Skip to main content

The WAT Proposal and the Role of Language

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Words as Social Tools: An Embodied View on Abstract Concepts

Part of the book series: SpringerBriefs in Psychology ((BRIEFSCOGNIT))

Abstract

The chapter illustrates the words as social tools (WAT) theory on abstract concepts and words. The theory has five main tenets. (1) embodiment and grounding. Both concrete and abstract concepts and words are embodied and grounded in perception, action, and emotional systems; (2) importance of language. For the representation of abstract concepts, the linguistic mediation is more crucial than for the representation of concrete ones, given that the scaffolding function of the physical environment is less powerful for abstract than for concrete concepts; (3) acquisition modality. The acquisition modality of abstract concepts and words relies more on language than the acquisition of concrete concepts and words; (4) brain representation. While both activate the sensorimotor network, the linguistic network is activated more by abstract than by concrete concepts and words. (5) linguistic diversity. Abstract concepts and words are more affected by differences between languages than concrete ones; that is, their meaning changes more depending on the cultural and linguistic milieu in which they are learned. Overall, abstract concepts do not differ from concrete ones in embodiment, but differ from them in acquisition modality, in brain representation, in variability across languages, and they are also likely to differ in the assessment of quantity. Once outlined the main principles of the theory, in the rest of the chapter we discuss the reasons of the dominance of language and the role played by labels and linguistic explanations for abstract concepts and words, as well as the possible mechanisms underlying the activation of its motor counterpart, the mouth effector.

You learned the concept “pain” when you learned language.

For a large class of cases—though not for all—in which we employ the word meaning, it can be explained thus: The meaning of a word is its use in the language.

Every explanation is after all an hypothesis.

Ludwig Wittgenstein

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Andrews, M., Frank, S. & Vigliocco, G. (2013). Reconciling embodied and distributional accounts of meaning in language. Topics in Cognitive Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badets, A., & Pesenti, M. (2010). Creating number semantics through finger movement perception. Cognition, 115, 46–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–609.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W., & Wiemer-Hastings, K. (2005). Situating abstract concepts. In D. Pecher & R. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thought (pp. 129–163). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Berlin, B., Breedlove, D., & Raven, P. (1973). General principles of classification and nomenclature in folk biology. American Anthropologist, 74, 214–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A.M., & Caruana, F. (in press). Embodiment theories. In J. Wright (Ed.) International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). S. Cappa (Ed.) Section of cognitive neuroscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., & Cimatti, F. (2009). Words as tools and the problem of abstract words meanings. In N. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2304–2309). Amsterdam: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A.M., & Cimatti, F. (2012). Words are not just words: the social acquisition of abstract words. RIFL—ISSN: 2036-6728. doi:10.4396/20120303

  • Borghi, A. M., Flumini, A., Cimatti, F., Marocco, D., & Scorolli, C. (2011). Manipulating objects and telling words: A study on concrete and abstract words acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 15. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00015

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cangelosi, A., & Parisi, D. (1998). The emergence of a “language” in an evolving population of neural networks. Connection Science, 10, 83–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cienki, A., & Mittelberg, I. (2013). Creativity in the forms and functions of gestures with speech. In T. Veale, K. Feyaerts, & C. Forceville (Eds.), Creativity and the agile mind: A multi-disciplinary study of a multi-faceted phenomenon (applications of cognitive linguistics) (pp. 231–252). Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind. Embodiment action, and cognitive extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58, 7–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2012). Strength of perceptual experience predicts word processing performance better than concreteness or imageability. Cognition, 125(3), 452–465.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Ausilio, A., Pulvermueller, F., Salmas, P., Bufalari, I., Begliomini, C., & Fadiga, L. (2009). The motor somatotopy of speech perception. Current Biology, 19(5), 381–385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deng, W., & Sloutsky, V.M. (2012). The role of linguistic labels in inductive generalization. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.011

  • Della Rosa, P.A., Catricalà, E., Vigliocco, G., & Cappa, S.F. (2010). Beyond the abstract-concrete dichotomy: mode of acquisition, concreteness, imageability, familiarity, age of acquisition, context availability, and abstractness norms for a set of 417 Italian words. Behavioral Research Methods, 42(4), 1042–1048

    Google Scholar 

  • Farias, A. R., Garrido, M. V., & Semin, G. R. (2013). Converging modalities ground abstract categories: The case of politics. PLoS ONE, 8(4), e60971. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0060971

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H. (2008). Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numerical associations. Cortex, 44, 386–392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H. (2011). The spatial mapping of numbers—its origin and flexibility. In Y. Coello & A. Bartolo (Eds.), Language and action in cognitive neurosciences. London: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., & Brugger, P. (2011). When digits help digits: spatial–numerical associations point to finger counting as prime example of embodied cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 260. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00260

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A., & Markman, E. (1986). Categories and induction in young children. Cognition, 23, 183–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ghio, M., Vaghi, M.M.S., & Tettamanti, M. (2013). Fine-grained semantic categorization across the abstract and concrete omains. PLoS ONE, 8(6): e67090. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0067090

  • Glenberg, A. M., & Robertson, D. A. (2000). Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 379–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Granito, C., Scorolli, C., & Borghi, A.M. (in preparation). Alice in Legoland: A study on abstract words acquisition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gliga, T., Volein, A., & Csibra, G. (2010). Verbal labels modulate perceptual object processing in one-year-old infants. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2781–2789.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, T. L., Steyvers, M., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Topics in semantic representation. Psychological Review, 114(2), 211–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jirak, D., Menz, M., Buccino, G., Borghi, A. M., & Binkofski, F. (2010). Grasping language. A short story on embodiment. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 711–720.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds and cognitive development. London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T., & Dumais, S. (1997). A solutions to Plato’s problem: the latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewkowicz, D. J. (1994). The development of intersensory perception in human infants. In D. J. Lewkowicz & R. Lickliter (Eds.), The development of perception: Comparative perspectives (pp. 165–203). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindemann, O., Alipour, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Finger counting habits in Middle-Eastern and Western individuals: An online survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 566–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Louwerse, M. M. (2011). Symbol interdependency in symbolic and embodied cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3, 273–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lugli, L., Baroni, G., Gianelli, C., Borghi, A.M., Nicoletti, R. (2012). Self, others, objects: How this triadic interaction modulates our behavior. Memory and Cognition, 40, 1373–1386

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupyan, G. (2012). What do words do? Toward a theory of language-augmented thought. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 57, pp. 255–297). London: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lupyan, G., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2012). The evocative power of words: Activation of concepts by verbal and nonverbal means. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 170–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malt, B., Gennari, S., & Imai, M. (2010). Lexicalization patterns and the world to words mapping. In B. C. Malt & S. Wolff (Eds.), Words and the mind. How words capture new experience. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meteyard, L., Cuadrado, S. R., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2012). Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex, 48, 788–804.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mittelberg, I. (2013). The exbodied mind: Cognitive-semiotic principles as motivating forces in gesture. In: C. Müller, A. Cienki, E. Fricke, S.H. Ladewig, D. McNeill & S. Tessendorf (eds.), Bodylanguagecommunication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction. Handbooks of linguistics and communication science (Vol. 38(1), pp. 750–779). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1931–1935, 1958). In: C. Hartshorne & P. Weiss (Eds.) Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce, vols. 1–6 (1931–35), A. W. Burks (Ed.), vols. 7–8 (1958), Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 329–347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. J. (2002). Furnishing the mind concepts and their perceptual basis. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. J. (2012). Beyond human nature. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ranzini, M., Lugli, L., Anelli, M., Carbone, R., Nicoletti, R., & Borghi, A.M. (2011). Graspable objects shape number processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, art.147, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2011.00147

  • Reilly, J., Westbury, C., Kean, J., & Peelle, J. E. (2012). Arbitrary symbolism in natural language revisited: When word forms carry meaning. PLoS ONE, 7(8), e42286. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0042286

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyews-Braem, P. (1976). Basic level in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sakreida, K., Scorolli, C., Menz, M.M., Heim, S., Borghi, A.M.,& Binkofski, F. (2013). Are abstract action words embodied? An fMRI investigation at the interface between language and motor cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 125

    Google Scholar 

  • Scorolli, C., Jacquet, P., Binkofski, F., Nicoletti, R., Tessari, A., & Borghi, A.M. (2012). Abstract and concrete phrases processing differently modulates cortico-spinal excitability. Brain Research, 1488, 60–71. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2012.10.004

  • Sloutsky V.M., & Fisher, A.V. (2004). When development and learning decrease memory. Evidence against category-based induction in children. Psychological science, 15(8), 533–548

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloutsky V.M., & Napolitano, A.C. (2003). Is a picture worth a thousand words? Preference for auditory modality in children. Child Development, 74(3), 822–33

    Google Scholar 

  • van Elk, M., van Schie, H. T., & Bekkering, H. (2010). From left to right: Processing acronyms referring to names of political parties activates spatial associations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(11), 2202–2219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind in Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L.S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wauters, L.N., Tellings, A.E.J.M., van Bon, W.H.J., & van Haaften, A.W. (2003). Mode of acquisition of word meanings: The viability of a theoretical construct. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 385–406

    Google Scholar 

  • Waxman, S.R., & Markow, D.B. (1995). Words as invitations to form categories: Evidence from 12–13-month-old infants. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 257–302

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoshida, H. & Smith, L.B. (2005). Linguistic cues enhance the learning of perceptual cues. Psychological Science, 16(2), 90–95

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anna M. Borghi .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Borghi, A.M., Binkofski, F. (2014). The WAT Proposal and the Role of Language. In: Words as Social Tools: An Embodied View on Abstract Concepts. SpringerBriefs in Psychology(). Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-9539-0_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics