Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Springer Series in Language and Communication ((SSLAN,volume 24))

Abstract

In reminiscence at least, Sartre displays a clear awareness of others’ imitations of his utterances and of his own production “recipe” that includes whole borrowed sentences. Moreover, Sartre seems to reach back and in autobiographical reconstruction show deferred imitation and reworking of particular verbal productions from his childhood language-learning days. So, Sartre has done us the favor of introducing this book on young children who are learning language and their conversational partners. Its concerns are who imitates whom, when, with what awareness, with what purpose, and with what impact.

I make childish remarks, they are remembered, they are repeated to me. I learn to make others. I make grown-up remarks. I know how to say thins “beyond my years” without meaning to. These remarks are poems. The recipe is simple: you must trust to the Devil, to chance, to emptiness, you borrow whole sentences from grown-ups, you string them together and repeat them without understanding them. In short, I pronounce true oracles, and each adult interprets them as he wishes.

Jean-Paul Sartre, 1964

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Anderson, J.R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, N., & Nelson, K.E. (1984). Recasting and related conversational techniques for triggering syntactic advances by young children. First Words, 5, 3–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, N., Pemberton, E.F., & Nelson, K.E. (1985, October). Facilitating young children’s language development through stories: Reading and recasting. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development

    Google Scholar 

  • Boston, MA. Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Bretherton, I., & Snyder, L. (1988). From first words to grammar. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D.F. (1987). A note on neonatal imitation. Developmental Review, 7, 86–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L., Hood, L., & Lightbown, P. (1974). Imitation in language development: If, when, and why? Cognitive Psychology, 6, 380–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonvillian, J.D., Nelson, K.E., & Charrow, V.R. (1976). Languages and language-related skills in deaf and hearing children. Sign Language Studies, 12, 211–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R., & Fraser, C. (1963). The acquisition of syntax. In C. Cofer & B.S. Musgrave (Eds.), Verbal behavior and learning: Problems and processes (pp. 158–197). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of Verbal Behavior by B.F. Skinner. Language, 35, 26–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1968). Language and mind. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, R. (1977). What’s the use of imitation? Journal of Child Language, 4, 341–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook-Gumperz, J. (1985, May). The child’s acquisition of languages and world views. Paper presented to the Georgetown Roundtable on Linguistics, Georgetown University, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • English, H.B., & English, A.C. (1958). A comprehensive dictionary of psychological and psychoanalytical terms. New York: David McKay Company, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ervin, S.M. (1964). Imitation and structural change in children’s language. In E.H. Lenneberg (Ed.), New directions in the study of language (pp. 163–189). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ervin Tripp, S.M. (1971). An overview of theories of grammatical development. In D.I. Slobin (Ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar (pp. 190–212). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fourcin, A.J. (1975). Language development in the absence of expressive speech. In E.H. Lenneberg & E. Lenneberg (Eds.), Foundations of language development: A multidisciplinary approach (Vol. 2, pp. 263–268). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraser, C., Bellugi, U., & Brown, R. (1963). Control of grammar in imitation, comprehension and production. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 121–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gleitman, H. (1981). Psychology (1st ed.). New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. (1988, July 4). The New Yorker,61–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh-Pasek, K., Treiman, R., & Schneiderman, M. (1984). Brown & Hanlon revisited: Mothers’ sensitivity to ungrammatical forms. Journal of Child Language, 11, 81–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J.J., & Palermo, D. (1964). Mediation processes and the acquisition of linguistic structure. In U. Bellugi & R. Brown (Eds.). The acquisition of language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 29–1, 141–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, E.O. (1977). Making it last: Repetition in children’s discourse. In S.M. Ervin-Tripp & C. Mitchell-Kernan (Eds.), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimble, G.A. (1961). Hilgard and Marquis’ conditioning and learning (2nd ed). NewYork: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucjaz, S.A. (1982). Language play and language acquisition. In H.W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 17, pp. 197–232). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A.V., & Mattingly, I.G. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition, 21, 1–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1966). On theories of language acquisition. In F. Smith & G.A. Miller (Eds.), The genesis of language: A psycholinguistic approach (pp. 15–84). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McTear, M. (1985). Children’s conversation. New York: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N., & Moore, M.K. (1983). The origins of imitation in infancy: Paradigm, phenomena, and theories. In L.P. Lipsitt (Ed.), Advances in infancy research (Vol.2, pp. 265–301). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moerk, E.L. (1977). Processes and products of imitation: Additional evidence that imitation is progressive. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 6, 187–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moerk, E.L., & Moerk, C. (1979). Quotations, imitations and generalizations. Factual and methodological analyses. International Journal of Behavioral Development,5, 95–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1981). Individual differences in language development. Developmental Psychology, 17, 170–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K.E. (1977). Facilitating children’s syntax acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 13, 101–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K.E. (1980). Theories of the child’s acquisition of syntax: A look at rare events and at necessary, catalytic, and irrelevant components of mother-child conversation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 345, 45–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K.E. (1987). Some observations from the perspective of the rare event cognitive comparison theory of language acquisition. In K.E. Nelson & A. van Kleeck (Eds.), Children’s language (Vol. 6, pp. 289–331). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K.E. (1988). Strategies for first language teaching. In R. Schiefelbusch & M. Rice (Eds.), The teachability of language. Baltimore, MD: Dan Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K.E., Baker, N.D., Denninger, M.M., Bonvillian, J.D., & Kaplan, B.J. (1985). Cookie versus do-it-again: Imitative-referential and person-social-syntactic-initiating language styles in children. Linguistics, 23, 443–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K.E., & Bonvillian, J.D. (1978). Early semantic development: Conceptual growth and related processes between 2 and 4 1/2 years of age. In K.E. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s language (Vol. 1, pp. 467–556). New York: Gardner Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs.,E. & Schieffelin, B.B. (1982). Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories and their implications. Sociololinguistic Working Paper, No. 105. Austin, TX: Southwest Educational Development Laboratory.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oksaar, E. (1982). Language acquisition in the early years. New York: St. Martin’s Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oksaar, E.Translated from the German, Spracherwerb im Vorschulalter (1977). Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1962). Play,dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton. (Original work published in 1946.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramer, A.L.H. (1976). The function of imitation in child language. Journal of Speech and Hearing, 19, 700–717.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reger, Z. (1986). The functions of imitation in child language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 7, 323–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rummelhart, D.E., & McClelland, J.L. (1987). Learning the past tenses of English verbs: Implicit rules or parallel distributed processing. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.) Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 195–248). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartre, J.P. (1964). The words (p. 19). New York: Fawcett World Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, R.G., & Leonard, L.B. (1985). Lexical imitation and language acquisition in language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 50, 141–149.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B.F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. New York: Appleton/Century Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D.I. (1968). Imitation and grammatical development in children. In N.S. Endler, L.R. Boulter, & H. Osser (Eds.), Contemporary issues in developmental psychology (pp. 437–443). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D.I. (Ed.). (1971). The ontogenesis of grammar. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C.E. (1981). The uses of imitation. Journal of Child Language, 8, 205–212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C.E. (1983). Saying it again: The role of expanded and deferred imitations in lan-guage acquisition. In K.E. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s language (Vol 4. pp. 187–230). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speidel, G.E. (1987). Conversation and language learning in the classroom. In K.E. Nelson and A. van Kleeck (Eds.), Children’s language (Vol. 6, pp. 99–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staats, A.W. (1968). Learning, language & cognition. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Statts, A.W. (1971a). Child learning, intelligence and personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staats, A.W. (1971b). Linguistic-mentalistic theory versus an explanatory S-R learning theory of language development. In D.I. Slobin (Ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar (pp. 103–150). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stine, E.L., & Bohannon, J.N. (1983). Imitations, interactions, and language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 10, 589–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E.C., Ritchie, B.F., and Kalish, D. (1946). Studies in spatial learning: II. Place learning versus response learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36, 221–229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uzgiris, J.C. (1984). Imitation in infancy: Its interpersonal aspects. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 17, pp. 1–32). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson-Gegeo, K.A., & Gegeo, D.W. (1986). Some aspects of calling out and repeating routines in Kwara’ae children’s language acquisition. In B.B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization across cultures. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G.J., & Vasta, R. (1975). Is language acquired through imitation? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 37–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Speidel, G.E., Nelson, K.E. (1989). A Fresh Look at Imitation in Language Learning. In: Speidel, G.E., Nelson, K.E. (eds) The Many Faces of Imitation in Language Learning. Springer Series in Language and Communication, vol 24. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-1011-5_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-1011-5_1

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-6983-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-1011-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics