Skip to main content

Further Insight into Cognitive and Metacognitive Processes of the Tip-of-the-Tongue State with an Amnesic Drug as Cognitive Tool

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Trends and Prospects in Metacognition Research

Abstract

The tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) state may be viewed as temporary and reversible micro-amnesia. Amnesic drugs, such as benzodiazepines, may be used as tools to reveal functional principles of normal cognitive processes. We investigated the effects of lorazepam on TOT state. With general information questions, Lorazepam participants provided more commission errors and more TOT states following commissions than placebo participants, whereas the resolution of the TOTs was unimpaired. The higher amount of incorrect recalls provided by lorazepam participants may partially result from the fact that they are more frequently in a state of transitory inaccessibility of a known item, identifiable as a commission TOT (participants provide a persistent alternate and experience the phenomenological TOT only after having been informed of the error). This way of resolving the TOT conflict is discussed in light of the anxiolytic effect of the drug. Lorazepam led the participants to generate more persistent alternates when in a TOT while preserving the cognitive process of TOT, that is, the temporary failure to retrieve a known word.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The Moses illusion is as follows: When asked “How many animals of each kind did Moses take on the ark?” people fail to notice the distortion introduced by the impostor “Moses” and respond “two”. This semantic illusion, which is known as the Moses illusion, has proved to be quite robust and can be generalized across other materials and conditions.

References

  • Allen, D., Curran, H. V., & Lader, M. (1993). The effects of single doses of CL 284, 846, lorazepam, and placebo on psychomotor and memory function in normal male volunteers. European Journal Clinical Pharmacology, 45, 313–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bacon, E., Danion, J. M., Kauffman-Muller, F., Schelstraete, M. A., Bruant, A., Sellal, F., & Grange, D. (1998). Confidence level and feeling of knowing for episodic and semantic memory: An investigation of lorazepam effects on metamemory. Psychopharmacology, 138, 318–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bacon E., Schwartz B., Paire-FicoutM., & Izaute M. (2007). Dissociation between the cognitive process and the phenomenological experience of TOT: Effect of the anxiolytic drug lorazepam on TOT states. Conscious Cognition, 16, 360–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bäckman L., Lipinska B. (1993). Monitoring of general knowledge: Evidence for preservation in early Alzheimer’s disease (1993). Neuropsychologia, 31, 335–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, M. J., Greenwood, K. M., Jackson, M., & Crowe, S. F. (2004). Cognitive effects of long-term benzodiazepine use: A meta-analysis. CNS Drugs, 18, 37–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beracochea, D. (2006). Anterograde and retrograde effects of benzodiazepines on memory. ScientificWorld Journal, 16, 1460–1465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biedermann, B., Ruh, N., Nickels, L., & Coltheart, M. (2008). Information retrieval in tip of the tongue states: New data and methodological advances. Journal of Psycholinguist Research, 37, 171–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brennen, T., Vikan, A., & Dybdahl, R. (2007). Are tip-of-the-tongue states universal? Evidence from the speakers of an unwritten language. Memory, 15, 167–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. S. (1991). A review of the tip-of-the-tongue experience. Psychological Bulletin,109, 204–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J., Brown, M. W., & Bowes, J. B. (1983). Effects of lorazepam on rate of forgetting, on retrieval from semantic memory and dexterity. Neuropsychologia, 21, 501–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R., & McNeill D. (1966). The tip of the tongue experience. Psychological Bulletin, 1090, 339–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burke, D. M., MacKay, D. G., Worthley, J. S., & Wade, E. (1991). On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults? Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 542–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curran, H. V. (1991). Benzodiazepines, memory and mood: A review. Psychopharmacology, 105, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curran, H. V. (1999). Effects of anxiolytics on memory. Human Psychopharmacology, 14, 72–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danion, J. M. (1994). Drugs as tools for investigating memory. European Neuropsycho-pharmacology, 4, 179–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duka, T., Curran, H. V., Rusted, J. M., & Weingartner, H. J. (1996). Perspectives on cognitive psychopharmacology research. Behavioral Pharmacology, 7, 401–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, T. D., & Mattson, M. E. (1981). From word to meaning: A semantic illusion. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 20, 540–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • File, S. E., Sharma, R., & Shaffer, J. (1992). Is lorazepam-induced amnesia specific to the type of memory or to the task used to assess it? Journal of Psychopharmacology, 61, 76–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flor, H. (2002). Phantom-limb pain: Characteristics, causes, and treatment. Lancet Neurology, 3, 182–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fluck, E., File, S. E., Springett, J., Kopelman, M. D., Rees, J., & Orgill, J. (1998). Does the sedation resulting from sleep deprivation and lorazepam cause similar cognitive deficits? Pharmacology Biochemistry Behavior, 59, 909–915.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giersch, A., Boucart, M., Elliott, M., & Vidailhet, P. (2010). Atypical behavioural effects of lorazepam: Clues to the design of novel therapies? Pharmacology and therapeutics, 126(1), 94–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, J. F., McElholm, A., & King, D. J. (1996). A comparison of the sedative and amnestic effects of chlorpromazine and lorazepam. Psychopharmacology, 128, 67–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harley, T. A., & Bown, H. E. (1998). What causes a tip-of-the-tongue state? Evidence for lexical neighbourhood effects in speech production. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 151–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, J. T. (1965). Memory and the feeling-of-knowing experience. Journal of Education and Psychology, 56, 208–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, S. C. (1980). Hypnotics and sedatives. In A. Goodman Gilman, L. S. Goodman, & A. Gilman (Eds.), The pharmacological basis of therapeutics (pp. 339–375). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Izaute, M., & Bacon, E. (2005). Specific effects of an amnesic drug: Effect of lorazepam on study time allocation and on judgment of learning. Neuropsychopharmacology, 30, 196–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Izaute, M., Paire-Ficout, L., & Bacon, E. (2004). Benzodiazepines and semantic memory: Effects of lorazepam on the Moses illusion. Psychopharmacology, 172, 309–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janowsky, J. S., Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1989a). Memory and metamemory: Comparisons between patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic patients. Psychobiology 17, 3–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janowsky, J. S., Shimamura, A. P, & Squire, L. R. (1989b). Source memory impairment in patients with frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 27, 1043–1056.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, G. V., & Langford, S. (1987). Phonological blocking in the tip-of-the tongue state. Cognition, 26, 115–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleven, M. S., & Koek, W. (1999). Effects of benzodiazepine agonists on punished responding in pigeons and their relationship with clinical doses in humans. Psychopharmacology, 141, 206–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1993). How do we know that we know? The accessibility model of the feeling of knowing. Psychological Review, 100, 609–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1995). Dissociating knowing and the feeling of knowing: Further Evidence for the Accessibility Model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 311–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1998). Illusions of knowing: The link between knowledge and metaknowledge. In V. Y. Yzerbyt, G. Lories, & B. Dardenne (Eds.), Metacognition: Cognitive and social dimensions (pp. 16–34). London: SAGE.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., & Goldsmith, M. (1996). Memory in naturalistic and laboratory contexts: Distinguish the accuracy-oriented and quantity-oriented approaches to memory assessment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 297–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maril, A., Simons, J. S., Weaver, J. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2005). Graded recall success: An event-related fMRI comparison of tip of the tongue and feeling of knowing. NeuroImage, 24, 1130–1138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Massin-Krauss, M., Bacon, E., & Danion, J. M. (2002). Effects of the benzodiazepine lorazepam on monitoring and control processes in semantic memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 11, 123–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matison, R., Mayeux, R., Rosen, J., & Fahn, S. (1982). “Tip-of-the-tongue” phenomenon in Parkinson disease. Neurology, 32, 567–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melzack, R., Coderre, T. J., Katz, J., & Vaccarino, A. L. (2001). Central neuroplasticity and pathological pain. Annual reports of New York Academy of Sciences, 933, 157–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mintzer, M., Kleykamp, B., & Griffiths, R. (2010). Dose effects of triazolam and scopolamine on metamemory. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 18(1), 17–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A. (1997). Retrieval of lexical-syntactic features in tip-of-the-tongue states. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1410–1423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O. (1984). A comparison of current measures of the accuracy of feeling-of-knowing predictions. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 109–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., McSpadden, M., Fromme, K., & Marlatt, G. A. (1986). Effects of alcohol intoxication on metamemory and on retrieval from long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 247–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1994). Why investigate metacognition? In J. Metcalfe & A. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 1–25). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norris, H. (1971). The action of sedatives on brain stem occulomotor system in man. Neuropharmacology, 10, 181–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pappas, B. A., Sunderland T., Weingartner, H. M., Vitiello, B., Martinson, H., & Putnam, K. (1992). Alzheimer’s disease and feeling-of-knowing for knowledge and episodic memory. The Journal of Gerontology. Series B: Psychological Sciences, 47, 159–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J., & Lucas, D. (1984). Using cognitive diaries to investigate naturally occuring memory blocks. In J. E. Harris & P. E. Morris (Eds.), Everyday memory, actions, and absentmindedness (pp. 53–70). London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reder, L. M., & Kusbit, G. W. (1991). Locus of the Moses illusion: Imperfect encoding, retrieval, or match? Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 385–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (1999). The seeven sins of memory. Insight from psychology and cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 54, 182–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L. (1998). Illusory tip-of-the-tongue states. Memory, 6, 623–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L. (1999). Sparkling at the end of the tongue: The etiology of tip-of-the-tongue phenomenology. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 6, 379–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L. (2001). The relation of tip-of-the-tongue states and retrieval time. Memory and Cognition, 229, 117–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L. (2002a). The phenomenology of naturally-occurring tip-of-the-tongue states: A diary study. In S. P. Shohov (Ed.), Advances in Psychology Research (Vol. 8, pp. 71–84). New York: Nova Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L. (2002b). Tip-of-the-tongue states. Phenomenology, mechanism, and lexical retrieval. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L. (2008). Working memory load differentially affects tip-of-the-tongue states and feeling-of-knowing judgments. Memory Cognition, 36, 9–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L., & Smith, S. M. (1997). The retrieval of related information influences tip-of-the-tongue states. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 68–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L., Travis, D. M., Castro, A. M., & Smith, M. S. (2000). The phenomenology of real and illusory tip-of-the-tongue states. Memory and Cognition, 28, 18–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1986). Memory and metamemory: A study of the feeling-of-knowing phenomenon in amnesic patients. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 452–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1988). Long-term memory in amnesia: Cued recall, recognition memory, and confidence ratings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 763–770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A. P. (1994). The neuropsychology of metacognition. In: J. Metcalfe & A. P. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 253–276). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M. (1994). Frustrated feelings of imminent recall: On the tip-of-the tongue. In J. Metcalfe & A. P. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 27–46). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, S. A. (2005). The effects of benzodiazepines on cognition. Journal Clinical Psychiatry, 66, 9–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. K., & MacKay, D. G. (2003). Tip-of-the-tongue phenomena: Goldmine or can of worms. Review of Tip-of-the-tongue states: Phenomenology, mechanism, and lexical retrieval. American Journal of Psychology, 116, 291–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanover, K.E., Robledo, S., Huber, M., & Carter, R. B. (1999). Pharmacological evaluation of a modified conflict procedure: Punished drinking in non-water-deprived rats. Psychopharmacology, 145, 333–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vermeeren, A., Jackson, J. L., Muntjewerff, N. D., Quint, P. J,. Harrison, E. M., & O’Hanlon, J. F. (1995). Comparison of acute alprazolam (0.25, 0.50 and 1.0 mg) effects versus those of lorazepam 2 mg and placebo on memory in healthy volunteers using laboratory and telephone tests. Psychopharmacology, 118, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vigliocco, G., Antonini, T., & Garrett, M. F. (1997). Grammatical gender is on the tip of Italian tongues. Psychological Science, 8, 314–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler memory scale – Revised manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Widner, R. L., Smith, S. M., & Graziano, W. G. (1996). The effects of demand characteristics on the reporting of tip-of-the-tongue states and feeling-of-knowing states. American Journal of Psychology, 109, 525–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by Inserm and by a Cognitique “Impairments and recovery of cognitive functions” grant from the French Ministry of Research. The author wishes to thank Marie Izaute, Bennett Schwartz, and Laurence Paire-Ficout for their valuable contribution to the TOT study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elisabeth Bacon .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bacon, E. (2010). Further Insight into Cognitive and Metacognitive Processes of the Tip-of-the-Tongue State with an Amnesic Drug as Cognitive Tool. In: Efklides, A., Misailidi, P. (eds) Trends and Prospects in Metacognition Research. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-6546-2_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics