Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Springer Series in Language and Communication ((SSLAN,volume 24))

Abstract

The literature on imitation in language learning has been fraught not only with problems of what should be considered an imitation, but also with vague views of how imitation can help (for a discussion,see Oksaar, 1982; Snow, 1983). We must construct models of language learning in order to advance our understanding of that learning process. Predictions and hypotheses of how imitation can help should then be made from such models. These models should also be the basis for (1) definitions of subcategories of imitation and (2) the aspects of language chosen in order to study the effects of imitation.

Our minds seem especially tuned to pick out correlations of features in the environment and to develop categories around these correlations

J. R. Anderson

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Anderson, J.R. (1980). Cognitive psychology and its implications. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J.R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A.D. (1976). The psychology of memory. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A.D., & Hitch, G.J. (1974). Working memory. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psy-chology of learning and motivation, 8 (pp. 47–90). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C., & Engelmann, S. (1966). Teaching disadvantaged children in the preschool. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernthal, J.E., Greenlee, M., Eblen, R., & Marking, K. (1987). Detection of mispronunciations: A comparison of adults, normal-speaking children, and children with articulation errors. Applied Psycholinguistics, 8, 209–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D.F. (1985). The role of conceptual knowledge in the development of organization in children’s memory. In C.J. Brainerd & M. Pressley (Eds.), Basic processes in memory development: Progress in cognitive development research. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D.F. (1987). How age changes in knowledge base contribute to the development of children’s memory: An interpretive review. Developmental Review, 7, 93–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L., Hood, L., & Lightbown (1974). Imitation in language development: If, when and why. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 380–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L., Rocissano, L., & Hood, L. (1976). Adult-child discourse: Developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 521–552.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J.K. (1982). Toward a cognitive psychology of syntax: Information processing contributions to sentence formulation. Psychological Review, 89, 1–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolinger, D., & Sears, D. (1981). Aspects of language (3rd ed.). New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M. (1973). Early syntactic development: A cross-linguistic study with special reference to Finnish. London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M.D.S. (1963). On learning the grammatical order of words. Psychological Review, 70, 323–348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M.D.S. (1971). On two types of models of the internalization of grammars. In D.I. Slobin (Ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar: A theoretical symposium (pp. 153–186). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M.D.S. (1974). Length constraints, reduction rules, and holophrastic processes in children’s word combinations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 448–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M.D.S. (1976). Children’s first word combinations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (1, Serial No. 164).

    Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M.D.S. (1987). What is learned in acquiring word classes-A step toward an acquisition theory. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 65–87). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M.D.S., & Hardy, J.A. (1982). On what case categories there are, why they are, and how they develop: An amalgam of a priori considerations, speculations, and evidence from children. In E. Wanner & L.R. Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: State of the art (pp. 219–239). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R., & Fraser, C. (1963). The acquisition of syntax. In C.N. Cofer & B. Musgrave (Eds.), Verbal behavior and learning: Problems and processes (pp. 158–201). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, R. (1977). What’s the use of imitation? Journal of Child Language, 4, 341–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F.I.M., & Watkins, M.J. (1973). The role of rehearsal in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 599–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuvo, A.J. (1975). Developmental differences in rehearsal and free recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 19, 265–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Day, R.R., Boggs, S.T., Tharp, R.G., Gallimore, R., & Speidel, G.E. (1974). A standard English performance measure for young children: The Standard English Repetition Test (SERT). Working Papers in Linguistics, 6, 73–85. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F.N. (1981). Memory span: Sources of individual and developmental differences. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 63–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L.M., Horton, K.B., & Smith, J.O. (1968). Peabody language development kits: Manual for level # P Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, M. (1984). Disorders of articulation: Aspects of dysarthria and verbal dyspraxia. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ervin, S. (1964). Imitation and structural change in children’s language. In E.H. Lenneberg (Ed.), New directions in the study of language (pp. 163–189). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fromkin, V.A. (1968). Speculations of performance models. Journal of Linguistics, 4, 47–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grimm, H. (1983). On the interrelation of internal and external factors in the development of language structures in normal and dysphasic preschoolers: A longitudinal study (Occasional Paper No. 5). Honolulu, HI: The Kamehameha Schools.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimm, H. (1986). Developmental dysphasia: New theoretical perspectives and empirical results. The German Journal of Psychology, 11, 8–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardcastle, W.J. (1976). Physiology of speech. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D.O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J. (1974). The origins of language comprehensions. In E.R. Solso (Ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology (pp. 331–368). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J.A., Tuller, B., & Harris, K.S. (1983). A “dynamic pattern” perspective on the control and coordination of movement. In P.F. MacNeilage (Ed.), The production of speech (pp. 137–173). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kent, R.D. (1983). The segmental organization of speech. In P.F. MacNeilage (Ed.), The production of speech (pp. 57–89). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kuczaj, S.A. (1982). Language play and language acquisition. Advances in Child Development and Behavior,17, 197–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laver, J. (1980). Neurolinguistic control of speech production. In V.A. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L.B., Schwartz, R.G., Folger, M.K., & Wilcox, M.J. (1978). Journal of Child Language, 5, 403–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A.V. (1982). On finding that speech is special. American Psychologist, 37, 148–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A.V., & Mattingly, I.G. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition, 21, 1–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacNeilage, P.F. (1970). Motor control of serial ordering of speech. Psychological Review, 77, 182–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1985). Cognitive psychology: An essay in cognitive science. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maratsos, M.P. (1982). The child’s construction of grammatical categories. In E. Wanner & L.R. Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 240–266). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maratsos, M.P., & Chalkley, M.A. (1980). The internal language of children’s syntax: The ontogenesis and representation of syntactic categories. In K.E. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s language (Vol.2, pp. 127–214). New York: Gardner Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menyuk, P. (1969). Sentences children use. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menyuk, P. (1971). The acquisition and development of language. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menyuk, P., & Looney, P.L. (1976). A problem of language disorder: Length versus structure. In D.M. Morehead and A.E. Morehead (Eds.), Normal and deficient child language (pp. 259–279). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G.A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P.J. (1982). Amy, Wendy, and Beth: Learning language in South Baltimore. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moerk, E.L. (1977). Processes and products of imitation: Additional evidence that imitation is progressive. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 6, 187–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moerk, E.L. (1983). The mother of Eve-As a first language teacher. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naus, M.J., Ornstein, P.A., & Aivano, S. (1977). Developmental changes in memory: The relationship between rehearsal and memory as test expectations change. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 237–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 48(Serial No. 149).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K.E. (1987). Some observations from the perspective of the rare event cognitive comparison theory of language acquisition. In K.E. Nelson & A. van Kleeck (Eds.), Children’s language (Vol. 6, pp. 289–331). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K.E., Baker, N.D., Denninger, M., Bonvillian, J.D., & Kaplan, B.J. (1985). Cookie versus do-it-again: Imitative-referential and personal-social-syntactic-initiating language styles in young children. Linguistics, 23, 433–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K.E., & Bonvillian, J.D. (1978). Early semantic development: Conceptual growth and related processes between 2 and 41/2 years of age. In K.E. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s language (Vol. 1, pp. 467–556). New York: Gardner Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Netsell, R. (1986). A neurobiologic view of speech production and the dysarthrias. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B.B. (1982). Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories and their implications. Sociolinguistic Working Paper (No. 105). Austin, TX: Southwest Educational Development Laboratory.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oksaar, E. (1982). Language acquisition in the early years. New York: St. Martin’s Press. Translated from German. Spracherwerb im Vorschulalter (1977). Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oller, D.K., & MacNeilage, P.F. (1983). Development of speech production: Perspectives from natural and perturbed speech. In P.F. MacNeilage (Ed.), The production of speech (pp. 91–108). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ornstein, P.A., & Naus, M.J. (1978). Rehearsal processes in children’s memory. In P.A. Ornstein (Ed.), Memory development in children (pp. 69–99). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ornstein, P.A., Naus, M.J., & Stone, B.P. (1977). Rehearsal training and developmental differences in memory. Developmental Psychology, 13, 15–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, A.M. (1983). The units of language acquisition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1987). The bootstrapping problem in language acquisition. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 399–441).Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramer, A.L.H. (1976). The function of imitation in child language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 19, 700–717.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reger, Z. (1986). The functions of imitation in child language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 7, 323–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E., & McClelland, J.L. (1987). Learning the past tenses of English verbs: Implicit rules or parallel distributed processing. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 195–248). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rundus, D. (1971). Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 63–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rundus, D., & Atkinson, R.C. (1970). Rehearsal processes in free recall: A procedure for direct observation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 99–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schieffelin, B.B. (1981). A developmental study of pragmatic appropriateness of word order and casemarking in Kaluli. In W. Deutsch (Ed.), The child’s construction of language (pp. 105–120). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shatz, M. (1987). Bootstrapping operations in child language. In K.E. Nelson & A. van Kleek (Eds.), Children’s language (Vol. 6, pp. 1–22). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, A., Sinclair, H., & de Marcellus, O. (1971). Young children’s comprehension and production of passive sentences. Archives de Psychologie, 41, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, H. (1975). The role of cognitive structures in language acquisition. In E.H. Lenneberg & E. Lenneberg (Eds.), Foundations of language development (pp. 223–238). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair-de Zwart, H. (1967). Acquisition du langage et développement de la pensée. Paris: Dunod.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D.I. (1971). Data for the symposium. In D.I. Slobin (Ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar (pp. 3–14). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C.B. (1967). Articulation problems and abilities to store and process stimuli. Journal of Speech and Hearing, 10, 348–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C.E. (1981). The uses of imitation. Journal of Child Language, 8, 205–212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C.E. (1983). Saying it again: The role of expanded and deferred imitations in lan-guage acquisition. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s language (Vol. 4, pp. 29–58). Hills-dale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speidel, G.E. (1984). The acquisition of linguistic structures and cognitive development. In C.L. Thew & C.E. Johnson (Eds.), Proceedings of the Second International Congress for the Study of Child Language (Vol. 2, pp. 283–296). Lanham, MD: University Press of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speidel, G.E. (1987a). Language differences in the classroom: Two approaches to language development. In E. Oksaar (Ed.), Sociocultural perspectives of multilingualism and language acquisition (pp. 239–259). Tübingen: Gunter Narr Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speidel, G.E. (1987b). Conversation and language learning in the classroom. In K.E. Nelson & A. van Kleek (Eds.), Children’s language (Vol. 6, pp. 99–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speidel, G.E., & Herreshoff, M. (1985). Learning to speak standard English as a second dialect. In G.E. Speidel (Chair), The many faces of imitation. Symposium conducted at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, L. (1985). The structures and strategies of human memory. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, B.J., & Schulz, R.W. (1960). Meaningfulness and verbal learning. Philadelphia: Lippincott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson-Gegeo, K.A., & Gegeo, D.W. (1986). Some aspects of calling out and repeating routines in Kwara’ae children’s language acquisition. In B.B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization across cultures. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G.J., Ironsmith, E.M., & Goldfein, M. (1974). Selective imitation of the passive construction through modeling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 17, 288–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G.J., & Vasta, R. (1975). Is language acquired through imitation? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 37–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winitz, H. (1984). Auditory considerations in articulation training. In H. Winitz (Ed.), Treating articulation disorders: For clinicians by clinicians (pp. 21–49). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, A.E., Bjork, R.A., & Jongeward, R.H. (1974). Recall and recognition as a function of primary rehearsal. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 608–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Speidel, G.E. (1989). Imitation: A Bootstrap for Learning to Speak?. In: Speidel, G.E., Nelson, K.E. (eds) The Many Faces of Imitation in Language Learning. Springer Series in Language and Communication, vol 24. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-1011-5_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-1011-5_7

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-6983-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-1011-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics