Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

Open Access 04-10-2016 | Brief Report

Brief Report: Which Came First? Exploring Crossmodal Temporal Order Judgements and Their Relationship with Sensory Reactivity in Autism and Neurotypicals

Auteurs: Daniel Poole, Emma Gowen, Paul A. Warren, Ellen Poliakoff

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 1/2017

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Previous studies have indicated that visual-auditory temporal acuity is reduced in children with autism spectrum conditions (ASC) in comparison to neurotypicals. In the present study we investigated temporal acuity for all possible bimodal pairings of visual, tactile and auditory information in adults with ASC (n = 18) and a matched control group (n = 18). No group differences in temporal acuity for crossmodal stimuli were observed, suggesting that this may be typical in adults with ASC. However, visual-tactile temporal acuity and bias towards vision when presented with visual-auditory information were both predictors of self-reported sensory reactivity. This suggests that reduced multisensory temporal acuity and/or attention towards vision may contribute to atypical sensory reactivity.

Introduction

Atypical reactivity across multiple sensory modalities (e.g. vision, touch, hearing) is widely reported in autism spectrum condition (ASC; O’Neill and Jones 1997). This includes hyper and hypo sensitivity to sensory information, and sensory seeking behaviours (Lane et al. 2010). Although differences in sensory reactivity have been observed since the early descriptions of ASC, the aetiology, and exact nature, of these differences remains unknown. Recently, it has been suggested that an atypical interaction between information from the different senses may account for these differences (Iarocci and McDonald 2006), and in particular that temporal acuity (the ability to separate stimuli in time) between the senses may be reduced (see Stevenson et al. 2015 for a review).
Simultaneity judgement and Temporal Order Judgement (TOJ) tasks are commonly used to measure temporal acuity across sensory modalities (Stone et al. 2001; Vroomen et al. 2004). In a simultaneity judgement task participants are presented with crossmodal stimuli separated by a range of stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) and judge whether the stimuli were simultaneous or sequential. Alternatively, in a typical crossmodal TOJ task the participant is presented with stimuli from two different sensory modalities separated by a range of SOAs and asked to judge which stimulus came first. At shorter SOAs, it is more difficult to determine the order of presentation. The participant’s data can be fitted by a Psychometric function to extract measures of temporal acuity between the senses (typically the Just Noticeable Difference; JND, where a smaller JND represents increased acuity), and bias towards a particular sense (Point of Subjective Simultaneity; PSS, which indicates the separation between the stimuli at which the person perceives them to be simultaneous. See Fig. 1a). Previous studies of crossmodal TOJs in neurotypical participants (NTs) have indicated that the PSS varies between individuals and can be influenced by attention to a particular modality (Spence et al. 2001; Stone et al. 2001). That is, if a participant is attending to the modality of stimulus A then it can be presented later than stimulus B, but perceived as simultaneous because the participant will process stimulus A more quickly.
Temporal acuity to crossmodal stimuli appears to be affected over the lifetime in NTs. Visual-auditory simultaneity judgement tasks, have revealed that the temporal acuity of children and adolescents is reduced in comparison to young adults (Hillock et al. 2011; Hillock-Dunn and Wallace 2012). It therefore seems plausible that differences in the developmental trajectory of ASC could affect the maturation of this process. Indeed, reduced temporal acuity for visual-auditory stimuli has been observed across a range of task types (see Stevenson et al. 2015). For instance, children and adolescents with ASC have previously completed simultaneity judgement tasks (Stevenson et al. 2014), and TOJ tasks (de Boer-Schellekens et al. 2013) with stimuli of varying complexity. Participants made judgements regarding simple flash-beeps, speech stimuli and complex, but non-social visual-auditory stimuli. Both studies indicated that acuity was reduced with increasing stimulus complexity. Participants with ASC had reduced acuity (larger JNDs) compared to controls (although this effect was only observed for speech stimuli in the Stevenson et al. 2014 study). Reduced temporal acuity to simple stimuli is associated with poorer visual-auditory speech perception in ASC (Stevenson et al. 2014), which may suggest that low-level differences in temporal processing of crossmodal stimuli can impact on higher level communication issues in ASC.
The temporal alignment between the senses can also be inferred from selective attention tasks in which participants make judgements about a stimulus in a particular modality, while ignoring distracting information presented in a second modality. The extent of temporal separation between the stimuli typically determines whether the stimuli will interact; stimuli which occur closer in time to the target being more likely to influence the participant’s response (Shams et al. 2002; Shore et al. 2006). Studies which have investigated the effects of auditory information on visual judgements across different delays have revealed differences in how NTs and individuals with ASC are affected by temporal separation. Children with ASC were influenced by crossmodal distractors over a range of SOAs twice the size of that for NTs (Foss-Feig et al. 2010; Kwakye et al. 2011).
In the present study, we sought to extend the characterisation of the temporal aspects of multisensory processing in ASC and explore the extent to which multisensory processing may be related to sensory reactivity. The majority of previous research has focused on visual-auditory processing in ASC, but sensory differences associated with touch and hearing are frequently reported (Kern et al. 2006). Consequently, it is important to explore whether any deficits in temporal processing in ASC are specific to visual-auditory interactions, or can also be observed for other bimodal pairings. Adult participants completed TOJs for low level visual-auditory, tactile-auditory and auditory-tactile pairings. TOJ tasks rather than simultaneity judgement tasks were used since apparently reduced temporal acuity in a simultaneity judgement task could be produced by a stronger inclination to report stimuli presented in close temporal proximity as simultaneous (response bias). It was anticipated that temporal acuity would be reduced in participants with ASC in comparison to controls (increased JNDs for each bimodal pairing). There was no directional hypothesis for differences in PSSs between the groups, since differences have not been reported in the literature. Finally, we investigated whether experimental measures (JNDs and PSS) of multisensory temporal processing were related to self-report of sensory reactivity across the groups. We anticipated that increased JNDs would predict more atypical sensory reactivity as this would indicate a reduced ability to separate crossmodal stimuli in time which could lead to perceptually overwhelming experiences. Similarly increased PSS might predict sensory reactivity as this would indicate a bias (or increased attention) towards a particular sense.

Methods

Participants

ASC (n = 18) and NT control (n = 18) participants were matched for age, IQ, gender and handedness (see Table 1 for demographic information). Four participants with ASC and four controls were female. One participant with ASC and one NT control were left-handed as self-reported using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield 1971). All participants had a full scale IQ > 80 as measured using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Wecshler 1999). The diagnosis of ASC participants was confirmed using module 4 of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-2; Lord et al. 2000) by a certified assessor. All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision (6/6 vision in both eyes as measured using Snellens test of visual acuity). To assess sensory reactivity all participants completed the Glasgow Sensory Quotient (GSQ; Robertson and Simmons 2013). The GSQ correlates strongly with autistic traits in both ASC and NT individuals and as such has been recommended as the most suitable instrument for measuring sensory reactivity in ASC (Horder et al. 2014).
Table 1
Participant characteristics
 
ASC (n = 18)
NT (n = 18)
t (34)
p
 
Age
31 ± 8.43
31.05 ± 8.71
0.02
.985
 
FSIQ
116.56 ± 9.67
112.18 ± 7.56
1.49
.147
 
ADOS
8.55 ± 2.28
GSQ score (Bonferonni corrected, α = .013)
Total
76.06 ± 24.28
31.44 ± 17.01
6.38
<.001
Hyper sensitivity
38.56 ± 14.47
16.78 ± 9.65
5.31
<.001
Hypo sensitivity
37.50 ± 12.42
14.67 ± 8.83
6.38
<.001

Apparatus and Stimuli

Participants sat at a desk in a dimly lit room and were instructed to focus on a central fixation point consisting of a grey cross (10 mm) in the centre of the screen. All stimuli were controlled through a PC running E-Prime (Psychology Software Tools Inc., USA). A speaker was used to present sound files (sine wave, 440 Hz, 0.8 AMPs) through a Tacamp amplifier (Dancer Design, St. Helens UK). Auditory stimuli comprised a single 8 ms beep. The speaker was placed on a foam cube to prevent the participants feeling vibrations emitted by the speaker through the table. A 65 × 85 mm foam block was positioned 25 mm in front of the speaker. A bone conductor (Oticon Limited, B/C 2-PIN, 100 Ω, Hamilton, UK) which was driven by the same sound files was embedded in the foam cube and attached to the participant’s index finger on their dominant hand using double sided adhesive. Tactile stimuli comprised a single vibration of 8 ms. A single red LED which subtended a visual angle of approximately 2.24∘ was embedded in a black plastic cube (25 mm) positioned at the tip of the participant’s index finger. Visual stimuli comprised a single 8 ms flash (see Fig. 1b for apparatus).
White noise (~75dB SPL) was played through headphones throughout the experiment to prevent the participant from hearing sounds emitted by the bone conductor. All stimuli were clearly supra-threshold as confirmed by each participant before beginning the experiment.

