: Early understandings of the non-obvious. Cognition, 38, 213–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. & Meltzoff, M. N. (1997). Words, Thoughts, and Theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (1994). The theory theory. In L. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 257–293). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschfeld, L. A., & Gelman, S. A. (1994). Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, J., & Newport, E. (1989). Critical period effects in second-language learning. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 60–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. E., & Mervis, C. B. (1988). Impact of intuitive theories on feature recruitment through the continuum of expertise. Memory and Cognition, 26, 383–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. (1991). Developmental changes in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 490–501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R., & Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Processing speed as mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86, 199–225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1984). Mechanisms of cognitive development and the structure of knowledge. In R. Sternberg (Ed.), Mechanisms of cognitive development (pp. 81–99). New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos, I. (1970). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge (pp. 91–196). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laudan, L. (1977). Progress and its problems: Towards a theory of scientific growth. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legerstee, M. (1991). The role of person and object in eliciting early imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 423–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Legerstee, M. (1992). A review of the animate-inanimate distinction in infancy. Early Development and Parenting, 1, 59–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1994). ToMM, ToBy, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity in cognition and culture. In L. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 119–148). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, D. (1954). Language development in children. In L. Carmichael (ed.) Manual of Child Psychology. (2nd, edition, pp. 492–630) New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M. (2000). Perceptual and conceptual processes in infancy. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 3–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marchant, G., Robinson, J., Anderson, U., & Schadewold, D. (1991). Analogical transfer and expertise in legal reasoning. Organization Behavior and Human Decision Processing, 48, 272–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M. (1989). Categorization and naming in children. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1995). A theory of the role of imitation in the emergence of self. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy, (pp. 73–93) Geneva: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Newport, E. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 11–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newport, E. L. (1991). Contrasting concepts of the critical period for language. In S. Carey & R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 111–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quine, W. V. O. (1961). From a logical point of view. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Remen, N. R. (1996). Kitchen table wisdom. New York: Riverview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Repacholi, B. M., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-month olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 12–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richards, D. D., & Siegler, R. S. (1986). Children’s understandings of the attributes of life. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 1–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sabbagh, M. A., & Baldwin, D. A. (1997). Low-cost constraints: What social understanding means for semantic development. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Washington,DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schult, C. A., & Wellman, H. M. (1997). Explaining human movements and actions: Children’s understanding of the limits of psychological explanation. Cognition, 62, 291–324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., & Keil, F. C. (1995). An abstract to concrete shift in the development of biological thought. Cognition, 56, 129–163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S. (1994). Initial knowledge: Six suggestions. Cognition, 50, 431–445.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., & Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605–632.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A. T., & Woodward, A. L. (1995). Infants’ knowledge of object motion and human action. In A. Premack (Ed.), Causal understanding in cognition and culture. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M, Kruger, A. C. & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 639–650.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turkewitz, G., & Kenny, P. A. (1982). Limitations on input as a basis for neural organization and perceptual development: A preliminary theoretical statement. Developmental Psychobiology, 15, 357–368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. K., Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (1998). Young children’s understanding of the non-physical nature of thoughts and the physical nature of the brain. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 321–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, a Bradford Book.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive development: Foundational theories of core domains. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 337–375.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1998). Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains. In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 2, Cognition, perception and language, (5th ed, pp. 523–573). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Harris, P. L., Banerjee, M., & Sinclair, A. (1995). Early understanding of emotion: Evidence from natural language. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 117–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Hickling, A. K., & Schult, C. A. (1997). Young children’s explanations: Psychological, physical, and biological reasoning. In H. M. Wellman & K. Inagaki (Eds.), The emergency of core domains of thought: Physical, psychological, and biological thinking. San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wierzabicka, A. (1992). Semantics, culture and cognition: Universal human concepts in culture-specific configurations. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, A. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2003 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Wellman, H.M. (2003). Enablement and Constraint. In: Staudinger, U.M., Lindenberger, U. (eds) Understanding Human Development. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0357-6_11

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0357-6_11

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4020-7383-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-0357-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics