Skip to main content

Convergence of Approaches in Localizing the Hyperactivity Deficit

  • Chapter
Advances in Clinical Child Psychology

Part of the book series: Advances in Clinical Child Psychology ((ACCP,volume 13))

Abstract

In recent years, there has been a growing interest in childhood psychopathology, which has been reflected in an increase in books and journals, and in an enlarged section concerning childhood disorders in the third edition (DSM-III) and the revised third edition (DSM-III-R) of the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual. Although this increase in the volume of interest is encouraging, the consideration of childhood psychopathology should be based on sound models of human behavior (Quay, 1988). In order to place childhood diagnostics on an experimental footing, the ideal would be to generate a model from the existing reliable research both on childhood dysfunction and on normal human functioning. It would be inadequate, merely to seize an impressive model that was developed for normal functioning and that has no conceptual coverage of abnormal functioning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 74.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Aman, M. G., & Turbott, S. H. (1986). Incidental learning, distraction, and sustained attention in hyperactive and control subjects. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 14,441–455.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R. P., Holcomb, C. G., & Doyle, R. B. (1973). The measurement of attentional deficits. Exceptional Children, 39,534–540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1976). The psychology of memory.London: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ballinger, C. T., Variey, C. K., and Nolen, P. A. (1984). Effects of methylphenidate on reading in children with attention deficit disorder.American Journal of Psychiatry, 141,1590–1593.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (1977). The effects of methylphenidate on various types of activity level and attention in hyperkinetic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 5,351–369.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beale, I. L., Matthew, P. J., Oliver, S., & Corballis, C. (1987). Performance of disabled and normal readers on the continuous performance test.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15,229–238.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benezra, E., & Douglas, V. I. (1988). Short-term serial recall in ADDH, normal, and reading disabled boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16,511–526.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertelson, P., & Barzeele, J. (1965). Interaction of time uncertainty and relative signal frequency in determining choice reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70,448–451.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bevan, W., Hardesty, D. L., & Avant, U. L. (1965). Response latency with constant and variable interval schedules. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 30,969–972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borcherding, B., Thompson, K., Kruesi, M., Bartko, J., Rapoport, J. L., & Weingartner, H. (1988). Automatic and effortful processing in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16,333–346.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, C. (1937). The behavior of children receiving Benzedrine.American Journal of Psychiatry, 94,577–585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1971). Decision and stress.London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brookhuis, K. A., Mulder, G., Mulder, L. J. M., Gloerich, A. B. M., Dellen, H. J. van, Meere, J. J. van der, & Ellerman, H. H. (1981). Late positive components and stimulus evaluation time. Biological Psychology, 13,107–123.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brumaghim, J. T, Klorman, R., Strauss, J., Lewine, J. D., & Goldstein, M. G. (1987). Does methylphenidate affect information processing? Findings from two studies on performance and P3b latency. Psychophysiology, 24,361–373.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Callaway, E. (1983). The pharmacology of human information processing.Psychophysiology, 20,359–370.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Callaway, E. (1984). Human information processing: Some effects of methylphenidate, age and dopamine. Biological Psychiatry, 19,649–662.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, S. B., Douglas, V. I., & Morgenstern, G. (1971). Cognitive styles in hyperactive children and the effect of methylphenidate. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 12,55–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, S. B., Endman, M. W., & Bemfeld, G. (1977). A three-year follow-up of hyperactive preschoolers into elementary school, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18,239–250.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charles, L., Schain, R. J., Zelniker, T., & Guthrie, D. (1979). Effects of methylphenidate on hyperactive children’s ability to sustain attention.Pediatrics, 64,312–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chee, P., Logan, G., Schachar, R., Lindsay, P., & Wachsmuth, R. (1989). Effects of event rate and display time on sustained attention in hyperactive, normal and control children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17,371–391.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, C. R., Geffen, G. M., & Geffen, L. B. (1986). Role of monoamine pathways in attention and effort: Effects of Clonidine and methylphenidate in normal adult humans. Psychophartnacology, 90,35–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clements, S. D., & Peters, J. (1962). Minimal brain dysfunction in the schoolaged child. Archives of General Psychiatry, 6,185–197.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, N. J., Weiss, G., & Minde, K. (1972). Cognitive styles in adolescents previously diagnosed as hyperactives. Journal Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 13,203–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coons, H. W., Klorman, R., & Borgstedt, A. D. (1987). Effects of methylphenidate on adolescents with a childhood history of attention deficit disorder: 2. Journal American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 26,368–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies, D. R., & Parasuraman, R. (1982). The psychology of vigilance.London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeUen, H. L. van, Brookhuis, K. A., Mulder, G., Okita, T., & Mulder, L. J. M. (1985). Evoked potential correlates of practice in a visual search task. In D. Papakostopoulos, S. Butler, & I. Martin (Eds.), Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychophysiology (pp. 132–156). London: Croom Helm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donchin, E. (1979). Event-related brain potentials: A tool in the study of human information processing. In H. Begleiter (Ed.), Evoked brain potentials and behavior.New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, V. I. (1983). Attentional and cognitive problems. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Developmental psychiatry (pp. 280–329). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Draeger, S., Prior, M., & Sanson, A. (1986). Visual and auditory performance in hyperactive children: Competence or compliance. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 14,411–424.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan-Johnson, C. C. (1981). P300 latency: A new metric of information processing. Psychophysiology, 18,207–215.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dykman, R. A., Ackerman, P. T., & Oglesby, D. M. (1979). Selective and sustained attention in hyperactive, learning disabled, and normal boys. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 167,288–297.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, H. B., & Pappas, B. A. (1979). Evaluation of psychophysiological neurochemical and animal models of hyperactivity. In R. L. Trites (Ed.), Hyperactivity in children (pp. 61–92). Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Firestone, P., & Douglas, V. I. (1975). The effects of reward and punishment on reaction times and autonomic activity in hyperactive and normal children.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 3,201–215.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisk, A. D., & Schneider, W. (1981). Control and automatic processing during tasks requiring sustained attention: A new approach to vigilance. Human Factors, 23,737–750.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitts, P. M., Peterson, J. R., & Wölpe, G. (1963). Cognitive aspects of information processing: 2. Adjustments to stimulus redundancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65,423–432.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzpatrick, P., Klorman, R., Brumaghim, J. T., & Keefover, R. W (1988). Effects of meth-ylphenidate on stimulus evaluation and response processes: Evidence from performance and event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 15,292–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, J. M., Roth, W. T., Mohs, R. C., Hopkins, W. F., & Koppeil, B. S. (1979). Event-related potentials recorded from young and old adults during a memory retrieval task. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 47,450–459.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, D., Vaughan, H. G., & Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L. (1978). Stimulus and response related components of the late positive complex in visual discrimination tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 45,319–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frowein, H. W. (1981). Selective drug effects on information processing. Thesis.Enschede: Sneldruk Boulevard.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaillard, A. W. K. (1978). Slow brain potentials preceding task performance.Doctoral dissertation, Amsterdam, Academic Press B. V.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halperin, J. M., Wolf, L. E., Pascualvaca, D. M., Newcorn, J. H., Healey, J. M., O’Brien, J. D., Morganstein, A., & Young, J. G. (1988). Differential assessment of attention and impulsivity in children. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27,326–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiscock, M., Kinsboume, M., Caplan, B., & Swanson, J. M. (1979). Auditory attention in hyperactive children: Effects of stimulant medication on dichotic listening performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88,27–32.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, W. F., Wagner, A. E., & lalong, N. (1989). Sex differences in school-aged children with pervasive attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17,109–125.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoy, E., Weiss, G., Minde, K., & Cohen, N. (1978). The hyperactive chĂĽd at adolescence: Cognitive, emotional, and social functioning. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 6,311–324.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J., Rosman, B. L., Day, D., Albert, J., & Philips, W. (1964). Information processing in the child, significance of analytic and reflective modes. Psychological Monographs (Whole No. 578).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort.Englewood Cliffs, NY: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klee, S. H., & Garfinkel, B. D. (1983). The computerized CPT: A new measure for inattention. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11,487–493.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klorman, R., Salzman, L. F., Pass, H. L., Borgstedt, A. D., & Dainer, K. B. (1979). Effects of methylphenidate on hyperactive children’s evoked responses during passive and active attention. Psychophysiology, 16,23–29.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klorman, R., Brumaghim, J. T, Coons, H. W., Peloquin, L. J., Strauss, J., Lewine, J. D., Borgstedt, A. D., & Goldstein, M. G. (1988a). The contributions of event-related potentials to understanding the effects of stimulants on information processing in attention deficit disorder. In L. F. Bloomingdale & J. A. Sergeant (Eds.), Attention deficit disorder (Vol. 5, pp. 199–218). Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klorman, R., Brumaghim, J. T., Salzman, L. F., Struass, J., Borgstedt, A. D., McBride, M. C., & Loeb, S. (1988b). Effects of methylphenidate on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with and without aggressive/noncompliant features. Journal Abnormal Child Psychology, 16,413–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kupietz, S. S., & Balka, E. B. (1976). Alterations in the vigilance performance of children receiving amitriptyline and methylphenidate pharmaco therapy.Psychopharmacology, 50,29–33.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kupietz, S. S., & Richardson, E. (1978). Children’s vigilance performance and inattentiveness in the classroom.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19,145–154.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., McCarthy, G., & Donchin, E. (1977). Augmenting mental chronometry: The P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time.Science, 197,792–795.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lacey, J. I. (1967). Somatic response patterning and stress: Some revisions of activation theory. In M. H. Appley & R. Trumbull (Eds.), Psychological stress: Some issues in research.New York: Appleton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, R., Lachman, J. L., & Butterfield, E. C. (1979). Cognitive psychology and information processing: An introduction.Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, F., & Hobbes, G. (1981). The diagnosis of attention deficit disorder (hyperkinesis) in children. Journal American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 20,376–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loiselle, D. L., Stamm, J. S., Matinsky, S., & Whipple, S. (1980). Evoked potential and behavioural signs of attentive dysfunctions in hyperactive boys.Psychophysiology, 17,193–201.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mackworth, J. F. (1965). Effects of amphetamine on the detectability of signals in a vigilance task. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 19,104–110.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mackworth, J. F. (1970). Vigilance and attention.London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackworth, J. F., & Taylor, M. M. (1963). The d’ measure of signal detectability in vigilance-like situations.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 17,302–325.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malone, M. A., Kershner, J. R., & Siegel, L. (1988). The effects of methylphenidate on levels of processing and laterality in children with attention deficit disorder.Journal Abnormal Child Psychology, 16,379–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massaro, D. W. (1975). Experimental psychology and information processing.Chicago: Rand McNally College Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, G., & Donchin, E. (1981). A metric for thought: A comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. Science, 211,77–80.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. (1979). On the time relations of mental processes: A framework for analysing processes in cascade. Psychological Review, 86,287–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meere, J. J. van der, & Sergeant, J. A. (1987). A divided attention experiment in pervasively hyperactive children. Journal Abnormal Child Psychology, 15,379–391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meere, J. J. van der, & Sergeant, J. (1988). Controlled processing and vigilance in hyperactivity: Time will tell.Journal Abnormal Child Psychology, 16,641–655.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meere, J. J. van der, Baal, M. van, & Sergeant, J. A. (1989). The additive factor method: A differential diagnostic tool in hyperactivity and learning disablement. Journal Abnormal Child Psychology, 17,409–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meere, J. J. van der, Vreeling, H. J., & Sergeant, J. A. (1990). An energetical study in hyperactive, learning disabled and control children. In preparation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D., Irwin, D. E., Oman, A. M., & Kounios, J. (1988). The dynamics of cognition and action: Mental processes inferred from speed-accuracy decomposition. Psychological Review, 95,183–236.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, B. M., Klorman, R., Saltzman, L. F., Borgstedt, A. D., & Dainer, K. B. (1981). Normalizing effects of methylphenidate on hyperactive children’s performance and evoked potentials. Psychophysiology, 18,665–677.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. (1988). Discrete and continuous models of human information processing: Theoretical distinctions and empirical results. Acta Psychologica, 67,191–257.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mulder, G., Gloerich, A. B. M., Brookhuis, K. A., Dellen, H. J. van, & Mulder, L. J. M. (1984). Stage analysis of the reaction process using brain-evoked potentials and reaction time. Psychological Research, 46,15–32.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resource limited processes. Cognitive Psychology, 7,44–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nuechterlein, K. H. (1977). Reaction time and attention in schizophrenia: A critical evaluation of the data and the theories. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 3,373–428.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nuechterlein, K. H. (1983). Signal detection in vigilance tasks and behavioral attributes among offspring of schizophrenic mothers and among hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92,4–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Dougherty, M., Nuechterlein, K. H., & Drew, B. (1984). Hyperactive and hypoxic children: Signal detection, sustained attention, and behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93,178–191.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hanlon, J. R, Fussier, C., Sancin, E., & Grandjean, E. (1978). Efficacy of an ACTH-4–9 analog, relative to that of a standard drug (d-amphetamine) for blocking the vigilance decrement.Unpublished report, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oilman, R. (1977). Choice reaction time and the problem of distinguishing task effects from strategy effects. In S. Domic (Ed.),Attention and performance (Vol. 6). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pachella, R. G. (1974). The interpretation of reaction time in information processing research. In B. Kantowitz (Ed.),Human information processing: Tutorial in performance and recognition (pp. 41–82). New York: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parasuraman, R. (1979). Memory load and event-rate control sensitivity decrements in sustained attention. Science, 205,924–927.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parasuraman, R., & Davies, D. R. (1976). Decision theory analysis of response latencies in vigilance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2,569–582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, R. B., & Hauty, G. T. (1954). The effects of experimentally induced attitudes upon task proficiency. Journal Experimental Psychology, 47,267–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, R. B., & Hauty, G. T. (1955). Effect of psychological feedback upon work decrement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50,343–351.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peeke, S., Halliday, R., Callaway, E., Prael, R., & Reks, V. (1984). Effects of two doses of methylphenidate on verbal information processing in hyperactive children. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 4,82–88.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peloquin, L. J., & Klorman, R. (1986). Effects of methylphenidate on normal children’s mood, event-related potentials, and performance in memory scanning and vigilance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95,88–98.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometrie explorations of mind.Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Boies, S. J. (1971). Components of attention. Psychological Review, 78,391–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Klein, R., Summers, J., & Buggie, S. (1973). On the selection of signals. Memory and Cognition, 1,2–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pribram, K. H., & McGuiness, D. (1975). Arousal, activation and effort in the control of attention. Psychological Review, 82,116–149.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prior, A., Sanson, A., Freethy, L., & Geffen, G. (1985). Auditory attentional abilities in hyperactive children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26,289–304.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quay, H. C. (1988). Attention deficit disorder and the behavioral inhibition system: The relevance of the neuropsychological theory of Jeffrey A. Gray. In L. M. Bloomingdale & J. A. Sergeant (Eds.), Attention deficit disorder (Vol. 5, pp. 116–126). Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1979). Current paradigms and models in human information processing. In V. Hamilton & D. M. Warburton (Eds.), Human stress and cognition.New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapoport, J. L., Buchsbaum, M. S., Weingartner, H., Zahn, T. P., Ludow, C., & Mikkelsen, E. J. (1980). Dextroamphetamine—Its cognitive and behavioral effects in normal and hyperactive boys and normal men. Archives of General Psychiatry,8, 933–943.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapport, M. D., Stoner, G., DuPaul, G. J., Birmingham, B. K., & Tucker, S. (1985). Methylphenidate in hyperactive children: Differential effects of dose on academic, learning, and social behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13,(2),227–244.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reid, M. K., & Borkowski, J. G. (1984). Effects of methylphenidate (Ritalin) on information processing in hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12,169–186.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Sarason, I., Bransome, E. D., & Beck, L. H. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20,343–350.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W. T., Tinklenberg, F. R., & Koppell, B. S. (1977). Ethanol and marihuana effects on event-related potentials in a memory retrieval paradigm. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 42,381–388.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W. T., Pfefferbaum, A., KeUy, A. R, Berger, A., & Koppell, B. S. (1981). P3 reduction in auditory evoked potentials in schizophrenics. Psychiatry Research, 4,199–212.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, A. F. (1975). The foreperiod revisited. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27,591–598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, A. F. (1977). Structural and functional aspects of the reaction process. In S. Domic (Ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. 6, pp. 3–25). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, A. F. (1980). Some effects of instructed muscle tension on choice reaction time and movement. In R. S. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and Performance (Vol. 8, pp. 59–74). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, A. F. (1983). Towards a model of stress and human performance.Acta Psychologica, 53,61–97.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, A. F. (1988). Some issues and trends in the debate on discrete vs. continuous processing of information.Paper presented at the symposium on Human information processing: Continuous versus discrete stage discussion, Amsterdam, September 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satterfield, J. H., & Dawson, W. E. (1971). Electrodermal correlates of hyperactivity in children. Psychophysiology, 8,191–198.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schachar, R., Logan, G., Wachsmuth, R., & Chajczyk, D. (1988). Attaining and maintaining preparation: A comparison of attention in hyperactive, normal, and disturbed control children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16,361–370.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schleifer, M., Weiss, G., Cohen, N., Elman, M., Dvejec, H., & Kruger, E. (1975). Hyperactivity in preschoolers and the effect of methylphenidate. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 45,38–50.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W, & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: 1. Detection, search & attention. Psychological Review, 84,1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergeant, J. A. (1981). Attentional studies in hyperactivity.Doctoral dissertation, Groningen, Veenstra-Visser.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergeant, J. A., & Scholten, C. A. (1983). A stages-of-information approach to hyperactivity. Journal Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24,49–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergeant, J. A., & Scholten, C. A. (1985a). On resource strategy limitations in hyperactivity: Cognitive impulsivity reconsidered. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26,97–109.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sergeant, J. A., & Scholten, C. A. (1985b). On data limitations in hyperactivity. Journal Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26,111–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergeant, J. A. (1988). From DSM attentional deficit disorder to functional defects. In L. F. Bloomingdale & J. A. Sergeant (Eds.), Attentional deficit disorder (Vol. 5, pp. 183–198). Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergeant, J. A., & Meere, J. van der. (1988). What happens after a hyperactive commits an error? Psychiatric Research, 28,157–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shagass, C., & Schwartz, M. (1975). Visual cerebral evoked response characteristics in a psychiatric population. American Journal of Psychiatry, 112,979–987.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: 1. Detection, Search and Attention. Psychological Review, 84,1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sostek, A. J., Buchsbaum, M. S., & Rapoport, J. L. (1980). Effects of amphetamine on vigilance performance in normal and hyperactive children. Jourrml of Abnormal Child Psychology, 8,491–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprague, R. L., & Sleator, E. K. (1975). What is the proper dose of stimulant drugs in children? Interrmtional Journal of Mental Health, 4,75–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprague, R. L., & Sleator, E. K. (1977). Methylphenidate in hyperkinetic children: Differences in dose effects on learning and social behavior. Science, 198,1274–1276.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sprague, R. L., Barnes, K. R., & Werry, J. S. (1970). Methylphenidate and thioridazine: Learning, reaction time, and classroom behavior in disturbed children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 40,615–628.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Squires, N. K., Squires, K. C., & Hillyard, S. A. (1975). Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 38,387–401.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Squires, N. K., Donchin, E., Herning, R. J., & McCarthy, G. (1977). On the influence of task relevance and stimulus probability on event-related potential components. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 42,1–14.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1969). Discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donder’s method. In W. G. Koster (Ed.),Attention and Performance (Vol. 2, pp. 276–315). Amsterdam: Noord-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1975). Memory scanning: New findings and current controversies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27,1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, D. A., Boydstun, J. A., Dykman, R. A., Peters, J. E., & Sinton, D. W. (1967). Presumed minimal brain dysfunction in children: Relationship to performance and selected behavioral tests. Archives General Psychiatry, 16,281–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, D. A., Stover, C. R, & Backus, J. T (1970). The hyperkinetic chĂĽd: Effects of incentives on the speed of rapid tapping. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34,56–59.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, J., Lewis, J. L., Klorman, R., Peloquin, L. J., Perlmutter, R. A., & Salzman, L. F. (1984). Effects of methylphenidate on young adults’ performance and event-related potentials in a vigilance and a paired-associates learning test. Psychophysiology, 21,609–621.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, S., Braren, M., Zubin, J., & John, E. R. (1965). Evoked potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. Science, 150,1187–1188.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, J. M., & Briggs, G. E. (1969). Information processing as a function of speed versus accuracy. Journal Experimental Psychology, 81,223–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swets, J., & Kristofferson, A. B. (1970). Attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 21,339–366.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sykes, D. H., Douglas, V. I., Weiss, G., & Minde, K. K. (1971). Attention in hyperactive children and the effect of methylphenidate (Ritalin). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 12,129–1139.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sykes, D. H., Douglas, V. I., & Morgenstern, G. (1973). Sustained attention in hyperactive children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14,213–220.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tecce, J. J., &: Cole, O. (1974). Amphetamine effects in man: Electrical drowsiness and lowered electrical brain activity (CNV). Science, 185,451–453.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Timsit-Berthier, M., Delanouy, J., Koninckx, N., & Rosseau, J. C. (1973). Slow potential changes in psychiatry: 1. The contingent negative variation.Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 35,355–361.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warm, J. S., & Jersion, H. J. (1989). The psychophysics of vigilance. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 15–59). Chicester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weingartner, H., Rapoport, J. L., Buchsbaum, M. S., Bunney, W. E., Ebert, M. H., Mikkelsen, E. J., & Caine, E. D. (1980). Cognitive processes in normal and hyperactive children and their response to amphetamine treatment. Journal Abnormal Psychology, 89,25–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, B., & Laties, V. G. (1962). Enhancement of human performance by caffeine and the amphetamines. Pharmacological Review, 14,1–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werry, J. S., & Aman, M. G. (1975). Methylphenidate and haloperidol in children: Effects on attention, memory and activity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32,790–795.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weny, J. S., & Sprague, R. (1974). Methylphenidate in children—Effect of dosage. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 8,9–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winsum, W. van. Sergeant, J. A., & Geuze, R. (1984). The functional significance of event-related desynchronization of alpha rhythm in attentional and activating tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 58,519–524.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yellot, J. I. (1971). Correcting for fast guessing and the speed accuracy trade-off in choice reaction time. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 8,159–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zentall, S. S., & Meyer, M. J. (1987). Self-regulation of stimulation for ADD-H chĂĽdren during reading and vigilance task performance. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15,519–536.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1990 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Sergeant, J., van der Meere, J. (1990). Convergence of Approaches in Localizing the Hyperactivity Deficit. In: Lahey, B.B., Kazdin, A.E. (eds) Advances in Clinical Child Psychology. Advances in Clinical Child Psychology, vol 13. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9835-6_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9835-6_6

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-9837-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4613-9835-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics