Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development 4/2017

Open Access 12-10-2016 | Original Article

Callous-Unemotional Traits Moderate the Relation Between Prenatal Testosterone (2D:4D) and Externalising Behaviours in Children

Auteurs: Alyson Blanchard, Luna C. Munoz Centifanti

Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development | Uitgave 4/2017

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Children who exhibit callous-unemotional (CU) traits are identified as developing particularly severe forms of externalising behaviours (EB). A number of risk factors have been identified in the development of CU traits, including biological, physiological, and genetic factors. However, prenatal testosterone (PT) remains un-investigated, yet could signal fetal programming of a combination of CU/EB. Using the 2D:4D digit ratio, the current study examined whether CU traits moderated the relationship between PT and EB. Hand scans were obtained from 79 children aged between 5 and 6 years old whose parents completed the parent report ICU (Inventory of Callous Unemotional Traits) and SDQ (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire). CU traits were found to moderate the relationship between PT and EB so that children who were exposed to increased PT and were higher in CU traits exhibited more EB. Findings emphasize the importance of recognising that vulnerability for EB that is accompanied by callousness may arise before birth.

Introduction

Biological factors identify children with a profile of externalizing behaviours (EB) accompanied by callous-unemotional (CU) personality traits: lack of empathy, callous disregard for others’ wellbeing and their feelings, and a lack of responsibility and care over performance [13]. Biological factors may include exposure to hormones in the womb, which might set the stage for callous or cruelly perpetrated problem behaviors [46]. Given the relation between testosterone and psychopathy and our recent demonstration of an association between prenatal testosterone (PT; measured by the 2D:4D ratio) and primary and secondary psychopathy (callousness and the affective traits; antisocial and impulsive behaviours) [7], we investigate the possibility that exposure to PT is related to CU traits and EB. Research demonstrates that children with CU traits and EB evince biological profiles that are distinct from those children with CU traits alone or those with EB that are not accompanied by CU traits [2, 8]. Thus, we tested interactive effects of PT and CU traits on EB.
CU traits have been shown to differentiate distinct subgroups of children and adolescents with serious EB (e.g., conduct problems, conduct disorder, aggression and antisocial behaviour) [9]. For example, children with EB and CU traits have been characterized as temperamentally fearless with diminished emotionality, which is suggested to explain their propensity toward lifelong antisocial behavior [10, 11]. Children with CU traits and EB have reduced emotional and physiological reactivity that is not evident in children with EB alone [12, 13]. Their emotional deficits may be driven by a dysfunctional amygdala, given research showing reduced potentiated startle to violent images for those children with CU traits and EB [14]. Atypical neurological development may explain the hypoactivity to others’ distress, which impairs important brain regions for social and affective functioning [1517]. Further, EB with CU traits show substantial heritability, greater than the heritability estimates for EB without CU traits [18]. These biological influences suggest that a lack of emotional and physiological reactivity to fearful events could explain why children with CU traits are less receptive to learning as a result of punitive measures, hindering normative social development, and predisposing these children to lifelong antisocial behavior [19].
Although the psychophysiological and biological profile of youths with CU traits is relatively well researched [2022] the prenatal biology of children who later show CU traits is unknown. There is reason to speculate that PT exposure might play a role in the development of behaviours associated with psychopathy [23, 24]. For example, using the 2D:4D digit ratio as a biomarker for PT, research shows that high PT exposure is related to higher trait aggression [25], indirect and reactive aggression [2628] sensation seeking and boredom susceptibility [29], recreational, financial and social risk taking [30, 31], increased sensitivity to status cues [32] and dis-inhibition [33]. In contrast, higher levels of prenatal estrogen (PE) are associated with empathy [34, 35], pro-social behaviour [36], neuroticism [29] and anxiety [37]. Thus, the masculinisation of the brain before birth appears to have long-lasting consequences for psychopathic behaviours, starting from childhood into adulthood.
One causal factor implicated in the fluctuation of PT is maternal stress, which could provide a proximate environmental determinant for the development of psychopathic behaviours. Maternal stress is hypothesised to elevate levels of cortisol, which, via the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis, act on the adrenal, ovarian/testicular functioning of the fetus thereby stimulating the production of PT [3841]. Indeed, EB (e.g., aggression and sensation seeking) associated with higher levels of PT are also related to maternal anxiety [42, 43]. This may indicate a kind of “fetal programming”[44] whereby maternal stress acts as a cue that the environment outside the womb is stressful. Therefore, in-utero hormone levels may prompt masculinisation of the unborn infant’s brain to prepare them for a competitive environment. The child is then equipped with masculinsed traits and behaviours that are adaptive in the harsh environment that they are born into. However, studies also show that the 2D:4D ratio is moderately to highly heritable [45] and therefore, while PT is evidently an important contributor to the development of certain behaviours, both non-shared environmental factors and genetic influence should be taken into consideration.
Nevertheless, the relationship between testosterone and psychopathic behaviours is not entirely clear. For example, testosterone has been associated with impulsivity, and people with high testosterone readily activate aggressive coping strategies when provoked [46]. People with CU traits tend to show instrumental or planned aggressive behaviour rather than reactive or provoked aggression [4749]. However, ratios between testosterone and cortisol, specifically lower ratios (i.e., lower levels of testosterone to higher levels of cortisol), have been argued to be indicative of good, rather than abnormal amygdala functioning, which is characteristic of CU traits and primary psychopathy [50]. As expected, people with high ratios, indicating high levels of testosterone and low levels of cortisol, have been shown to be high on psychopathic traits [51, 52]. Interestingly, a similar finding has emerged from a 2D:4D ratio study of adolescent males in which those that had been exposed to higher levels of PT, low cortisol reactivity was associated with self-reported aggression and rule-breaking behaviour [53]. Thus, the relation between testosterone and CU traits may be complex and involves interacting hormonal systems.
In light of the current literature, we examined whether children between the ages of 5 and 6 years who were exposed to greater levels of PT expressed higher levels of CU traits and EB. Children at the age of 5–6 years are the age at which they enter school, and this group is of particular relevance to study because of developments in empathy, emotion understanding, and cognition that demonstrate extensive growth at this age [54]. Additionally, empathy and emotion understanding deficits have been found to be associated with CU traits at this age [55]. Children who are entering school are in a position to develop independence from their parents and therefore become susceptible to positive and negative peer influences [56, 57]. Furthermore, CU traits have yet to be examined in this particular age group. Previous studies have shown that CU traits emerge as early as age 2 years [58, 59] and remain relatively stable throughout childhood [5961]. Therefore, the influence of prenatal experiences in the development of CU traits and EB may be observable in our sample of children. Studies investigating “fetal programming” [7, 62] with regards to psychopathic and antisocial behaviour have also so far only concerned adults.
Therefore, based on our prior research [7] and that both genes and environmental (i.e., maternal stress) factors are implicated in the status of PT, we expected elevated levels of PT to be associated with higher CU traits and higher EB. Considering that the presence of CU traits combined with EB designates a unique group of children with serious EB, we hypothesized an interaction between exposure to increased PT and more CU traits in the expression of higher levels of EB.

Method

Participants and Procedure

Seventy-nine parents and children (48 girls) were recruited from five primary schools in the Merseyside area of the United Kingdom. Schools came from areas of varying socio-economic backgrounds as indicated by their Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) score, ranging from 3.40 to 47.93. Four children came from 10 % of the most deprived areas; 24 children came from the 20 % most deprived areas; nine children came from the 40 % most deprived areas and 43 children came from the 10 % least deprived areas in England. Children were in Year 1 of The British Education System and aged between 5 and 6 years.

Measures

Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (ICU)

The ICU Parent Report [63] is a 24-item questionnaire that assesses CU traits in children. Using a four-point Likert scale, the parent rates how true (0 = not at all true, 3 = definitely true) certain statements are of their child (e.g., “Does not show emotions” and “Does not care about doing things well”). Ratings are summed to produce an overall score of CU traits. Internal consistency was good and was improved by removing item 10 (“Does not let feelings control him/her”) (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.83), which is a consistent underperformer in prior research [64].

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)

The SDQ [65] is a 25-item questionnaire that screens for various positive and negative behaviours. Each subscale consists of five items, and we combined the Conduct Problems and Hyperactivity subscales to produce an overall Externalising score, as has been done in prior research [55]. On a three-point Likert scale, parents rated how true (0 = not true, 2 = certainly true) statements such as: “Often lies or cheats” (Conduct problems), and “Restless, overactive” (Hyperactivity) were of their child. The Externalising score produced acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.76).

Prenatal Testosterone Exposure

The 2D:4D digit ratio is an accepted measure for PT exposure [6668] and is calculated by dividing the length of the second finger digit (2D) by the length of the fourth finger digit (4D). Sexual dimorphism in 2D:4D is present from at least the 14th week of pregnancy and remains stable into adulthood [6973]. Postnatal hormonal surges also drive finger length growth; however research shows that high levels of circulating testosterone during adolescence actually reduce the impact of stress [74]. Therefore, the 2D:4D ratio should be robust to postnatal stresses.
We used a Canon Canoscan LiDE120 scanner to obtain hand scans from which fingers were measured using the measurement tool in Adobe Photoshop CS5. Hand scans and computer-assisted measurement are argued as a preferable method to using callipers or rulers [75, 76]. The length of the finger measurement is taken from the tip of the finger to the proximal crease of the palm. Both right-hand (RH) and left-hand (LH) ratios were calculated. Inter-observer repeatabilities of the finger measurements were assessed using Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) [77] and revealed good reliability between two observers. ICCs were 0.848 for R2D, 0.868 for R4D, 0.347 for RH2D4D, 0.892 for L2D, 0.913 for L4D and 0.468 for LH2D:4D (all ps < 0.001).

Procedure

Head Teachers were approached via email or telephone and were provided with an Access Letter that described the nature and purpose of the study, and the data collection process. On obtaining authorization for the study to be carried out, individual study packs for each child containing an Information Sheet, Consent Forms (Parent Consent for child participation, Child Consent and Parent Consent), ICU and SDQ were sent to the school. The Information Sheet stated the nature and purpose of the study; that it involved the parent completing two questionnaires about their child’s behaviour, and for their child’s hands to be scanned at school. Teacherd distributed the packs to children who were to take them home to their parents. A period of at least two weeks was given for parents to return the packs (in a sealed envelope provided) with completed consent forms and questionnaires. The children whose parents had consented for them to take part were asked for their consent. If they agreed, they had their hand scanned at a later date whilst they were at school.

Results

Descriptive statistics are shown in Table 1. Digit ratios for both hands were smaller in boys than in girls but were not significantly so. Boys also scored higher in all reported measures, but not significantly. In order to look at the relationship between 2D:4D ratio, CU traits and EB, we conducted a series of zero-order correlations (Table 2). Due to multiple comparisons and the increased likelihood of making a Type 1 error, a Bonferonni correction set the minimum alpha level to 0.001 [78]. No significant relationships at 0.001 were found between any of the variables. Next we conducted two stepwise regression analyses (Table 3) where in the first step either RH or LH2D:4D and CU traits were regressed onto the SDQ Externalising score, and then on the second step we added an interaction term of either RH × CU traits or LH2D:4D × CU traits. In the first step of the RH2D:4D model, SDQ Externalising was significantly and uniquely predicted by CU traits, but not RH2D:4D. With the addition of the interaction term, CU remained a significant predictor, and the standardised beta for RH2D:4D became significant. The interaction term was also significant. The interaction between RH2D:4D and CU traits explained 4 % of the variance in SDQ Externalising. In the first step of the LH2D:4D model, SDQ Externalising was uniquely predicted by CU traits. Neither LH2D:4D nor the interaction term was significant in predicting SDQ Externalising scores. Post hoc testing was applied using PROCESS [79] to examine the association between RH2D:4D and SDQ Externalising at low (−1SD), mean, and high (+1SD) levels of CU traits. The form of the interaction is shown in Fig. 1.
Table 1
Means and stand deviations for all variables
 
Total
Boys
Girls
t
RH2D:4D
0.956 (0.037)
0.956 (0.036)
0.956 (0.037)
−0.09
LH2D:4D
0.963 (0.038)
0.959 (0.037)
0.965 (0.039)
−0.72
ICU
16.34 (4.64)
17.26 (5.26)
15.83 (4.15)
1.34
SDQ Externalising
4.92 (3.47)
5.48 (3.54)
4.56 (3.41)
1.15
Comparisons are between boys and girls
Table 2
Zero order correlations for RH2D:4D and LH2D:4D
 
RH2D:4D
LH2D:4D
ICU
SDQ externalising
RH2D:4D
1
0.74***
0.23*
0.10
LH2D:4D
 
1
0.14
0.11
ICU
  
1
0.47***
SDQ externalising
   
1
*p < .05
**p < .01
***p < .001
Table 3
Stepwise regression of 2D:4D and CU traits on SDQ Externalising scores
 
SDQ (RH2D:4D)
SDQ (LH2D:4D)
B
SE
β
∆R²
B
SE
β
∆R²
Step 1
 2D:4D
−1.93
9.37
−0.02
 
2.95
9
0.03
 
 CU traits
0.04
0.08
0.54**
15.12**
0.40
0.07
0.53**
15.17**
Step 2
 2D:4D
69.7
34.28
0.74*
 
39.61
32.45
0.43
 
 CU traits
4.65
1.96
6.21
 
2.44
1.74
3.26
 
 2D:4D × CU traits
−4.34
2.03
−5.9
4.70*
−2.12
1.81
−2.82
1.38
SDQ and RH2D:4D model: R 2  = 0.33, F (3, 78) = 12.14, p < .001; Step 1: ∆R² = 0.29, F (2, 76) = 15.12, p < .001; Step 2: ∆R² = 0.04, F (1, 75) = 4.70, p = .03. SDQ and LH2D:4D model: R 2 = 0.30, F (3, 78) = 10.62, p < .001; Step 1: ∆R² = 0.29, F (2, 76) = 15.12, p < .001; Step 2: ∆R² = 0.01, F (1, 75) = 1.38, p = ns
*p < .05
**p < .01

Discussion

We investigated whether PT was related to CU traits and EB in children between 5 and 6 years of age. We also examined whether there was an interaction between exposure to PT and CU traits in being associated with higher levels of EB. Indeed, we found that children who were higher in CU traits and who had been exposed to greater levels of PT were higher in EB reported by parents. Children who were higher in CU traits, but who had been exposed to lower levels of PT (i.e., indicative of greater prenatal estrogen), demonstrated fewer EB. This finding suggests that CU traits can worsen or enhance the masculinising influence of PT in the development of EB. To our knowledge, this is the first study to suggest that prenatal neuroendocrinology may be a factor involved in CU traits and EB exhibited in children aged between 5 and 6 years old.
Our findings are consistent with what was understood about the interplay between genetic and environmental factors in the development of child psychopathy. Although our study is the first to demonstrate that this interplay may start before birth, research reliably indicates that some children are genetically vulnerable to the development of a cold and callous temperament style of interacting with others. Such findings are demonstrated in 7-year olds [20, 80, 81], 9–10 year olds [82], 12-year olds [83], adolescents [84, 85] and adults [86]. However, children with CU traits may be further exposed to stressors that result in pervasive and serious EB because of how they interact with their environment. For example, children with CU traits have been shown to experience greater negative life events over time, which may be a consequence of their own fearless and risky behaviours that lead to encountering dangerous environments or situations where they are likely to suffer stressful events [8791]. Our main finding could suggest that a child with a genetic vulnerability to CU traits is already, pre-birth, susceptible to developing EB caused by environmental factors; in this case, elevated levels of PT activated by maternal stress. Specifically, the high-CU child may be more sensitive to the masculinising effects that higher levels of PT have on neural organisation. We would like to address whether it is adaptive to be predisposed to problem behaviours so early on in life. Essentially, does our finding indicate a potential role for fetal programming [44]?
Maternal stress has been suggested to operate as a signal of impending harsh environmental conditions to the fetus. Specifically, stress increases cortisol, which changes fetal adrenal, ovarian and testicular functioning, and therefore PT production [3841]. We would therefore expect an association between higher levels of PT and aggressive, competitive behaviours once entering the world. Fetal programming predicts that this association should be observed in both adults and children because an early start to problem behaviour would be adaptive in successfully navigating a hostile environment through to adulthood. Yet, a range of diverse and unaccounted for factors may act on PT and thus the association may not be straightforward. For example, PT levels are highly heritable [45] and thus genetic effect should be given due consideration. In which case, one might observe externalizing behaviour in those high on CU traits and PT regardless of the harshness of the environment. Thus, when either researching or in treatment planning, the child’s entire life history, including whether the mother experienced stress during pregnancy, is needed to construct the most informative account of their developmental trajectory to problem behaviour. This might also be helpful in prevention, by monitoring the expectant mothers mental health and intervening as appropriate (e.g., additional support) during pregnancy.
Our results suggest that from pre-birth, children with CU traits who were also exposed to more PT, are potentially more liable in developing behaviours that are adaptive in harsh environments, thereby providing some support for “fetal programming”. Interestingly, our findings corroborate what prior studies revealed. Namely, that high PT is related to hyperactivity, ADHD symptoms, conduct problems and poor social cognitive functioning in children from 3 to 7 years of age [36, 37]. We extend these findings to include children who exhibit traits and behaviours associated with child psychopathy.
The case for fetal programming is therefore gaining support, although further investigation is needed to identify the precise biological mechanism between maternal stress and PT, which currently remains a topic for investigation. The fetal programming hypothesis is challenged by high heritability values for 2D:4D [45, 92], and therefore multiple factors (biological and environmental) need consideration. Perhaps there is an association between the genes that code for CU traits and those that code for PT levels. However, in our study, we did not find evidence for zero-order correlations between CU traits and PT. It should be borne in mind that phenotypic output is the product of a highly complex process involving genes, the environment and gene x environment interactions. Therefore we can only speculate as to the implications of our results at this time.
A final point of interest is that children exposed to higher levels of PE had fewer EB only if they were higher on CU traits. Research reliably shows that high PE is associated with empathy and prosocial (need ref) behaviour [5, 34, 35], as well as anxiety and neuroticism [37, 93, 94]. Seeing that psychopathy overall is hypothesised to be a male adaptation [95], it is possible to speculate that feminising effects of PE counterbalance CU traits by some yet unknown mechanism. Children high on CU traits and exposed to higher levels of PE may not end up eliciting adverse reactions from parents or peers, perhaps because they are more prosocial or empathetic, at least cognitively rather than affectively (i.e., they can “talk the talk”). Consequently, they reduce the likelihood of developing EB usually associated with harsh environments. Of particular relevance is evidence from prior 2D:4D ratio research where PT moderated the association between exposure to aggression cues and prosocial behaviour. Specifically, individuals exposed to higher levels of PE became more prosocial in the presence of an aggression cue [32]. The authors suggested that contextual cues should be considered as moderating effects when interpreting associations between PT and personality traits, and might explain why findings from 2D:4D research can produce inconsistent results [32]. Our findings similarly highlight the need to consider other factors that might potentially moderate the relationship between PT and personality.
There are limitations to our study. We used the parent report versions of both the SDQ and ICU, which increases the potential for shared-method variance. It would have been beneficial to include the teacher report versions by way of verification. However, due to the need to limit the time required by the school to administer the data collection, we felt that the parent report versions were adequate. Assigning CU traits as the main focus for psychopathy research in children has also been challenged. Some argue that this ignores other important behavioural and interpersonal aspects of psychopathic personality that the ICU does not measure [96]. However, research consistently demonstrates CU traits as a key factor in identifying children who go on to develop psychopathy [911]. In the absence of equally compelling research that pinpoints another key factor, we are confident in using the ICU as an indicator of child psychopathic behaviour. Debate also surrounds the accuracy of using the 2D:4D ratio as a biomarker for PT and results can be inconsistent across sex, and even studies investigating similar behaviours [97]. However, there is good evidence to suggest that the 2D:4D reflects PT exposure [6668] and has become a popular measure used in studies over the last 15 years. We therefore feel that our results make a unique contribution to the literature and provide the basis for investigating the subject area further. A final issue concerns sex differences. Both psychopathy and 2D:4D ratio are sexually dimorphic, however in our study, due to our sample size, to compare boys and girls would have failed to generate enough power to make any findings truly inferential. Future studies should aim to investigate gender as a potential moderator.
Our study is unique in including children. Studies that have linked aggression [2527, 98]; although c.f. [99], sensation seeking [29], low empathy [4, 5, 34, 35], dominance [100] and antisocial behaviour [24] to high PT have only used adult samples, and there are almost no studies in children. Our findings will also contribute to the further advancement of developmental psychopathological theories of CP behaviour. Frick and colleagues [9] state that the most sufficient causal model can only be achieved by considering multiple risk factors, both biological and environmental. We have not only presented a hitherto unconsidered risk factor, but have also highlighted the timing for when this risk factor (e.g., before birth) takes effect. The most comprehensive theory helps to improve prevention or intervention treatment for at-risk or affected children. Indeed, early intervention is more effective [101104] and may prevent the negative consequences of harsh parenting [57, 105]. Pregnant women who are encountering stress should be identified early in the pregnancy so that they can be provided with mental health support. Unresolved maternal stress may also make these women more vulnerable to developing a harsh parenting style; thus early intervention is critical to prevention of CP [32, 96].
Our findings contribute to an ever increasing and important body of research in child psychopathy. Researchers acknowledge that developmental pathways to adult psychopathy are not easily discovered and concern varied environmental and biological factors [9]. This is the first study to bring forward another biological factor in the form of PT, and therefore highlights the need to acknowledge that children are on the path to problem behaviour even before they are born.

Summary

CU traits are readily acknowledged as the key to the development of serious EB behaviour in children. Multiple risk factors for CU combined with EB have already been identified in previous studies. However, this is the first to examine if prenatal experiences also contribute to this type of behaviour. Studies show that adverse traits and behaviours are expressed in adults who were subject to higher levels of PT, thereby highlighting PT as a potential risk factor for CU traits in children. Hence, in the current study, we examined CU traits as a moderating factor in the association between exposure to PT and EB. The 2D:4D digit ratio was used to measure exposure to PT in children 5–6 years old, who were also evaluated for CU traits and EB by their parents. A moderating effect was found for CU traits such that children exposed to higher levels of PT expressed more EB if they were high in CU traits. Conversely, children exposed to lower levels of PT but were high in CU traits expressed less EB. These findings suggest that CU traits can enhance or weaken the influence of prenatal masculinisation on CP EB. This study has therefore provided a fresh perspective on CU traits and EB in children by highlighting neuroendocrinoloy and prenatal experiences as potential factors in their development.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Shezhana Brepotra and Ellie Henry for faciliating access to schools, and the Head Teachers for agreeing to take participate in the study. Special mention should go to Jennifer Green whose help was particularly instrumental in making the study happen.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Literatuur
1.
go back to reference Levy T, Bloch Y, Bar-Maisels M, Gat-Yablonski G, Djalovski A, Borodkin K, Apter A (2015) Salivary oxytocin in adolescents with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 24:1543–1551CrossRefPubMed Levy T, Bloch Y, Bar-Maisels M, Gat-Yablonski G, Djalovski A, Borodkin K, Apter A (2015) Salivary oxytocin in adolescents with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 24:1543–1551CrossRefPubMed
2.
go back to reference Lockwood PL, Sebastian CL, McCrory EJ, Hyde ZH, Gu X, De Brito SA, Viding E (2013) Association of callous traits with reduced neural response to others’ pain in children with conduct problems. Curr Biol 23:901–905CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lockwood PL, Sebastian CL, McCrory EJ, Hyde ZH, Gu X, De Brito SA, Viding E (2013) Association of callous traits with reduced neural response to others’ pain in children with conduct problems. Curr Biol 23:901–905CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
3.
go back to reference Moul C, Dobson-Stone C, Brennan J, Hawes D, Dadds M (2013) An exploration of the serotonin system in antisocial boys with high levels of callous-unemotional traits. PLoS One 8(2) Moul C, Dobson-Stone C, Brennan J, Hawes D, Dadds M (2013) An exploration of the serotonin system in antisocial boys with high levels of callous-unemotional traits. PLoS One 8(2)
4.
go back to reference Chapman E, Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G (2006) Fetal testosterone and empathy: evidence from the empathy quotient (EQ) and the “reading the mind in the eyes” test. Soc Neurosci 1:135–148CrossRefPubMed Chapman E, Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G (2006) Fetal testosterone and empathy: evidence from the empathy quotient (EQ) and the “reading the mind in the eyes” test. Soc Neurosci 1:135–148CrossRefPubMed
5.
go back to reference Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Taylor K, Hackett G (2006) Fetal testosterone and empathy. Horm Behav 49:282–292CrossRefPubMed Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Taylor K, Hackett G (2006) Fetal testosterone and empathy. Horm Behav 49:282–292CrossRefPubMed
6.
go back to reference Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P (2002) Foetal testosterone and eye contact in 12-month-old human infants. Infant Behav Dev 25:327–335CrossRef Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P (2002) Foetal testosterone and eye contact in 12-month-old human infants. Infant Behav Dev 25:327–335CrossRef
7.
go back to reference Blanchard A, Lyons M, Centifanti L (2016) Baby was a black sheep: Digit ratio (2D:4D), maternal bonding and primary and secondary psychopathy. Pers Individ Differ 99:67–71CrossRef Blanchard A, Lyons M, Centifanti L (2016) Baby was a black sheep: Digit ratio (2D:4D), maternal bonding and primary and secondary psychopathy. Pers Individ Differ 99:67–71CrossRef
8.
go back to reference Gao Y, Tuvblad C, Schell A, Baker L, Raine A (2015) Skin conductance fear conditioning impairments and aggression: A longitudinal study. Psychophysiology 52:288–295CrossRefPubMed Gao Y, Tuvblad C, Schell A, Baker L, Raine A (2015) Skin conductance fear conditioning impairments and aggression: A longitudinal study. Psychophysiology 52:288–295CrossRefPubMed
9.
go back to reference Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE (2014) Annual research review: a developmental psychopathology approach to understanding callous-unemotional traits in children and adolescents with serious conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 55:532–548CrossRefPubMed Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE (2014) Annual research review: a developmental psychopathology approach to understanding callous-unemotional traits in children and adolescents with serious conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 55:532–548CrossRefPubMed
10.
go back to reference Panayiotou G, Fanti K A, Lazarou C (2015) Fearful victims and fearless bullies? Subjective reactions to emotional imagery scenes of children involved in school aggression. Pers Individ Differ 78:29–33CrossRef Panayiotou G, Fanti K A, Lazarou C (2015) Fearful victims and fearless bullies? Subjective reactions to emotional imagery scenes of children involved in school aggression. Pers Individ Differ 78:29–33CrossRef
11.
go back to reference Viding E, Sebastian CL, Dadds MR, Lockwood PL, Cecil CAM. De Brito SA, McCrory EJ (2012) Amygdala response to preattentive masked fear in children with conduct problems: the role of callous-unemotional traits. Am J Psychiatry 169:1109–16.CrossRefPubMed Viding E, Sebastian CL, Dadds MR, Lockwood PL, Cecil CAM. De Brito SA, McCrory EJ (2012) Amygdala response to preattentive masked fear in children with conduct problems: the role of callous-unemotional traits. Am J Psychiatry 169:1109–16.CrossRefPubMed
12.
go back to reference De Wied M, Van Boxtel A, Matthys W, Meeus W (2012) Verbal, facial and autonomic responses to empathy-eliciting film clips by disruptive male adolescents with high versus low callous-unemotional traits. J Abnorm Child Psychol 40:211–223CrossRefPubMed De Wied M, Van Boxtel A, Matthys W, Meeus W (2012) Verbal, facial and autonomic responses to empathy-eliciting film clips by disruptive male adolescents with high versus low callous-unemotional traits. J Abnorm Child Psychol 40:211–223CrossRefPubMed
13.
go back to reference Muñoz LC, Frick PJ, Kimonis ER, Aucoin KJ (2008) Verbal ability and delinquency: testing the moderating role of psychopathic traits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 49:414–421CrossRefPubMed Muñoz LC, Frick PJ, Kimonis ER, Aucoin KJ (2008) Verbal ability and delinquency: testing the moderating role of psychopathic traits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 49:414–421CrossRefPubMed
14.
go back to reference Fanti KA, Kyranides MN, Panayiotou G (2015) Facial reactions to violent and comedy films: association with callous-unemotional traits and impulsive aggression. Cognit Emot 9931:1–16 Fanti KA, Kyranides MN, Panayiotou G (2015) Facial reactions to violent and comedy films: association with callous-unemotional traits and impulsive aggression. Cognit Emot 9931:1–16
15.
go back to reference Breeden AL, Cardinale EM, Lozier LM, VanMeter JW, Marsh AA (2015) Callous-unemotional traits drive reduced white-matter integrity in youths with conduct problems. Psychol Med 1–14 Breeden AL, Cardinale EM, Lozier LM, VanMeter JW, Marsh AA (2015) Callous-unemotional traits drive reduced white-matter integrity in youths with conduct problems. Psychol Med 1–14
16.
go back to reference Hoppenbrouwers SS, Nazeri A, de Jesus DR, Stirpe T, Felsky D, Schutter DJLG, Daskalkis Z, Voimeskos AN (2013). White matter deficits in psychopathic offenders and correlation with factor structure. PLoS One 8(8). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0072375 Hoppenbrouwers SS, Nazeri A, de Jesus DR, Stirpe T, Felsky D, Schutter DJLG, Daskalkis Z, Voimeskos AN (2013). White matter deficits in psychopathic offenders and correlation with factor structure. PLoS One 8(8). doi:10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0072375
17.
go back to reference Wolf RC, Pujara MS, Motzkin JC, Newman JP, Kiehl KA, Decety J, Kosson DS, Koenigs M (2015) Interpersonal traits of psychopathy linked to reduced integrity of the uncinate fasciculus. Hum Brain Mapp 36:4202–4209CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wolf RC, Pujara MS, Motzkin JC, Newman JP, Kiehl KA, Decety J, Kosson DS, Koenigs M (2015) Interpersonal traits of psychopathy linked to reduced integrity of the uncinate fasciculus. Hum Brain Mapp 36:4202–4209CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
18.
go back to reference Viding E, Spinath FM, Price TS, Bishop DVM, Dale PS, Plomin R (2004) Genetic and environmental influence on language impairment in 4-year-old same-sex and opposite-sex twins. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45:315–325CrossRefPubMed Viding E, Spinath FM, Price TS, Bishop DVM, Dale PS, Plomin R (2004) Genetic and environmental influence on language impairment in 4-year-old same-sex and opposite-sex twins. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45:315–325CrossRefPubMed
19.
go back to reference Kochanska G (1993) Toward a synthesis of parental Socialization and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Dev 64:325–347CrossRef Kochanska G (1993) Toward a synthesis of parental Socialization and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Dev 64:325–347CrossRef
20.
go back to reference Viding E, Larsson H (2007) Aetiology of antisocial behaviour. Int Congr Ser 1304:121–132CrossRef Viding E, Larsson H (2007) Aetiology of antisocial behaviour. Int Congr Ser 1304:121–132CrossRef
21.
go back to reference Viding E, Larsson H, Jones AP (2008) Quantitative genetic studies of antisocial behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 363:2519–2527CrossRef Viding E, Larsson H, Jones AP (2008) Quantitative genetic studies of antisocial behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 363:2519–2527CrossRef
22.
go back to reference Marsh AA, Finger EC, Mitchell DG V, Reid ME, Sims C, Kosson DS, Towbin KE, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, Blair RJR (2008) Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. Am J Psychiatry 165:712–720CrossRefPubMed Marsh AA, Finger EC, Mitchell DG V, Reid ME, Sims C, Kosson DS, Towbin KE, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, Blair RJR (2008) Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. Am J Psychiatry 165:712–720CrossRefPubMed
23.
go back to reference Lombardo MV, Ashwin E, Auyeung B, Chakrabarti B, Lai MC, Taylor K, Hackett G, Bullmore ET, Baron-Cohen S (2012) Fetal programming effects of testosterone on the reward system and behavioral approach tendencies in humans. Biol Psychiatry 72:839–847CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lombardo MV, Ashwin E, Auyeung B, Chakrabarti B, Lai MC, Taylor K, Hackett G, Bullmore ET, Baron-Cohen S (2012) Fetal programming effects of testosterone on the reward system and behavioral approach tendencies in humans. Biol Psychiatry 72:839–847CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
24.
go back to reference Yildirim BO, Derksen JJL (2012) A review on the relationship between testosterone and life-course persistent antisocial behavior. Psychiatry Res 200:984–1010CrossRefPubMed Yildirim BO, Derksen JJL (2012) A review on the relationship between testosterone and life-course persistent antisocial behavior. Psychiatry Res 200:984–1010CrossRefPubMed
25.
go back to reference Bailey AA, Hurd PL (2005) Finger length ratio (2D:4D) correlates with physical aggression in men but not in women. Biol Psychol 68:215–222CrossRefPubMed Bailey AA, Hurd PL (2005) Finger length ratio (2D:4D) correlates with physical aggression in men but not in women. Biol Psychol 68:215–222CrossRefPubMed
26.
go back to reference Coyne SM, Manning JT, Ringer L, Bailey L (2007) Directional asymmetry (right–left differences) in digit ratio (2D:4D) predict indirect aggression in women. Pers Individ Differ 43:865–872CrossRef Coyne SM, Manning JT, Ringer L, Bailey L (2007) Directional asymmetry (right–left differences) in digit ratio (2D:4D) predict indirect aggression in women. Pers Individ Differ 43:865–872CrossRef
27.
go back to reference Benderlioglu Z, Nelson RJ (2004) Digit length ratios predict reactive aggression in women, but not in men. Horm Behav 46:558–564CrossRefPubMed Benderlioglu Z, Nelson RJ (2004) Digit length ratios predict reactive aggression in women, but not in men. Horm Behav 46:558–564CrossRefPubMed
28.
go back to reference Hampson E, Ellis CL, Tenk CM (2008) On the relation between 2D:4D and sex-dimorphic personality traits. Arch Sex Behav 37:133–144CrossRefPubMed Hampson E, Ellis CL, Tenk CM (2008) On the relation between 2D:4D and sex-dimorphic personality traits. Arch Sex Behav 37:133–144CrossRefPubMed
29.
go back to reference Fink B, Neave N, Laughton K, Manning JT (2006) Second to fourth digit ratio and sensation seeking. Pers Individ Differ 41:1253–1262CrossRef Fink B, Neave N, Laughton K, Manning JT (2006) Second to fourth digit ratio and sensation seeking. Pers Individ Differ 41:1253–1262CrossRef
30.
go back to reference Stenstrom E, Saad G, Nepomuceno M V., Mendenhall Z (2011) Testosterone and domain-specific risk: Digit ratios (2D:4D and rel2) as predictors of recreational, financial, and social risk-taking behaviors. Pers Individ Differ 51:412–416CrossRef Stenstrom E, Saad G, Nepomuceno M V., Mendenhall Z (2011) Testosterone and domain-specific risk: Digit ratios (2D:4D and rel2) as predictors of recreational, financial, and social risk-taking behaviors. Pers Individ Differ 51:412–416CrossRef
31.
go back to reference Hönekopp J (2011) Relationships between digit ratio 2D:4D and self-reported aggression and risk taking in an online study. Pers Individ Differ 51:77–80CrossRef Hönekopp J (2011) Relationships between digit ratio 2D:4D and self-reported aggression and risk taking in an online study. Pers Individ Differ 51:77–80CrossRef
32.
go back to reference Millet K, Dewitte S (2009) The presence of aggression cues inverts the relation between digit ratio (2D:4D) and prosocial behaviour in a dictator game. Br J Psychol 100:151–162CrossRefPubMed Millet K, Dewitte S (2009) The presence of aggression cues inverts the relation between digit ratio (2D:4D) and prosocial behaviour in a dictator game. Br J Psychol 100:151–162CrossRefPubMed
33.
go back to reference Austin E, Manning J, Mcinroy K, Mathews E (2002) A preliminary investigation of the associations between personality, cognitive ability and digit ratio. Pers Individ Differ 33(7):1115–1124CrossRef Austin E, Manning J, Mcinroy K, Mathews E (2002) A preliminary investigation of the associations between personality, cognitive ability and digit ratio. Pers Individ Differ 33(7):1115–1124CrossRef
34.
go back to reference Kempe V, Heffernan E (2011) Digit ratio is linked to affective empathy in women. Pers Individ Differ 50:430–433CrossRef Kempe V, Heffernan E (2011) Digit ratio is linked to affective empathy in women. Pers Individ Differ 50:430–433CrossRef
35.
go back to reference Wakabayashi A, Nakazawa Y (2010) On relationships between digit ratio (2D:4D) and two fundamental cognitive drives, empathizing and systemizing, in Japanese sample. Pers Individ Differ 49:928–931CrossRef Wakabayashi A, Nakazawa Y (2010) On relationships between digit ratio (2D:4D) and two fundamental cognitive drives, empathizing and systemizing, in Japanese sample. Pers Individ Differ 49:928–931CrossRef
36.
go back to reference Fink B, Manning JT, Williams JHG, Podmore-Nappin C (2007) The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and developmental psychopathology in school-aged children. Pers Individ Differ 42:369–379CrossRef Fink B, Manning JT, Williams JHG, Podmore-Nappin C (2007) The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and developmental psychopathology in school-aged children. Pers Individ Differ 42:369–379CrossRef
37.
go back to reference de Bruin EI, Verheij F, Wiegman T, Ferdinand RF (2006) Differences in finger length ratio between males with autism, pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified, ADHD, and anxiety disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol 48:962–965CrossRefPubMed de Bruin EI, Verheij F, Wiegman T, Ferdinand RF (2006) Differences in finger length ratio between males with autism, pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified, ADHD, and anxiety disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol 48:962–965CrossRefPubMed
38.
go back to reference Sarkar P, Bergman K, O’Connor TG, Glover V (2008) Maternal antenatal anxiety and amniotic fluid cortisol and testosterone: possible implications for foetal programming. J Neuroendocrinol 20:489–496CrossRefPubMed Sarkar P, Bergman K, O’Connor TG, Glover V (2008) Maternal antenatal anxiety and amniotic fluid cortisol and testosterone: possible implications for foetal programming. J Neuroendocrinol 20:489–496CrossRefPubMed
39.
go back to reference Barrett ES, Swan SH (2015) Stress and androgen activity during fetal development. Endocrinology 156:2015–1335CrossRef Barrett ES, Swan SH (2015) Stress and androgen activity during fetal development. Endocrinology 156:2015–1335CrossRef
40.
go back to reference Gitau R, Adams D, Fisk NM, Glover V (2005) Fetal plasma testosterone correlates positively with cortisol. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 90:F166–F169CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gitau R, Adams D, Fisk NM, Glover V (2005) Fetal plasma testosterone correlates positively with cortisol. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 90:F166–F169CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
41.
go back to reference Barrett ES, Parlett LE, Sathyanarayana S, Liu F, Redmon JB, Wang C, Swan SH (2013) Prenatal exposure to stressful life events is associated with masculinized anogenital distance (AGD) in female infants. Physiol Behav 114–115:14–20CrossRefPubMed Barrett ES, Parlett LE, Sathyanarayana S, Liu F, Redmon JB, Wang C, Swan SH (2013) Prenatal exposure to stressful life events is associated with masculinized anogenital distance (AGD) in female infants. Physiol Behav 114–115:14–20CrossRefPubMed
42.
go back to reference O’Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, Glover V (2003) Maternal antenatal anxiety and behavioural/emotional problems in children: a test of a programming hypothesis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 44:1025–1036CrossRefPubMed O’Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, Glover V (2003) Maternal antenatal anxiety and behavioural/emotional problems in children: a test of a programming hypothesis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 44:1025–1036CrossRefPubMed
43.
go back to reference Van den Bergh BRH, Marcoen A (2004) High antenatal maternal anxiety is related to ADHD symptoms, externalizing problems, and anxiety in 8- and 9-year-olds. Child Dev 75:1085–1097CrossRefPubMed Van den Bergh BRH, Marcoen A (2004) High antenatal maternal anxiety is related to ADHD symptoms, externalizing problems, and anxiety in 8- and 9-year-olds. Child Dev 75:1085–1097CrossRefPubMed
44.
go back to reference Del Giudice M (2012) Fetal programming by maternal stress: Insights from a conflict perspective. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:1614–1629CrossRefPubMed Del Giudice M (2012) Fetal programming by maternal stress: Insights from a conflict perspective. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:1614–1629CrossRefPubMed
45.
go back to reference Gobrogge KL, Breedlove SM, Klump KL (2008) Genetic and environmental influences on 2D:4D finger length ratios: a study of monozygotic and dizygotic male and female twins. Arch Sex Behav 37:112–118CrossRefPubMed Gobrogge KL, Breedlove SM, Klump KL (2008) Genetic and environmental influences on 2D:4D finger length ratios: a study of monozygotic and dizygotic male and female twins. Arch Sex Behav 37:112–118CrossRefPubMed
46.
go back to reference Susman EJ, Inoff-Germain G, Nottelmann ED, Loriaux DL, Gordon B. Cutler J, Chrousos GP (1987) Hormones, emotional dispositions, and aggressive attributes in young adolescents. Child Dev 58:1114–1134CrossRefPubMed Susman EJ, Inoff-Germain G, Nottelmann ED, Loriaux DL, Gordon B. Cutler J, Chrousos GP (1987) Hormones, emotional dispositions, and aggressive attributes in young adolescents. Child Dev 58:1114–1134CrossRefPubMed
47.
go back to reference Coyne S, Thomas T (2008) Psychopathy, aggression, and cheating behavior: a test of the Cheater–Hawk hypothesis. Pers Individ Differ 44:1105–1115CrossRef Coyne S, Thomas T (2008) Psychopathy, aggression, and cheating behavior: a test of the Cheater–Hawk hypothesis. Pers Individ Differ 44:1105–1115CrossRef
48.
go back to reference Vaillancourt T, Sunderani S (2011) Psychopathy and indirect aggression: the roles of cortisol, sex, and type of psychopathy. Brain Cogn 77:170–175CrossRefPubMed Vaillancourt T, Sunderani S (2011) Psychopathy and indirect aggression: the roles of cortisol, sex, and type of psychopathy. Brain Cogn 77:170–175CrossRefPubMed
49.
go back to reference Lee Z, Salekin RT (2010) Psychopathy in a noninstitutional sample: differences in primary and secondary subtypes. Personal Disord 1:153–169 Lee Z, Salekin RT (2010) Psychopathy in a noninstitutional sample: differences in primary and secondary subtypes. Personal Disord 1:153–169
50.
go back to reference Van Honk J, Peper JS, Schutter DJLG (2005) Testosterone reduces unconscious fear but not consciously experienced anxiety: implications for the disorders of fear and anxiety. Biol Psychiatry 58:218–225CrossRefPubMed Van Honk J, Peper JS, Schutter DJLG (2005) Testosterone reduces unconscious fear but not consciously experienced anxiety: implications for the disorders of fear and anxiety. Biol Psychiatry 58:218–225CrossRefPubMed
52.
go back to reference van Honk J, Schutter DJLG (2006) Unmasking feigned sanity: a neurobiological model of emotion processing in primary psychopathy. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 11:285–306CrossRefPubMed van Honk J, Schutter DJLG (2006) Unmasking feigned sanity: a neurobiological model of emotion processing in primary psychopathy. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 11:285–306CrossRefPubMed
53.
go back to reference Portnoy J, Raine A, Glenn AL, Chen FR, Choy O, Granger DA (2015) Digit ratio (2D:4D) moderates the relationship between cortisol reactivity and self-reported externalizing behavior in young adolescent males. Biol Psychol 112:94–106CrossRefPubMed Portnoy J, Raine A, Glenn AL, Chen FR, Choy O, Granger DA (2015) Digit ratio (2D:4D) moderates the relationship between cortisol reactivity and self-reported externalizing behavior in young adolescent males. Biol Psychol 112:94–106CrossRefPubMed
54.
go back to reference Izard CE, Fine S, Schultz D, Mostow AJ, Ackerman B, Youngstrom E (2001) Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. Psychol Sci 12:18–23CrossRefPubMed Izard CE, Fine S, Schultz D, Mostow AJ, Ackerman B, Youngstrom E (2001) Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. Psychol Sci 12:18–23CrossRefPubMed
55.
go back to reference Centifanti LCM, Meins E, Fernyhough C (2016) Callous-unemotional traits and impulsivity: distinct longitudinal relations with mind-mindedness and understanding of others. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 57:84–92CrossRef Centifanti LCM, Meins E, Fernyhough C (2016) Callous-unemotional traits and impulsivity: distinct longitudinal relations with mind-mindedness and understanding of others. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 57:84–92CrossRef
56.
go back to reference Deater-Deckard K (2001) Annotation: recent research examining the role of peer relationships in the development of psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 42:565–579CrossRefPubMed Deater-Deckard K (2001) Annotation: recent research examining the role of peer relationships in the development of psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 42:565–579CrossRefPubMed
57.
go back to reference Gulay H (2011) Assessment of the prosocial behaviors of young children with regard to social development, social skills, parental acceptance-rejection and peer relationships. J Instr Psychol 38:164–172 Gulay H (2011) Assessment of the prosocial behaviors of young children with regard to social development, social skills, parental acceptance-rejection and peer relationships. J Instr Psychol 38:164–172
58.
59.
go back to reference Waller R, Gardner F, Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Wilson MN (2012) Do harsh and positive parenting predict parent reports of deceitful-callous behavior in early childhood? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 53:946–953CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Waller R, Gardner F, Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Wilson MN (2012) Do harsh and positive parenting predict parent reports of deceitful-callous behavior in early childhood? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 53:946–953CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
60.
go back to reference Salekin RT, Frick PJ (2005) Psychopathy in children and adolescents: the need for a developmental perspective. J Abnorm Child Psychol 33:403–409CrossRefPubMed Salekin RT, Frick PJ (2005) Psychopathy in children and adolescents: the need for a developmental perspective. J Abnorm Child Psychol 33:403–409CrossRefPubMed
61.
go back to reference Hawes DJ, Dadds MR (2007) Stability and malleability of callous-unemotional traits during treatment for childhood conduct problems. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 36:347–355CrossRefPubMed Hawes DJ, Dadds MR (2007) Stability and malleability of callous-unemotional traits during treatment for childhood conduct problems. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 36:347–355CrossRefPubMed
62.
go back to reference Hoskin AW, Ellis L (2015) Fetal testosterone and criminality: test of evolutionary neuroandrogenic theory. Criminology 53:54–73.CrossRef Hoskin AW, Ellis L (2015) Fetal testosterone and criminality: test of evolutionary neuroandrogenic theory. Criminology 53:54–73.CrossRef
63.
go back to reference Frick PJ (2004) The inventory of callous-unemotional traits. University of New Orleans, New Orleans (Unpublished rating scale). Frick PJ (2004) The inventory of callous-unemotional traits. University of New Orleans, New Orleans (Unpublished rating scale).
64.
go back to reference Kimonis ER, Frick PJ, Skeem JL, Marsee MA, Cruise K, Munoz LC, Aucoin KJ, Morris AS (2008) Assessing callous–unemotional traits in adolescent offenders: validation of the inventory of callous–unemotional traits. Int J Law Psychiatry 31:241–252CrossRefPubMed Kimonis ER, Frick PJ, Skeem JL, Marsee MA, Cruise K, Munoz LC, Aucoin KJ, Morris AS (2008) Assessing callous–unemotional traits in adolescent offenders: validation of the inventory of callous–unemotional traits. Int J Law Psychiatry 31:241–252CrossRefPubMed
65.
go back to reference Goodman R (1997) The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 38:581–586CrossRefPubMed Goodman R (1997) The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 38:581–586CrossRefPubMed
66.
go back to reference Manning JT, Scutt D, Wilson J, Lewis-Jones DI (1998) The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. Hum Reprod 13:3000–3004CrossRefPubMed Manning JT, Scutt D, Wilson J, Lewis-Jones DI (1998) The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. Hum Reprod 13:3000–3004CrossRefPubMed
67.
go back to reference Putz D A., Gaulin SJC, Sporter RJ, McBurney DH (2004) Sex hormones and finger length. Evol Hum Behav 25:182–199CrossRef Putz D A., Gaulin SJC, Sporter RJ, McBurney DH (2004) Sex hormones and finger length. Evol Hum Behav 25:182–199CrossRef
68.
go back to reference Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Knickmeyer R, Manning JT (2004) 2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol. Early Hum Dev 77:23–28CrossRefPubMed Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Knickmeyer R, Manning JT (2004) 2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol. Early Hum Dev 77:23–28CrossRefPubMed
69.
go back to reference Malas MA, Dogan S, Evcil EH, Desdicioglu K (2006) Fetal development of the hand, digits and digit ratio (2D:4D). Early Hum Dev 82:469–475CrossRefPubMed Malas MA, Dogan S, Evcil EH, Desdicioglu K (2006) Fetal development of the hand, digits and digit ratio (2D:4D). Early Hum Dev 82:469–475CrossRefPubMed
70.
go back to reference Galis F, Ten Broek CMA, Van Dongen S, Wijnaendts LCD (2010) Sexual dimorphism in the prenatal digit ratio (2D:4D). Arch Sex Behav 39:57–62CrossRefPubMed Galis F, Ten Broek CMA, Van Dongen S, Wijnaendts LCD (2010) Sexual dimorphism in the prenatal digit ratio (2D:4D). Arch Sex Behav 39:57–62CrossRefPubMed
71.
go back to reference Trivers R, Manning J, Jacobson A (2006) A longitudinal study of digit ratio (2D:4D) and other finger ratios in Jamaican children. Horm Behav 49:150–156CrossRefPubMed Trivers R, Manning J, Jacobson A (2006) A longitudinal study of digit ratio (2D:4D) and other finger ratios in Jamaican children. Horm Behav 49:150–156CrossRefPubMed
72.
go back to reference Ventura T, Gomes MC, Pita a, Neto MT, Taylor A (2013) Digit ratio (2D:4D) in newborns: influences of prenatal testosterone and maternal environment. Early Hum Dev 89:107–112CrossRefPubMed Ventura T, Gomes MC, Pita a, Neto MT, Taylor A (2013) Digit ratio (2D:4D) in newborns: influences of prenatal testosterone and maternal environment. Early Hum Dev 89:107–112CrossRefPubMed
73.
go back to reference Zheng Z, Cohn M (2011) Developmental basis of sexually dimorphic digit proportions (2D:4D ratio). Dev Biol 356:171CrossRef Zheng Z, Cohn M (2011) Developmental basis of sexually dimorphic digit proportions (2D:4D ratio). Dev Biol 356:171CrossRef
74.
go back to reference Lürzel S, Kaiser S, Krüger C, Sachser N (2011) Inhibiting influence of testosterone on stress responsiveness during adolescence. Horm Behav 60:691–698CrossRefPubMed Lürzel S, Kaiser S, Krüger C, Sachser N (2011) Inhibiting influence of testosterone on stress responsiveness during adolescence. Horm Behav 60:691–698CrossRefPubMed
75.
go back to reference Kemper CJ, Schwerdtfeger A (2009) Comparing indirect methods of digit ratio (2D:4D) measurement. Am J Hum Biol 21:188–191CrossRefPubMed Kemper CJ, Schwerdtfeger A (2009) Comparing indirect methods of digit ratio (2D:4D) measurement. Am J Hum Biol 21:188–191CrossRefPubMed
76.
go back to reference Allaway HC, Bloski TG, Pierson RA, Lujan ME (2009) Digit ratios (2D:4D) determined by computer-assisted analysis are more reliable than those using physical measurements, photocopies, and printed scans. Am J Hum Biol 21:365–370CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Allaway HC, Bloski TG, Pierson RA, Lujan ME (2009) Digit ratios (2D:4D) determined by computer-assisted analysis are more reliable than those using physical measurements, photocopies, and printed scans. Am J Hum Biol 21:365–370CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
77.
go back to reference Voracek M, Manning JT, Dressler SG (2007) Short report repeatability and interobserver error of digit ratio (2D: 4D) measurements made by experts. Am J Hum Biol 146:142–146CrossRef Voracek M, Manning JT, Dressler SG (2007) Short report repeatability and interobserver error of digit ratio (2D: 4D) measurements made by experts. Am J Hum Biol 146:142–146CrossRef
78.
79.
go back to reference Hayes AF (2012) PROCESS: a versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling [White paper]. Hayes AF (2012) PROCESS: a versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling [White paper].
80.
go back to reference Humayun S, Kahn RE, Frick PJ, Viding E (2014) Callous-unemotional traits and anxiety in a community sample of 7-year-olds. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 43:36–42CrossRefPubMed Humayun S, Kahn RE, Frick PJ, Viding E (2014) Callous-unemotional traits and anxiety in a community sample of 7-year-olds. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 43:36–42CrossRefPubMed
81.
go back to reference Bezdjian S, Tuvblad C, Raine A, Baker LA (2011) The genetic and environmental covariation among psychopathic personality traits, and reactive and proactive aggression in childhood. Child Dev 82:1267–1281CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bezdjian S, Tuvblad C, Raine A, Baker LA (2011) The genetic and environmental covariation among psychopathic personality traits, and reactive and proactive aggression in childhood. Child Dev 82:1267–1281CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
82.
go back to reference Fontaine NMG, McCrory EJP, Boivin M, Moffitt TE, Viding E (2011) Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous–unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. J Abnorm Psychol 120:730–742CrossRefPubMed Fontaine NMG, McCrory EJP, Boivin M, Moffitt TE, Viding E (2011) Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous–unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. J Abnorm Psychol 120:730–742CrossRefPubMed
83.
go back to reference Hicks BM, Carlson MD, Blonigen DM, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG, Mgue M (2012) Psychopathic personality traits and environmental contexts: differential correlates, gender differences, and genetic mediation. Personal Disord 3:209–227.CrossRefPubMed Hicks BM, Carlson MD, Blonigen DM, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG, Mgue M (2012) Psychopathic personality traits and environmental contexts: differential correlates, gender differences, and genetic mediation. Personal Disord 3:209–227.CrossRefPubMed
84.
go back to reference Taylor J, Loney BR, Bobadilla L, Iacono WG, McGue M (2003) Genetic and environmental influences on psychopathy trait dimensions in a community sample of male twins. J Abnorm Child Psychol 31:633–645CrossRefPubMed Taylor J, Loney BR, Bobadilla L, Iacono WG, McGue M (2003) Genetic and environmental influences on psychopathy trait dimensions in a community sample of male twins. J Abnorm Child Psychol 31:633–645CrossRefPubMed
85.
go back to reference Blonigen D, Carlson S, Krueger R, Patrick C (2003) A twin study of self-reported psychopathic personality traits. Pers Individ Differ 35:179–197CrossRef Blonigen D, Carlson S, Krueger R, Patrick C (2003) A twin study of self-reported psychopathic personality traits. Pers Individ Differ 35:179–197CrossRef
86.
go back to reference Blonigen DM, Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG (2006) Continuity and change in psychopathic traits as measured via normal-range personality: a longitudinal-biometric study. J Abnorm Psychol 115:85–95CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Blonigen DM, Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG (2006) Continuity and change in psychopathic traits as measured via normal-range personality: a longitudinal-biometric study. J Abnorm Psychol 115:85–95CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
87.
go back to reference Kimonis ER, Centifanti LCM, Allen JL, Frick PJ (2014) Reciprocal influences between negative life events and callous-unemotional traits. J Abnorm Child Psychol 42:1287–1298CrossRefPubMed Kimonis ER, Centifanti LCM, Allen JL, Frick PJ (2014) Reciprocal influences between negative life events and callous-unemotional traits. J Abnorm Child Psychol 42:1287–1298CrossRefPubMed
88.
go back to reference Blair RJR (2006) The emergence of psychopathy: implications for the neuropsychological approach to developmental disorders. Cognition 101:414–442CrossRefPubMed Blair RJR (2006) The emergence of psychopathy: implications for the neuropsychological approach to developmental disorders. Cognition 101:414–442CrossRefPubMed
89.
go back to reference Frick PJ, Ellis M (1999) Callous-unemotional traits and subtypes of conduct disorder. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2:149–168CrossRefPubMed Frick PJ, Ellis M (1999) Callous-unemotional traits and subtypes of conduct disorder. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2:149–168CrossRefPubMed
90.
go back to reference Muñoz LC (2009) callous-unemotional traits are related to combined deficits in recognizing afraid faces and body poses. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 48:554–562CrossRefPubMed Muñoz LC (2009) callous-unemotional traits are related to combined deficits in recognizing afraid faces and body poses. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 48:554–562CrossRefPubMed
91.
go back to reference O’ Brien BS, Frick PJ (1996) Reward dominance: associations with anxiety, conduct problems, and psychopathy in children. J Abnorm Psychol 24:223–224CrossRef O’ Brien BS, Frick PJ (1996) Reward dominance: associations with anxiety, conduct problems, and psychopathy in children. J Abnorm Psychol 24:223–224CrossRef
92.
go back to reference Voracek M, Dressler SG (2009) Brief communication: familial resemblance in digit ratio (2D:4D). Am J Phys Anthropol 140:376–380CrossRefPubMed Voracek M, Dressler SG (2009) Brief communication: familial resemblance in digit ratio (2D:4D). Am J Phys Anthropol 140:376–380CrossRefPubMed
93.
go back to reference Manning JT, Fink B (2011) Digit ratio (2D:4D) and aggregate personality scores across nations: data from the BBC internet study. Pers Individ Differ 51:387–391CrossRef Manning JT, Fink B (2011) Digit ratio (2D:4D) and aggregate personality scores across nations: data from the BBC internet study. Pers Individ Differ 51:387–391CrossRef
94.
go back to reference Austin E, Manning J, Mcinroy K, Mathews E (2002) A preliminary investigation of the associations between personality, cognitive ability and digit ratio. Pers Individ Differ 33:1115–1124CrossRef Austin E, Manning J, Mcinroy K, Mathews E (2002) A preliminary investigation of the associations between personality, cognitive ability and digit ratio. Pers Individ Differ 33:1115–1124CrossRef
95.
go back to reference Jonason PK, Li NP, Webster GD, Schmitt DP (2009) The dark triad: facilitating a short-term mating strategy in men. Eur J Pers 18:5–18CrossRef Jonason PK, Li NP, Webster GD, Schmitt DP (2009) The dark triad: facilitating a short-term mating strategy in men. Eur J Pers 18:5–18CrossRef
96.
go back to reference Colins OF, Andershed H, Frogner L, Lopez-Romero L, Veen V, Andershed A-K (2014) A new measure to assess psychopathic personality in children: the child problematic traits inventory. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 36:4–21CrossRefPubMed Colins OF, Andershed H, Frogner L, Lopez-Romero L, Veen V, Andershed A-K (2014) A new measure to assess psychopathic personality in children: the child problematic traits inventory. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 36:4–21CrossRefPubMed
97.
98.
go back to reference Cleveland ES (2014) Digit ratio, emotional intelligence and parenting styles predict female aggression. Pers Individ Differ 58:9–14CrossRef Cleveland ES (2014) Digit ratio, emotional intelligence and parenting styles predict female aggression. Pers Individ Differ 58:9–14CrossRef
99.
go back to reference Hönekopp J, Watson S (2011) Meta-analysis of the relationship between digit-ratio 2D:4D and aggression. Pers Individ Differ 51(4):381–386CrossRef Hönekopp J, Watson S (2011) Meta-analysis of the relationship between digit-ratio 2D:4D and aggression. Pers Individ Differ 51(4):381–386CrossRef
100.
go back to reference Manning JT, Fink B (2008) Digit ratio (2D:4D), dominance, reproductive success, asymmetry, and sociosexuality in the BBC internet study. Am J Hum Biol 20:451–461CrossRefPubMed Manning JT, Fink B (2008) Digit ratio (2D:4D), dominance, reproductive success, asymmetry, and sociosexuality in the BBC internet study. Am J Hum Biol 20:451–461CrossRefPubMed
101.
go back to reference Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Gardner F, Cheong J, Dishion TJ, Wilson M (2013) Dimensions of callousness in early childhood: links to problem behavior and family intervention effectiveness. Dev Psychopathol 25:347–363CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Gardner F, Cheong J, Dishion TJ, Wilson M (2013) Dimensions of callousness in early childhood: links to problem behavior and family intervention effectiveness. Dev Psychopathol 25:347–363CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
102.
go back to reference Mcdonald R, Dodson MC, Rosenfield D, Jouriles EN (2011) Effects of a parenting intervention on features of psychopathy in children. Changes 39:1013–1023 Mcdonald R, Dodson MC, Rosenfield D, Jouriles EN (2011) Effects of a parenting intervention on features of psychopathy in children. Changes 39:1013–1023
103.
go back to reference Feinberg ME, Jones DE, Roettger ME, Solmeyer A, Hostetler ML (2016) Long-term follow-up of a randomized trial of family foundations: effects on children’s emotional, behavioral, and school a djustment. J Fam Psychol 28:821–831CrossRef Feinberg ME, Jones DE, Roettger ME, Solmeyer A, Hostetler ML (2016) Long-term follow-up of a randomized trial of family foundations: effects on children’s emotional, behavioral, and school a djustment. J Fam Psychol 28:821–831CrossRef
104.
go back to reference Feinberg ME, Jones DE, Roettger ME, Hostetler ML, Sakuma K-L, Paul IM, Ehrenthal DB (2015) Preventive effects on birth outcomes: buffering impact of maternal stress, depression, and anxiety. Matern Child Health J 20:56–65CrossRef Feinberg ME, Jones DE, Roettger ME, Hostetler ML, Sakuma K-L, Paul IM, Ehrenthal DB (2015) Preventive effects on birth outcomes: buffering impact of maternal stress, depression, and anxiety. Matern Child Health J 20:56–65CrossRef
105.
go back to reference Viding E, Blair RJR, Moffitt TE, Plomin R (2005) Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46:592–597CrossRefPubMed Viding E, Blair RJR, Moffitt TE, Plomin R (2005) Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46:592–597CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Callous-Unemotional Traits Moderate the Relation Between Prenatal Testosterone (2D:4D) and Externalising Behaviours in Children
Auteurs
Alyson Blanchard
Luna C. Munoz Centifanti
Publicatiedatum
12-10-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Child Psychiatry & Human Development / Uitgave 4/2017
Print ISSN: 0009-398X
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3327
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-016-0690-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2017

Child Psychiatry & Human Development 4/2017 Naar de uitgave