Procedure

Participants were asked to report which stimulus came first for each bimodal pairing and their verbal response was recorded by the experimenter.
Stimuli were presented at ±28, 63, 98, 208 and 408 ms SOAs (Poliakoff et al. 2006; Spence et al. 2003; Zampini et al. 2003a, b, 2005). Each trial began with the onset of the central fixation cross followed by a delay randomly selected from a uniform distribution of 500–1000 ms (to prevent additional temporal cues). Then participants were randomly presented with either a visual-auditory, tactile-visual or auditory-tactile pairing. Each of the 30 possible trial types was presented twice in each block. There were five blocks, meaning that each SOA was presented 10 times and there were 300 trials in total. There was a delay of 1000 ms after the experimenter entered the participant’s response, during which time the screen went blank, before the central fixation cross appeared to indicate the next trial was due to commence. Before beginning the main experiment, participants completed 4 trials for each bimodal pairing with the SOA at ±408 ms as practice. These trials included feedback and the participant continued to the main experiment once performance for each pairing was at 75 % accuracy, confirming that all stimuli were suprathreshold and the task instructions understood.

Data Analysis

Responses for each stimulus pairing were converted to proportion of vision first (visual-auditory trials), tactile first (tactile-visual trials) and auditory first (auditory-tactile trials). Each participant’s data was then fitted by a cumulative Gaussian Psychometric function using the Palamedes toolbox for MATLAB (Kingdom and Prins 2009). Each participant’s JND (\(\frac{0.675}{\beta }\); Zampini et al. 2003a, b, 2005) and PSS were then extracted for each stimulus pairing (See Fig. 1b). Individual JND and PSS values were removed prior to analysis where the standard deviation (\(\frac{1}{\beta }\)) of the psychometric function was larger than the range of SOAs presented (visual-auditory ASC n = 1, auditory-tactile ASC n = 1; see Spence et al. 2001 in which similar exclusion criteria were used). A measure of the goodness of fit of each function was estimated (pDev, range from 0 to 1 with values closer to 1 representing better fits; see Table 2 for group averages). Further data points were removed from analysis where pDev < 0.05 (Kingdom and Prins 2009). It is unlikely that poorly fitted functions, or those with an SOA beyond the stimulus range, gave a reasonable estimate of the participant’s underlying sensory mechanisms.
Table 2
Measures of psychometric function goodness of fit (mean pDev ± SD) for each modality pairing where values closer to 1 represent better fits. The final sample size in each condition is also included
 
pDev
Final n
Visual-auditory
 ASC
0.29 ± 0.165
15
 NT
0.34 ± 0.23
15
Tactile-visual
 ASC
0.45 ± 0.29
18
 NT
0.46 ± 0.30
16
Auditory-tactile
 ASC
0.41 ± 0.31
14
 NT
0.37 ± 0.21
12
As the remaining samples contained a number of missing cases a Kruskall-Wallis test was then conducted to compare the JND and PSS in each condition between the groups.
Bayes factors were also calculated to compare the strength of the evidence for our principle research hypothesis (participants with ASC would produce higher JNDs than NT participants in each condition) relative to that for the null hypothesis (no difference in JNDs between the groups). Bayesian independent samples t-tests with default priors were conducted comparing JNDs between the groups in each condition using JASP (https://​jasp-stats.​org/​). A Bayes factor measuring evidence for the research hypothesis over the null hypothesis given the observed data is usually denoted BF10. The value BF10 indicates how many times more likely the research hypothesis is than the null hypothesis. Accordingly a value of BF10 = 1 suggests the evidence does not favour either the research or the null hypothesis. Increasingly large values of BF10 > 1 suggest greater evidence for the research evidence over the null, whereas decreasing values of BF10 < 1 suggest increasing evidence for the null hypothesis over the research hypothesis (Dienes 2014).
As an exploratory analysis, multiple regressions were calculated for each group to explore the relationship between sensory scores reported using the GSQ and participants JND and PSS in each condition.

Results

Each group’s median responses to stimuli presented at each SOA for each modality pairing are given in Fig. 2.

Just Noticable Difference

The size of the JNDs did not differ between participants with ASC or NT for any modality pairing (see Fig. 3). ASC participant JNDs were larger than NTs for the visual-auditory (χ2 (1, n = 30) < 0.01, p = .950) and for tactile-visual (χ2 (1, n = 34) = 1.22, p = .270) modality pairings, but these differences did not reach statistical significance. The JNDs of NT participants were larger than the ASC group for the tactile-auditory modality pairings (χ2 (1, n = 26) = 1.17, p = .280), but this did not reach statistical significance. A Bayes Factor (BF) was calculated to compare JNDs between the groups for each modality pairing. For visual-auditory JNDs BF10 = 0.35 meaning that the research hypothesis was 0.35 times more likely than the null given the data (or equivalently that the null was 2.82 times more likely than the research hypothesis). For tactile-visual JNDs BF10 = 0.74 meaning the research hypothesis was 0.74 times more likely than the null given the data (or that the null was 1.34 times more likely). For auditory-tactile JNDs BF10 = 0.19 meaning the research hypothesis was 0.19 times more likely than the null given the data (or that the null was 5.28 times more likely).
A multiple regression was calculated exploring the relationship between self-reported sensory reactivity score as measured using the GSQ and the JND for each bimodal pairing (Table 3). The regression model was not statistically significant (F (3, 19) = 2.20, p = .128). However, JNDs for the visual-tactile modality pairing were a significant predictor of sensory reactivity, indicating that participants who had reduced temporal acuity to tactile-visual stimuli also reported more atypical sensory reactivity. A follow-up Pearson’s correlation coefficient revealed a non-significant positive correlation between tactile-visual JNDs and sensory reactivity scores (r (34) = .323, p = .063). See Fig. 4a.
Table 3
Regression data comparing JND and PSS in each modality pairing with total sensory scores
 
R2 adjusted
β
t
p
JND
.16
   
 Visual-auditory JND
 
.24
0.59
.562
 Tactile-visual JND
 
.65
2.14
.048*
 Auditory-tactile JND
 
.04
0.12
.903
PSS
.16
   
 Visual-auditory PSS
 
.68
2.56
.021*
 Tactile-visual PSS
 
.17
0.74
.473
 Auditory-tactile PSS
 
.35
1.39
.184
Significant predictors are highlighted with an asterisk

Point of Subjective Simultaneity

There were no statistically significant differences in the PSS between the groups for any of the modality pairings (see Fig. 3). The ASC participants exhibited a more positive PSS for the visual-auditory pairings than NTs, but this difference was not statistically significant (χ2 (1, n = 30) = 0.36, p = .548). For the tactile-visual pairing, the ASC participants were more positive than the NTs, but this difference was not statistically significant (χ2 (1, n = 34) = 0.57, p = .448). For the auditory-tactile pairing, NT participants were more negative, but this difference was not statistically significant (χ2 (1, n = 26) = 0.04, p = .837).
A multiple regression was conducted to explore the relationship between self-reported sensory reactivity and the PSS for each bimodal pairing (Table 3). The regression model revealed that PSS scores overall did not significantly predict sensory reactivity (F (3, 19) = 2.21, p = .127). However, PSS scores in the visual-auditory condition were a significant positive predictor of sensory reactivity, indicating that participants who required the auditory stimulus to be presented prior to the visual stimulus in order to judge the stimuli as simultaneous reported more atypical sensory reactivity (see Fig. 4). A follow-up Pearson’s correlation coefficient revealed a non-significant positive correlation between visual-auditory PSS and sensory reactivity scores (r (30) = .29, p = .129).

Discussion

The aim of the current study was to explore temporal acuity between the senses in adults with ASC. The data suggest that temporal acuity (JND) does not differ between the groups for any modality pairing, although there was greater variability in performance in the ASC group. The data also suggest that there are no between group differences in bias towards a stimulus (PSS). Interestingly, both the JND in the visual-tactile condition and the PSS in the visual-auditory condition predicted self-reported sensory reactivity.
The findings of the current study contrast with previous research suggesting that temporal acuity is reduced for simple visual-auditory stimuli in children and young adults with ASC (de Boer-Schellekens et al. 2013; Stevenson et al. 2014). As visual-auditory temporal acuity is believed to be reduced in ASC, it is interesting that in the present study the effect size was smallest (d = 0.02) for the between group comparison of visual-auditory JNDs. This suggests that temporal acuity is comparable to NTs in some adults with ASC (see de Boer-Schellekens et al. 2013; Poole et al. 2015 for similar recent findings). As multisensory temporal acuity typically matures across development (Hillock et al. 2011; Hillock-Dunn and Wallace 2012) there may be a delay in the maturation of this processing in ASC which may have ‘caught up’ with NT performance by adulthood (see Foxe et al. 2015; Taylor et al. 2010, for evidence of maturation of visual-auditory speech processing in adolescents with ASC). However, the variability in the ASC group’s performance is worth noting. For example, the most extreme values for visual-auditory JNDs (Fig. 3) display a participant close to 0 and another over 200 ms. In addition, a participant with ASC excluded from analysis produced a standard deviation beyond the range of SOAs which could suggest very poor acuity for visual-auditory stimuli (although see below for a possible limitation in the selection of SOA). This variability in acuity may reflect individual differences in the development of multisensory processing, or in the use of compensatory strategies.
The present study is the first to explore differences in multisensory temporal acuity in ASC across multiple modality pairings, but there are some limitations. The SOAs used were based on previous TOJ studies in NT participants. However, differences between ASC and NT performance on visual tasks including task-irrelevant auditory information have previously revealed between group differences for stimuli presented between 150 ms-300 ms SOA (Foss-Feig et al. 2010; Kwakye et al. 2011). As only a single SOA was presented within this range, it is possible that the current experiment was not optimised to draw out differences between the groups. Similarly, there were only 10 repetitions of each SOA which may have contributed to a number of participant’s functions being poorly fitted. Nevertheless, the current investigation suggests that temporal acuity is not universally impaired in adults with ASC. There is a need for further studies exploring temporal acuity between the senses across adolescence and into early adulthood in both NTs and in ASC.
The current findings revealed novel relationships between aspects of multisensory temporal processing and self-reports of sensory reactivity. In the tactile-visual condition participant JNDs were a significant predictor of self-reported sensory reactivity, such that those with reduced tactile-visual temporal acuity reported greater sensory reactivity. Reduced tactile-visual temporal acuity could lead to atypical experiences of touch which could impact on higher level processes such as the experience, and use, of inter-personal touch (Poole et al. 2015) and in planning movements, particularly involving objects (Gowen and Hamilton 2013). Furthermore, participants who required a greater auditory lead to perceive visual-auditory stimuli as simultaneous reported more atypical sensory reactivity. Counterintuitively, the auditory lead suggests a bias towards visual information as it means that the auditory stimulus must be presented before the visual stimulus in order for them to be judged as simultaneous; i.e. the visual stimulus is processed more quickly. Previous research in NTs has suggested that the PSS can be biased towards an attended stimulus and is processed more quickly (Spence et al. 2001; Spence and Parise 2010) and selective attention has been implicated in TOJ tasks (Binder, 2015). The current finding therefore suggests that increased attention towards visual information may also contribute to sensory reactivity. Indeed, recent studies have observed differences in the selective attention to vision in ASC (Murphy et al. 2014; Occelli et al. 2013). The finding that a visual bias predicted sensory traits is partially consistent with findings from a visual-auditory simultaneity judgement task in NTs, which indicated that a shift in PSS towards the auditory stimulus was correlated positively with autistic traits (Donohue et al. 2012 1). That is, those with higher autistic traits showed a similar pattern to the participants in the current study with more atypical sensory reactivity.
As previous investigations have indicated that the temporal acuity of crossmodal stimuli can be enhanced with experience (Donohue et al. 2010), and training (Powers et al. 2009; Stevenson et al. 2013; Vroomen et al. 2004) the current findings suggest that such approaches may be effective in reducing sensory reactivity in ASC.
In summary, the current investigation provided no evidence for reduced temporal acuity to crossmodal stimuli in adults with ASC. It may be that there is a developmental delay in the maturation of this process which has ‘caught up’ with NT performance by adulthood. However, the variability in the ASC data suggests that there are important individual differences in temporal processing of crossmodal stimuli, which merit further investigation. Indeed, the tactile-visual JND and visual-auditory PSS data were predictors of sensory reactivity. Participants with reduced tactile-visual temporal acuity and those who were more biased towards vision for visual-auditory judgements reported more sensory reactivity. This preliminary finding suggests that both reduced tactile-visual temporal acuity and atypical selective attention to vision warrant further investigation to understand and ameliorate atypical sensory reactivity.

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by the Medical Research Council, UK (Grant Code: MR/J500410/1).

Author Contributions

Daniel Poole designed the experiment, collected the data and wrote the manuscript. Emma Gowen contributed to the design of the experiment and provided comments on the manuscript. Paul A. Warren contributed to the design of the experiment and provided comments on the manuscript. Ellen Poliakoff contributed to the design of the experiment and provided comments on the manuscript.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

Dr. Daniel Poole, Dr. Emma Gowen, Dr. Paul A Warren, Dr. Ellen Poliakoff declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the University of Manchester ethics committee in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

BSL GOP_opleiding GZ-psycholoog

Voetnoten
1
Although the authors of this study erroneously interpreted this effect as a bias towards auditory information.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Foxe, J. J., Molholm, S., Del Bene, V. A., Frey, H. P., Russo, N. N., Blanco, D., Saint-Amour, D., & Ross, L. A. (2015). Severe multisensory speech integration deficits in high-functioning school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and their resolution during early adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 25(2), 298–312. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht213.CrossRefPubMed Foxe, J. J., Molholm, S., Del Bene, V. A., Frey, H. P., Russo, N. N., Blanco, D., Saint-Amour, D., & Ross, L. A. (2015). Severe multisensory speech integration deficits in high-functioning school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and their resolution during early adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 25(2), 298–312. doi:10.​1093/​cercor/​bht213.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kern, J. K., Trivedi, M. H., Garver, C. R., Grannemann, B. D., Andrews, A., Savla, J. S., Johnson, D. G., Mehta, J. G., & Schroeder, J. L. (2006). The pattern of sensory processing abnormalities in autism. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 10(5), 480–494. doi:10.1177/1362361306066564.CrossRef Kern, J. K., Trivedi, M. H., Garver, C. R., Grannemann, B. D., Andrews, A., Savla, J. S., Johnson, D. G., Mehta, J. G., & Schroeder, J. L. (2006). The pattern of sensory processing abnormalities in autism. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 10(5), 480–494. doi:10.​1177/​1362361306066564​.CrossRef
go back to reference Kingdom, F. A. A., & Prins, N. (2009). Psychophysics: A practical introduction. London: Academic Press. Kingdom, F. A. A., & Prins, N. (2009). Psychophysics: A practical introduction. London: Academic Press.
go back to reference Lane, A. E., Young, R. L., Baker, A. E. Z., & Angley, M. T. (2010). Sensory processing subtypes in autism: Association with adaptive behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(1), 112–122. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0840-2.CrossRefPubMed Lane, A. E., Young, R. L., Baker, A. E. Z., & Angley, M. T. (2010). Sensory processing subtypes in autism: Association with adaptive behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(1), 112–122. doi:10.​1007/​s10803-009-0840-2.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., Pickles, A. & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., Pickles, A. & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.
go back to reference Murphy, J. W., Foxe, J. J., Peters, J. B., & Molholm, S. (2014). Susceptibility to distraction in autism spectrum disorder: Probing the integrity of oscillatory alpha-band suppression mechanisms. Autism Research, 7(4), 442–458. doi:10.1002/aur.1374.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Murphy, J. W., Foxe, J. J., Peters, J. B., & Molholm, S. (2014). Susceptibility to distraction in autism spectrum disorder: Probing the integrity of oscillatory alpha-band suppression mechanisms. Autism Research, 7(4), 442–458. doi:10.​1002/​aur.​1374.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Occelli, V., Esposito, G., Venuti, P., Arduino, G. M., & Zampini, M. (2013). Attentional shifts between audition and vision in autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(4), 517–525. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2012.12.003.CrossRef Occelli, V., Esposito, G., Venuti, P., Arduino, G. M., & Zampini, M. (2013). Attentional shifts between audition and vision in autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(4), 517–525. doi:10.​1016/​j.​rasd.​2012.​12.​003.CrossRef
go back to reference Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.CrossRefPubMed Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Poole, D., Gowen, E., Warren, P. A., & Poliakoff, E. (2015). Investigating visual–tactile interactions over time and space in adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(10), 3316–3326. doi:10.1007/s10803-015-2492-8.CrossRefPubMed Poole, D., Gowen, E., Warren, P. A., & Poliakoff, E. (2015). Investigating visual–tactile interactions over time and space in adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(10), 3316–3326. doi:10.​1007/​s10803-015-2492-8.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Robertson, A. E., & Simmons, D. R. (2013). The relationship between sensory sensitivity and autistic traits in the general population. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 775–784. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1608-7. Robertson, A. E., & Simmons, D. R. (2013). The relationship between sensory sensitivity and autistic traits in the general population. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 775–784. doi:10.​1007/​s10803-012-1608-7.
go back to reference Shams, L., Kamitani, Y., & Shimojo, S. (2002). Visual illusion induced by sound. Cognitive Brain Research, 14(1), 147–152.CrossRefPubMed Shams, L., Kamitani, Y., & Shimojo, S. (2002). Visual illusion induced by sound. Cognitive Brain Research, 14(1), 147–152.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Spence, C., Baddeley, R., Zampini, M., James, R., & Shore, D. I. (2003). Multisensory temporal order judgments: When two locations are better than one. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(2), 318–328.CrossRef Spence, C., Baddeley, R., Zampini, M., James, R., & Shore, D. I. (2003). Multisensory temporal order judgments: When two locations are better than one. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(2), 318–328.CrossRef
go back to reference Spence, C., Shore, D., & Klein, R. (2001). Multisensory prior entry. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 130(4), 799–832.CrossRefPubMed Spence, C., Shore, D., & Klein, R. (2001). Multisensory prior entry. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 130(4), 799–832.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stevenson, R. A., Segers, M., Ferber, S., Barense, M. D., Camarata, S., & Wallace, M. T. (2015). Keeping time in the brain: Autism spectrum disorder and audiovisual temporal processing. Autism Research, 24(9). doi:10.1002/aur.1566. Stevenson, R. A., Segers, M., Ferber, S., Barense, M. D., Camarata, S., & Wallace, M. T. (2015). Keeping time in the brain: Autism spectrum disorder and audiovisual temporal processing. Autism Research, 24(9). doi:10.​1002/​aur.​1566.
go back to reference Stevenson, R. A., Siemann, J. K., Woynaroski, T. G., Schneider, B. C., Eberly, H. E., Camarata, S. M., & Wallace, M. T. (2014). Evidence for Diminished Multisensory Integration in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2179-6. Stevenson, R. A., Siemann, J. K., Woynaroski, T. G., Schneider, B. C., Eberly, H. E., Camarata, S. M., & Wallace, M. T. (2014). Evidence for Diminished Multisensory Integration in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. doi:10.​1007/​s10803-014-2179-6.
go back to reference Stone, J. V., Hunkin, N. M., Porrill, J., Wood, R., Keeler, V., Beanland, M., Portn, M., & Porter, N. R. (2001). When is now? Perception of simultaneity. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 268(1462), 31–38. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1326.CrossRef Stone, J. V., Hunkin, N. M., Porrill, J., Wood, R., Keeler, V., Beanland, M., Portn, M., & Porter, N. R. (2001). When is now? Perception of simultaneity. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 268(1462), 31–38. doi:10.​1098/​rspb.​2000.​1326.CrossRef
go back to reference Taylor, N., Isaac, C., & Milne, E. (2010). A comparison of the development of audiovisual integration in children with autism spectrum disorders and typically developing children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(11), 1403–1411. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1000-4.CrossRefPubMed Taylor, N., Isaac, C., & Milne, E. (2010). A comparison of the development of audiovisual integration in children with autism spectrum disorders and typically developing children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(11), 1403–1411. doi:10.​1007/​s10803-010-1000-4.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. San Antonia: The Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. San Antonia: The Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Zampini, M., Shore, D. I., & Spence, C. (2003b). Multisensory temporal order judgments: The role of hemispheric redundancy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 50(1–2), 165–180. doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00132-6. Zampini, M., Shore, D. I., & Spence, C. (2003b). Multisensory temporal order judgments: The role of hemispheric redundancy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 50(1–2), 165–180. doi:10.​1016/​S0167-8760(03)00132-6.
Metagegevens
Titel
Brief Report: Which Came First? Exploring Crossmodal Temporal Order Judgements and Their Relationship with Sensory Reactivity in Autism and Neurotypicals
Auteurs
Daniel Poole
Emma Gowen
Paul A. Warren
Ellen Poliakoff
Publicatiedatum
04-10-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 1/2017
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2925-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2017

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1/2017 Naar de uitgave