Skip to main content

The Goal-Free Effect

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Cognitive Load Theory

Abstract

The goal-free effect was the first instructional effect investigated within a ­cognitive load theory framework. Goal-free problems occur when a conventional problem with a specific goal is replaced by a problem with a non-specific goal. For example, in high school geometry, a typical problem will ask students to calculate a specific angle, such as angle ABC. In contrast, goal-free problems will not require students to specifically calculate this angle, but use a more general wording such as ‘calculate the value of as many angles as you can’. This particular wording of the problem will still allow students to calculate the targeted angle of the conventional problem (angle ABC), but students are free to calculate as many other angles as they can, and are not required to focus on one ultimate goal. Goal-free problems are ­sometimes called no-goal problems, and the goal-free effect is sometimes referred to as the goal-specificity effect. Consider an example taken from the domain of geometry. The goal-free effect occurs when students, having solved goal-free ­problems with an instruction to ‘calculate the value of as many angles as you can’ during acquisition, demonstrate superior learning outcomes to students who have solved the equivalent, conventional problems that include a goal such as ‘calculate the value of angle ABC’.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ayres, P. L. (1993). Why goal-free problems can facilitate learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 18, 376–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ayres, P. (1998). The effectiveness of a no-goal approach in solving Pythagorean problems. In C. Kanes, M. Goos, & E. Warren (Eds.), Published Proceedings of the Mathematical Education Research Group of Australasia (MERGA 21) (pp. 68–73). Gold Coast, Australia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ayres, P., & Sweller, J. (1990). Locus of difficulty in multistage mathematics problems. The American Journal of Psychology, 103, 167–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berry, D. C., & Broadbent, D. E. (1984). On the relationship between task performance and associated verbalizable knowledge. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 36A, 209–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobis, J., Sweller, J., & Cooper, M. (1994). Demands imposed on primary-school students by geometric models. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19, 108–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burns, B. D., & Vollmeyer, R. (2002). Goal specificity effects on hypothesis testing in problem solving. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 55A, 241–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geddes, B. W., & Stevenson, R. J. (1997). Explicit learning of a dynamic system with a non-salient pattern. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 50A, 742–765.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, T. E., Johnson, P. J., & Acton, W. H. (1991). Assessing structural knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 88–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. (1978). Natures of problem-solving abilities. In W. K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol. 5, pp. 239–270). Oxford/England: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, S. G., & Staveland, L. E. (1988). Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of empirical and theoretical research. In P. A. Hancock & N. Meshkati (Eds.), Human mental workload (Advances in psychology, Vol. 52, pp. 139–183). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J., McDermott, J., Simon, D. P., & Simon, H. A. (1980a). Expert and novice performance in solving physics problems. Science, 208, 1335–1342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mawer, R. F., & Sweller, J. (1982). Effects of subgoal density and location on learning during problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 252–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, C. S., Lehman, J. F., & Koedinger, K. R. (1999). Goals and learning in microworlds. Cognitive Science, 23, 305–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osman, M. (2008). Observation can be as effective as action in problem solving. Cognitive Science, 32, 162–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owen, E., & Sweller, J. (1985). What do students learn while solving mathematics problems? Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 272–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., Camp, G., & Rikers, R. (2001). Instructional compensation for age-related cognitive declines: Effects of goal specificity in maze learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 181–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A., & Lea, G. (1974). Problem solving and rule induction: A unified view. Knowledge and cognition. In L. W. Gregg (Ed.), Knowledge and cognition. Oxford/England: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, D. P., & Simon, H. A. (1978). Individual differences in solving physics problems. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? (pp. 325–348). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (1983). Control mechanisms in problem solving. Memory and Cognition, 11, 32–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12, 257–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., & Levine, M. (1982). Effects of goal specificity on means–ends analysis and learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 463–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Mawer, R. F., & Howe, W. (1982). Consequences of history-cued and means–end strategies in problem solving. The American Journal of Psychology, 95, 455–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Mawer, R. F., & Ward, M. R. (1983). Development of expertise in mathematical problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 639–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trumpower, D. L., Goldsmith, T. E., & Guynn, M. J. (2004). Goal specificity and knowledge acquisition in statistics problem solving: Evidence for attentional focus. Memory and Cognition, 32, 1379–1388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vollmeyer, R., & Burns, B. D. (2002). Goal specificity and learning with a hypermedia program. Experimental Psychology, 49, 98–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vollmeyer, R., Burns, B. D., & Holyoak, K. J. (1996). The impact of goal specificity on strategy use and the acquisition of problem structure. Cognitive Science, 20, 75–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wirth, J., Künsting, J., & Leutner, D. (2009). The impact of goal specificity and goal type on learning outcome and cognitive load. Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 299–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John Sweller .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Sweller, J., Ayres, P., Kalyuga, S. (2011). The Goal-Free Effect. In: Cognitive Load Theory. Explorations in the Learning Sciences, Instructional Systems and Performance Technologies, vol 1. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-8126-4_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics