Skip to main content

A Biological Basis for Individual Differences in Learning to Speak

  • Chapter
The Many Faces of Imitation in Language Learning

Part of the book series: Springer Series in Language and Communication ((SSLAN,volume 24))

Abstract

Children go about learning language in different ways and at different rates. Brown’s (1973) Eve had utterances averaging more than four morphemes when she was a little over 2 years old; Adam’s utterances did not even quite reach that length when he was 3 years and 8 months old. Are such variations due to different neurological organization, different general cognitive ability, or different kinds and amounts of language input? Most likely, all three factors play a role in the variations observed in learning to speak and in the speaking skill eventually attained. Nevertheless, if the contribution of each could be isolated, it would help us to understand how language is acquired and help us to compare language learning to the learning of other cognitive skills.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A.D. (1976). The psychology of memory. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A.D., & Hitch, G.J. (1974). Working memory. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 12, pp. 47–90). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bannatyne, A. (1976). Language, reading and learning disabilities: Psychology, neuropsychology, diagnosis and remediation. Springfield, IL: Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J.K. (1982). Toward a cognitive psychology of syntax: Information processing contributions to sentence formulation. Psychological Review, 89, 1–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P.E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read—A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I., McNew, S., Snyder, L., & Bates, E. (1983). Individual differences at 20 months: Analytic and holistic strategies in language acquisition, Journal of Child Language,10, 293–320.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R., & Fraser, C. (1963). The acquisition of syntax. In C.N. Cofer & B. Musgrave (Eds.), Verbal behavior and learning: Problems and processes (pp. 158–201). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P.E. (1986, April). Phonological awareness, rhyme, and reading. Symposium presentation at the Annual American Educational Research Association Meeting, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B. (1984). On teaching articulatory phonetics via an orthography. Memory & Cognition,12, 181–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, R. (1977). What’s the use of imitation? Journal of Child Language,4, 341–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. (1983). Human laterality. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M.C. (1980). Laterality and myth. American Psychologist,35, 284–295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, M. (1981). Dyslexia: An overview. In G.Th. Pavlidis & T.R. Miles (Eds.), Dyslexia research and its applications to education (pp. 1–34). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Ajuriaguerra, J., Jaeggi, A., Guignard, E, Kocher, F., Macquard, M., Roth, S., & Schmid, E. (1976). The development and prognosis of dysphasia in children. In D.M. Morehead & A.E. Morehead (Eds.), Normal and deficient child language (pp. 345385). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, M. (1984). Disorders of articulation. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ervin, S.M., & Osgood, S.E. (1954). Second language learning and bilingualism. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 139–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feagans, L. (1986). Otitis media in children: A model of effects and implications for intervention. In J. Kavanaugh (ed.), Otitis media and child development (pp. 192–208). Parkton, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feagans, L., Blood, I., & Tubman, J.G. (in press). Otitis media: Models of effects and implications for intervention.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feagans, L., Sanyal, M., Henderson, E, Collier, A., & Appelbaum, M. (in press). Middle ear disease in early childhood and later language skills. Journal of Pediatric Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finitzo, T., & Friel-Patti, S. (1987, April). Assessment of hearing in infants and young children: The otitis media quandary. Symposium presentation at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore, MD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friel-Patti, S., Finitzo-Heber, T., Conti, G., & Brown, C.K. (1982). Language delay in infants associated with middle ear disease and mild, fluctuating hearing impairment. Pediatric Infectious Disease, 1, 104–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A.M. (1986, December). Keynote Address to the Convention of the Hawaii Dyslexia Association, Honolulu, HI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. (1987). Thinking in the two cerebral hemispheres. Plenary presentation at the Third International Conference on Thinking, Honolulu, HI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N., & Behan, P. (1984). Laterality, hormones and immunity. In N. Geschwind & A.M. Galaburda (Eds.), Biological foundations of cerebral dominance (pp. 211–224). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimm, H. (1983). On the interrelation of internal and external factors in the development of language structures in normal and dysphasic preschoolers: A longitudinal study (Occasional Paper No. 5). Honolulu, HI: The Kamehameha Schools.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimm, H. (1984). Zur Frage der spachlichen Wissenskonstruction. Erwerben dysphasische Kinder die Sprache anders? In E. Oskar (ed.), Spracherwerb-sprachkontakt-Sprachkonflikt (pp. 30–53). Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimm, H. (1986). Developmental dysphasia: New theoretical perspectives and empirical results. The German Journal of Psychology, 11, 8–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimm, H., & Weinert, S. (1987, July). Deviant language processing in dysphasic children. Paper presented at the Fourth International Congress for the Study of Child Language, Lund, Sweden.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardcastle, W.J. (1976). Physiology of speech. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heath, R.W., Plett, J.D., & Tibbetts, K.A. (1987). Some significant educational, ethnic, and social correlates of mild hearing dysfunction in preschoolers. Honolulu, HI: Center for Development of Early Education, Kamhehameha Schools.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J.A., Tuller, B., & Harris, K.S. (1983). A “dynamic pattern” perspective on the control and coordination of movement. In P.F. MacNeilage (ed.), The production of speech (pp. 137–173). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A.V. & Mattingly, LG. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition, 21, 1–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindamood, P. (1985, December). Cognitively exploring the speech-spelling-reading link. Symposium presentation at the 35th Annual National Reading Conference, San Diego, California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I., Frost, J., & Peterson, O. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacNeilage, P.F. (1970). Motor control of serial ordering of speech. Psychological Review, 77, 182–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V.A. (1984). Reading skill and language skill. Developmental Review, 4, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V.A., & Liberman, I.Y. (1984). Phonological awareness and verbal short-term memory. Journal Learning Disabilities,17, 592–599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marchant, C.D., Shuring, P.A., Turczyk, V.A., Wasikowski, D.E., Tutihasi, M.A., & Kinney, S.E. (1984). Course and outcome of otitis media in early infancy: A prospective study. The Journal of Pediatrics,102, 826–831.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menyuk, P. (1978). Linguistic problems in children with developmental dysphasia. In M.A. Wyke (Ed.), Developmental dysphasia. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menyuk, P. (1986). Predicting speech and language problems with persistent otitis media. In J.F. Kavanagh (Ed.), Otitis media and child development (pp. 83–96). Parkton: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menyuk, R., & Looney, P.L. (1976). A problem of language disorder: Length versus structure. In D.M. Morehead & A.E. Morehead (Eds.), Normal and deficient child language (pp. 259–279). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morehead, D.M., & Ingram, D. (1976). The development of base syntax in normal and linguistically deviant children. In D.M. Morehead & A.E. Morehead (Eds.), Normal and deficient child language (pp. 209–238). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 48(149).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K.E. (1987). Some observations from the perspective of the rare event cognitive comparison theory of language acquisition. In K.E. Nelson & A. van Kleeck (Eds.), Children’s language (Vol. 6, pp. 289–331). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K.E., Baker, N.D., Denninger, M., Bonvillian, J.D., & Kaplan, B.J. (1985). Cookie versus do-it-again: Imitative-referential and person-social-syntactic-initiating language styles in young children. Linguistics, 23, 433–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, A. (1977). Language learning strategies: Does the whole equal the sum of the parts? Language, 53, 560–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, M.B. (1968). Developmental language disability: Adult accomplishments of dyslexic boys. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University Press. (Reprinted in paperback, 1978, Cambridge, MA: Educators Publishing Service.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, M.B. (1981). A diversity model for dyslexia. In G.T. Pavlidis & T.R. Miles (Eds.), Dyslexia research and its applications to education (pp. 13–34). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, G.D., Sherman, G.F., & Galaburda, A.M. (1986). Biological interactions in dyslexia. In J.R. Obrzut & G.W. Hynd (Eds.), Child neurology, Vol. 1, Theory and research (pp. 155–173). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E., & McClelland, J.L. (1987). Learning the past tenses of English verbs: Implicit rules or parallel distributed processing. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 195–248). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, R.H., Stool, S.E., Rodriguez, W.J., & Grundfast, K.M. (1981). Acute otitis media: Toward a more precise definition. Clinical Pediatrics, 20, 549–554.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Speidel, G.E., & Brandt, B. (1987, April). Individual differences in learning to speak and to read. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore, MD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speidel, G.E., & Power, M. (1987, April). A cornucopia of language skills in learning to read. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore, MD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K., Cunningham, A.E., & Cramer, B.B. (1984). Assessing phonological awareness in kindergarten children: Issues of task comparability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 175–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 361–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. (1977). Alternative conceptualizations of dyslexia: Evidence in support of a verbal-deficit hypothesis. Harvard Educational Review, 47, 334–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinreich, W. (1953). Language in contact. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Speidel, G.E. (1989). A Biological Basis for Individual Differences in Learning to Speak. In: Speidel, G.E., Nelson, K.E. (eds) The Many Faces of Imitation in Language Learning. Springer Series in Language and Communication, vol 24. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-1011-5_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-1011-5_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-6983-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-1011-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics