Skip to main content

Embodied and Hybrid Theories of Abstract Concepts and Words

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Words as Social Tools: An Embodied View on Abstract Concepts

Part of the book series: SpringerBriefs in Psychology ((BRIEFSCOGNIT))

  • 1310 Accesses

Abstract

The chapter overviews the most important recent theories of abstract concepts. We argue that it is still an open issue whether a unifying theory explaining all abstract concepts is possible. We highlight the strengths and limitations of each theory and underline their similarities and differences from the WAT proposal. First, we focus on embodied cognition theories according to which abstract and concrete concepts do not differ as they are both grounded in action, emotion, and force dynamics. Then, we illustrate theories which, even if maintaining an embodied stance, argue that abstract and concrete concepts are grounded on different aspects, for example situations versus perceptual properties and direct experience versus metaphors. Finally, we describe approaches arguing for the necessity of a double representation, some of which originate from the classical theory of Paivio. In line with all other embodied proposals, according to WAT, all concepts, not only concrete ones, are embodied and grounded. As other theories, WAT stresses the fact that the grounding of abstract concepts differs from that of concrete ones: Abstract concepts activate more situations, more linguistic information, and more emotions compared to concrete concepts, which evoke more sensorimotor information. In line with distributional models and similarly to hybrid models, WAT stresses the role of language for abstract concepts representation. However, it does not equate the role of language only to the information derived from word associations in a distributed network. WAT intends language in a complex sense, as a social experience which involves our body and triggers our emotions.

To say it another way, thinking, however abstract, originates in an embodied subjectivity, at once overdetermined and permeable to contingent events.

Teresa de Lauretis

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Anderson, M. L. (2010). Neural re-use as a fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 245–266.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, M., Frank, S., & Vigliocco, G. (2013). Reconciling embodied and distributional accounts of meaning in language. Topics in Cognitive Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, M., Vigliocco, G., & Vinson, D. P. (2009). Integrating experiential and distributional data to learn semantic representations. Psychological Review, 116(3), 463–498.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Brain and Behavioural Sciences, 22, 577–660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W., Santos, A., Simmons, K. W., & Wilson, C. D. (2008). Language and simulations in conceptual processing. In M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols, embodiment and meaning (pp. 245–283). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W., & Wiemer-Hastings, K. (2005). Situating abstract concepts. In D. Pecher & R. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thought (pp. 129–163). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baumgaertner, A., Buccino, G., Lange, R., McNamara, A., & Binkofski, F. (2007). Polymodal conceptual processing of human biological actions in the left inferior frontal lobe. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25(3), 881–889.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Binkofsk, F., Amunts, K., Stephan, K. M., Posse, S., Schormann, T., Freund, H. J., et al. (2000). Broca’s region subserves imagery of motion: A combined cytoarchitectonic and fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 11(4), 273–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonato, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2012). When time is space: Evidence for a mental time line. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Review, 36(10), 2257–2273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boot, I., & Pecher, D. (2010). Similarity is closeness: Metaphorical mapping in a perceptual task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 942–954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boot, I., & Pecher, D. (2011). Representation of categories. Experimental Psychology, 58(2), 162–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M. (2005). Object concepts and action. In D. Pecher & R. A. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory (pp. 8–34). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M. (2012). Action language comprehension, affordances and goals. In Y. Coello & A. Bartolo (Eds.), Language and action in cognitive neuroscience. Contemporary topics in cognitive neuroscience series (pp. 125–143). Psychology Press. ISBN: 978-1-84872-082-4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., & Caruana, F. (in press). Embodiment theories. In J. Wright (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). S. Cappa (Ed.) Section of cognitive neuroscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., & Cimatti, F. (2010). Embodied cognition and beyond: Acting and sensing the body. Neuropsychologia, 48, 763–773.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., & Cimatti, F. (2012). Words are not just words: The social acquisition of abstract words. RIFL - ISSN: 2036-6728. doi: 10.4396/20120303

  • Borghi, A. M., Scorolli, C., Caligiore, D., Baldassarre, G., & Tummolini, L. (2013). The embodied mind extended: Words as social tools. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 214. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00214

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M. (2002). The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological Science, 13(2), 185–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, C., & Lund, K. (1997). Modeling parsing constraints with high-dimensional context space. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 177–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cangelosi, A., & Harnad, S. (2000). The adaptive advantage of symbolic theft over sensorimotor toil: Grounding language in perceptual categories. Evolution of Communication, 4(1), 117–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caramelli, N., Setti, A. & Borghi, A. M. (in preparation). How abstract is risk for workers? Expertise and contextual constraints in abstract concepts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casasanto, D. (2008). Similarity and proximity: When does close in space mean close in mind? Memory and Cognition, 36, 1047–1056.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106, 579–593.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 215–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2012). Strength of perceptual experience predicts word processing performance better than concreteness or imageability. Cognition, 125(3), 452–465.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2013). Flexible and fast: Linguistic shortcut affects both shallow and deep conceptual processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(3), 542–550. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0368-x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coulson, S. (2000). Semantic leaps: Frame-shifting and conceptual blending in meaning construction. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coulson, S., & Van Petten, C. (2002). Conceptual integration and metaphor: An event-related potential study. Memory and Cognition, 30, 958–968.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Della Rosa, P. A., Catricalà, E., Vigliocco, G., & Cappa, S. F. (2010). Beyond the abstract-concrete dichotomy: Mode of acquisition, concreteness, imageability, familiarity, age of acquisition, context availability, and abstractness norms for a set of 417 Italian words. Behavioral Research Methods, 42(4), 1042–1048.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dove, G. (2009). Beyond perceptual symbols: A call for representational pluralism. Cognition, 110, 412–431.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dove, G. (2011). On the need for embodied and disembodied cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 242. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00242

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dove, G. (2013). Thinking in words: Language as an embodied medium of thoughts. Topics in Cognitive Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egorova, N., Shtyrov, Y., & Pulvermüller, F. (2013). Early and parallel processing of pragmatic and semantic information in speech acts: Neurophysiological evidence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 86.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (1998). Conceptual integration networks. Cognitive Science, 22(2), 133–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Firth, J. R. (1957). A synopsis of linguistic theory 1930–1955. Studies in Linguistic Analysis (special volume of the Philological Society, Oxford) (pp. 1–32). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., & Brugger, P. (2011). When digits help digits: Spatial–numerical associations point to finger counting as prime example of embodied cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 260. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00260

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flusberg, S. J., Thibodeau, P. H., Sternberg, D. A., & Glick, J. J. (2010). A connectionist approach to embodied conceptual metaphor. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 197. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00197

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308, 662–667.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Förster, J., & Strack, F. (1996). Influence of overt head movements on memory for valenced words: A case of conceptual-motor compatibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 421–430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freina, L., Baroni, G., Borghi, A. M., & Nicoletti, R. (2009). Emotive concept-nouns and motor responses: Attraction or repulsion? Memory & Cognition, 37, 493–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fusaroli, R., & Morgagni, S. (2013). Introduction: Thirty years later. Journal of Cognitive Semiotics, 5(1–2), 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2008). Mirror neurons and the social nature of language: The neural exploitation hypothesis. Social Neuroscience, 3, 317–333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentilucci, M., & Rizzolatti, G. (1988). Cortical motor control of arm and hand movements. In M. Goodale (Ed.), Vision and action. The control of grasping. Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gianelli, C., Lugli, L., Baroni, G., Nicoletti, R., & Borghi, A. M. (2013). The impact of social context and language comprehension on behaviour: A kinematic investigation. Plos One. 8(12), e85151. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085151.

  • Gibbs, R. W. J. (1994). The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language, and understanding. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, R. W. J. (2005). The psychological status of image schemas. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From perception to meaning (pp. 113–135). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Giessner, S. R., & Schubert, T. W. (2007). High in the hierarchy: How vertical location and judgments of leaders’ power are interrelated. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 104, 30–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, W. R. (1992). Picture naming. Cognition, 42, 61–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 558–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Sato, M., & Cattaneo, L. (2008a). Use-induced motor plasticity affects the processing of abstract and concrete language. Current Biology, 18(7), R290–R291. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.036

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Riggio, L., Palumbo, D., & Buccino, G. (2008b). Processing abstract language modulates motor system activity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 905–919.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A., & de Vignemont, F. (2009). Is social cognition embodied? Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(4), 154–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guan, C. Q., Meng, W., Yao, R., & Glenberg, A. M. (2013). Motor system contribution to the comprehension of abstract language. PLoS ONE, 8(9), e75183. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0075183

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 42, 335–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchinson, S., & Louwerse, M. M. (in press). Language statistics explain the spatial-numerical association of response codes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, L. (2013). The importance of situational information for abstract concepts. Dissertation, University of Western Ontario.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kousta, S., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D.P., Andrews, M. (2009). Happiness is an abstract word. The role of affect in abstract knowledge representation. In N. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.). Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Amsterdam: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kousta, S. T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2011). The representation of abstract words: Why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 14–34. doi:10.1037/a0021446

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kranjec, A., & Chatterjee, A. (2010). Are temporal concepts embodied? A challenge for cognitive neuroscience. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 240. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00240

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by Chicago. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T., & Dumais, S. (1997). A solutions to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Louwerse, M. (2008). Embodied relations are encoded in language. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 838–844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Louwerse, M. M. (2011). Symbol interdependency in symbolic and embodied cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3(2), 273–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Louwerse, M. M., & Connell, L. (2011). A taste of words: Linguistic context and perceptual simulation predict the modality of words. Cognitive Science, 35, 381–398.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Louwerse, M. M., & Jeuniaux, P. (2008). Language comprehension is both embodied and symbolic. In A. C. G. M. de Vega & A. Glenberg (Eds.), Symbols and embodiment: Debates on meaning and cognition (pp. 309–326). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Louwerse, M. M., & Jeuniaux, P. (2010). The linguistic and embodied nature of conceptual processing. Cognition, 114(1), 96–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lugli, L., Baroni, G., Anelli, F., Borghi, A. M., & Nicoletti, R. (2013). Counting is easier while experiencing a congruent motion. PLoS ONE, 8(5), e64500. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0064500

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lugli, L., Baroni, G., Gianelli, C., Borghi, A. M., & Nicoletti, R. (2012). Self, others, objects: How this triadic interaction modulates our behavior. Memory and Cognition, 40, 1373–1386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Madden, C. & Pecher, D. (2010). The force behind language: Are concrete and abstract sentences understood in terms of underlying force patterns? (Manuscript submitted for publication, reported in Pecher et al., 2011).

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology Paris, 102, 59–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meier, B. P., Hauser, D. J., Robinson, M. D., Friesen, C. K., & Schjeldahl, K. (2007). What’s “up” with god? Vertical space as a representation of the divine. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 699–710.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meier, B. P., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Why the sunny side is up: Associations between affect and vertical position. Psychological Science, 15, 243–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mirolli, M., & Parisi, D. (2009). Language as a cognitive tool. Minds and Machines, 19(4), 517–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, G. L. (1996). On metaphoric representation. Cognition, 60, 173–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (2013). Dual coding theory, word abstractness, and emotion: a critical review of Kousta et al. (2011). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1):282–287. doi: 10.1037/a0027004

  • Parkinson, C., & Wheatley, T. (2013). Old cortex, new contexts: Re-purposing spatial perception for social cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 645. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00645

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pecher, D., Boot, I., & van Dantzig, S. (2011). Abstract concepts: Sensory-motor grounding, metaphors, and beyond. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 54, pp. 217–248). Burlington: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Verifying different-modality properties for concepts produces switching costs. Psychological Science, 14, 119–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pezzulo, G., & Castelfranchi, C. (2007). The symbol detachment problem. Cognitive Processing, 8, 115–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. J. (2002). Furnishing the mind: Concepts and their perceptual basis. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. J. (2005). The return of concept empiricism. In H. Cohen & C. Leferbvre (Eds.), Categorization and cognitive science. New Jersey: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. J. (2012). Beyond human nature. How culture and experience shape our lives. London: Penguin, New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (2013). Action representation: Crosstalk between semantics and pragmatics. Neuropsychologia. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.08.015 (pii: S0028-3932(13)00279-0).

  • Puglisi, A., Baronchelli, A., & Loreto, V. (2008). Cultural route to the emergence of linguistic categories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 105, 7936–7940.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pulvermüller, F., Shtyrov, Y., & Ilmoniemi, R. (2005). Brain signatures of meaning access in action word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 884–892.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Camarda, R., Fogassi, L., Gentilucci, M., Luppino, G., & Matelli, M. (1988). Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. II. Area F5 and the control of distal movements. Experimental Brain Research, 71(3), 491–507.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roversi, C., Borghi, A. M., & Tummolini, L. (2013). A marriage is an artefact and not a walk that we take together: An experimental study on the categorization of artefacts. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4(3), 527–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sakreida, K., Scorolli, C., Menz, M.M., Heim, S., Borghi, A.M., Binkofski, F. (2013). Are abstract action words embodied? An fMRI investigation at the interface between language and motor cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., Harnishfeger, K. K., & Stowe, R. W. (1988). Context availability and lexical decisions for abstract and concrete words. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 499–520. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(88)90022-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scorolli, C., Binkofski, F., Buccino, G., Nicoletti, R., Riggio, L., Borghi, A.M. (2011). Abstract and concrete sentences, embodiment, and languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 227. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00227.

  • Scorolli, C., Jacquet, P., Binkofski, F., Nicoletti, R., Tessari, A., Borghi, A.M. (2012). Abstract and concrete phrases processing differently modulates cortico-spinal excitability. Brain Research, 1488, 60–71. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.10.004.

  • Simmons, W. K., Hamann, S. B., Harenski, C. L., Hu, X. P., & Barsalou, L. W. (2008). fMRI evidence for word association and situated simulation in conceptual processing. Journal of Physiology Paris, 102(1–3), 106–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slepian, M. L., & Ambady, N. (2014). Simulating sensorimotor metaphors: Novel metaphors influence sensory judgments. Cognition, 130(3), 309–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanfield, R. A., & Zwaan, R. A. (2001). The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychological Science, 12, 153–156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (1988). Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science, 12, 49–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80, 352–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dantzig, S., Pecher, D., & Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Approach and avoidance as action effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1298–1306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vigliocco, G., Kousta, S. T., Della Rosa, P. A., Vinson, D. P., Tettamanti, M., Devlin, J. T., & Cappa, S. F. (2013a). The neural representation of abstract words: The role of emotion. Cerebral Cortex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vigliocco, G., Kousta, S., Vinson, D., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2013b).The representation of abstract words: What matters? Reply to Paivio’s (2013) comment on Kousta et al. (2011). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1):288–291. doi: 10.1037/a0028749

  • Wiemer-Hastings, K., & Xu, X. (2005). Content differences for abstract and concrete concepts. Cognitive Science, 29, 719–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Simmons, W. K., Martin, A., & Barsalou, L. W. (2014). Contextual processing of abstract concepts reveals neural representations of non-linguistic semantic content. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(6), 920–935.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winner, E., Rosenstiel, A. K., & Gardner, H. (1976). The development of metaphoric understanding. Developmental Psychology, 12(4), 289–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2003). Spatial iconicity affects semantic relatedness judgment. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(4), 954–958.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anna M. Borghi .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Borghi, A.M., Binkofski, F. (2014). Embodied and Hybrid Theories of Abstract Concepts and Words. In: Words as Social Tools: An Embodied View on Abstract Concepts. SpringerBriefs in Psychology(). Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-9539-0_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics