Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2017

26-09-2015 | Original Article

Bimanual comfort depends on how extreme either hand’s posture is, not on which hand is in the more extreme posture

Auteurs: Kate M. Chapman, David A. Rosenbaum

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Although hand preference is one of the best known features of performance, a recent study of object transfer behavior (Coelho, Studenka, & Rosenbaum, J Exp Psychol Human Percept Perform, 40:718–730, 2014) showed that people place greater emphasis on using the hand that avoids extreme joint angles than on using the hand they normally prefer. In the present study, we sought converging evidence for the hypothesis that adopting midrange joint angles by either hand (the preferred-posture hypothesis) is more important than using the preferred hand in particular to adopt midrange joint angles (the preferred-hand hypothesis). We asked participants to hold both of their hands in different orientations and to rate their comfort. Consistent with the preferred-posture hypothesis but contrary to the preferred-hand hypothesis, the bimanual comfort ratings were more strongly affected by how extreme the two hands’ postures were than by which of the hands was in the more extreme posture. The data support a theory of action planning, the posture-based motion planning theory, which says that whole-body postural comfort is a key ingredient for physical action planning.
Literatuur
go back to reference Aruin, A. S., & Latash, M. L. (1995). Directional specificity of postural muscles in feed-forward postural reactions during fast voluntary arm movements. Experimental Brain Research, 103, 323–332.CrossRefPubMed Aruin, A. S., & Latash, M. L. (1995). Directional specificity of postural muscles in feed-forward postural reactions during fast voluntary arm movements. Experimental Brain Research, 103, 323–332.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movements. London: Pergamon. Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movements. London: Pergamon.
go back to reference Bullock, D., Bongers, R. M., Lankhorst, M., & Beek, P. J. (1999). A vector-integration-to-endpoint model for performance of viapoint movements. Neural Networks, 12, 1–29.CrossRefPubMed Bullock, D., Bongers, R. M., Lankhorst, M., & Beek, P. J. (1999). A vector-integration-to-endpoint model for performance of viapoint movements. Neural Networks, 12, 1–29.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chapman, K. M. (2013). One good turn deserves another: Constraint hierarchies for action selection in bimanual rotation (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). State College: The Pennsylvania State University. Chapman, K. M. (2013). One good turn deserves another: Constraint hierarchies for action selection in bimanual rotation (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). State College: The Pennsylvania State University.
go back to reference Chapman, K. M. & Rosenbaum, D. A. Two-hand grasp choices reveal constraint hierarchies for manual action planning (in preparation). Chapman, K. M. & Rosenbaum, D. A. Two-hand grasp choices reveal constraint hierarchies for manual action planning (in preparation).
go back to reference Coelho, C. J., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2013). Is handedness just response bias? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 957–962.CrossRef Coelho, C. J., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2013). Is handedness just response bias? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 957–962.CrossRef
go back to reference Coelho, C. J., Studenka, B. E., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2014). End-state comfort trumps handedness in object manipulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 718–730.PubMed Coelho, C. J., Studenka, B. E., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2014). End-state comfort trumps handedness in object manipulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 718–730.PubMed
go back to reference Corballis, M. (1997). The genetics and evolution of handedness. Psychological Review, 104, 714–727.CrossRefPubMed Corballis, M. (1997). The genetics and evolution of handedness. Psychological Review, 104, 714–727.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Costantini, M., & Haggard, P. (2007). The rubber hand illusion: Sensitivity and reference frame for body ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 229–240.CrossRefPubMed Costantini, M., & Haggard, P. (2007). The rubber hand illusion: Sensitivity and reference frame for body ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 229–240.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cruse, H., Wischmeyer, E., Brüwer, M., Brockfeld, P., & Dress, A. (1990). On the cost functions for the control of the human arm movement. Biological Cybernetics, 62, 519–528.CrossRefPubMed Cruse, H., Wischmeyer, E., Brüwer, M., Brockfeld, P., & Dress, A. (1990). On the cost functions for the control of the human arm movement. Biological Cybernetics, 62, 519–528.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., Rosenbaum, D. A., & Vaughan, J. (1997). Speed and sequential effects in reaching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 404–428.PubMed Fischer, M. H., Rosenbaum, D. A., & Vaughan, J. (1997). Speed and sequential effects in reaching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 404–428.PubMed
go back to reference Fisher, S., Greenberg, R. P., & Reihman, J. (1984). Body perception and somatic discomfort in relation to right- versus left-handedness. Cortex, 20(2), 285–294.CrossRefPubMed Fisher, S., Greenberg, R. P., & Reihman, J. (1984). Body perception and somatic discomfort in relation to right- versus left-handedness. Cortex, 20(2), 285–294.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Franz, E. A., & Ramachandran, V. S. (1998). Bimanual coupling in amputees with phantom limbs. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 443–444.CrossRefPubMed Franz, E. A., & Ramachandran, V. S. (1998). Bimanual coupling in amputees with phantom limbs. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 443–444.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Guiard, Y. (1987). Asymmetric division of labor in human skilled bimanual action: The kinematic chain as a model. Journal of Motor Behavior, 19, 486–517.CrossRefPubMed Guiard, Y. (1987). Asymmetric division of labor in human skilled bimanual action: The kinematic chain as a model. Journal of Motor Behavior, 19, 486–517.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hamilton, A. F. D. C., & Wolpert, D. M. (2002). Controlling the statistics of action: obstacle avoidance. Journal of Neurophysiology, 87, 2434–2440.PubMed Hamilton, A. F. D. C., & Wolpert, D. M. (2002). Controlling the statistics of action: obstacle avoidance. Journal of Neurophysiology, 87, 2434–2440.PubMed
go back to reference Helmholtz, H. (1866/1962). Handbook of physiological optics. New York: Dover (Translation of Handbuch der physiologischen Optik . Hamburg: Voss). Helmholtz, H. (1866/1962). Handbook of physiological optics. New York: Dover (Translation of Handbuch der physiologischen Optik . Hamburg: Voss).
go back to reference Huhn III, J. M., Schimpf, K. A., & van der Wel, R. P. (2014). Symmetries in action: On the interactive nature of planning constraints for bimanual object manipulation. Experimental Brain Research, 1–9. Huhn III, J. M., Schimpf, K. A., & van der Wel, R. P. (2014). Symmetries in action: On the interactive nature of planning constraints for bimanual object manipulation. Experimental Brain Research, 1–9.
go back to reference Jones, L. A., & Lederman, S. J. (2006). Human hand function. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Jones, L. A., & Lederman, S. J. (2006). Human hand function. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Jung, E. S., & Choe, J. (1996). Human reach posture prediction based on psychophysical discomfort. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 18, 173–179.CrossRef Jung, E. S., & Choe, J. (1996). Human reach posture prediction based on psychophysical discomfort. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 18, 173–179.CrossRef
go back to reference Kudar, K. (1994). Az egyes tesre´szek e´s testta´jak tudatossa´gi szintjei a testke´pben, e´s ezek szeme´ lyise´gtulajdonsa´gokkal valo´ o¨ sszefu¨ gge´se´nek vizsga´lata fe´rfi sportolo´kon. Kandida´tusi e´rtekeze´s, Budapest. Kudar, K. (1994). Az egyes tesre´szek e´s testta´jak tudatossa´gi szintjei a testke´pben, e´s ezek szeme´ lyise´gtulajdonsa´gokkal valo´ o¨ sszefu¨ gge´se´nek vizsga´lata fe´rfi sportolo´kon. Kandida´tusi e´rtekeze´s, Budapest.
go back to reference Latash, M. (2000). There is no motor redundancy in human movements. There is motor abundance. Motor Control, 4(3), 259.CrossRefPubMed Latash, M. (2000). There is no motor redundancy in human movements. There is motor abundance. Motor Control, 4(3), 259.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lederman, S. J., & Klatzky, R. L. (1987). Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 342–368.CrossRefPubMed Lederman, S. J., & Klatzky, R. L. (1987). Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 342–368.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lederman, S. J., & Klatzky, R. L. (2009). Haptic perception: A tutorial. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(7), 1439–1459.CrossRef Lederman, S. J., & Klatzky, R. L. (2009). Haptic perception: A tutorial. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(7), 1439–1459.CrossRef
go back to reference Mayer, A., Kudar, K., Bretz, K., & Tihanyi, J. (2008). Body schema and body awareness of amputees. Prosthetics and Orthotics International, 32(2), 363–382.CrossRefPubMed Mayer, A., Kudar, K., Bretz, K., & Tihanyi, J. (2008). Body schema and body awareness of amputees. Prosthetics and Orthotics International, 32(2), 363–382.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Meulenbroek, R. G. J., Rosenbaum, D. A., Thomassen, A. J. W. M., Loukopoulos, L. D., & Vaughan, J. (1996). Adaptation of a reaching model to handwriting: How different effectors can produce the same written output, and other results. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 59, 64–74.CrossRefPubMed Meulenbroek, R. G. J., Rosenbaum, D. A., Thomassen, A. J. W. M., Loukopoulos, L. D., & Vaughan, J. (1996). Adaptation of a reaching model to handwriting: How different effectors can produce the same written output, and other results. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 59, 64–74.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.CrossRefPubMed Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ramachandran, V. S., & Rogers-Ramachandran, D. (1996). Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 263, 377–386.CrossRef Ramachandran, V. S., & Rogers-Ramachandran, D. (1996). Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 263, 377–386.CrossRef
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Chapman, K. M., Weigelt, M., Weiss, D. J., & van der Wel, R. (2012). Cognition, action and object manipulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 924–946.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Rosenbaum, D. A., Chapman, K. M., Weigelt, M., Weiss, D. J., & van der Wel, R. (2012). Cognition, action and object manipulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 924–946.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Herbort, O., van der Wel, R., & Weiss, D. J. (2014). What’s in a grasp? American Scientist (September–October issue), 102, 366–373. Rosenbaum, D. A., Herbort, O., van der Wel, R., & Weiss, D. J. (2014). What’s in a grasp? American Scientist (September–October issue), 102, 366–373.
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Loukopoulos, L. D., Meulenbroek, R. G. M., Vaughan, J., & Engelbrecht, S. E. (1995). Planning reaches by evaluating stored postures. Psychological Review, 102, 28–67.CrossRefPubMed Rosenbaum, D. A., Loukopoulos, L. D., Meulenbroek, R. G. M., Vaughan, J., & Engelbrecht, S. E. (1995). Planning reaches by evaluating stored postures. Psychological Review, 102, 28–67.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Meulenbroek, R. G., Vaughan, J., & Jansen, C. (2001). Posture-based motion planning: Applications to grasping. Psychological Review, 108, 709–734.CrossRefPubMed Rosenbaum, D. A., Meulenbroek, R. G., Vaughan, J., & Jansen, C. (2001). Posture-based motion planning: Applications to grasping. Psychological Review, 108, 709–734.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., van Heugten, C. M., & Caldwell, G. E. (1996). From cognition to biomechanics and back: The end-state comfort effect and the middle-is-faster effect. Acta Psychologica, 71, 59–85.CrossRef Rosenbaum, D. A., van Heugten, C. M., & Caldwell, G. E. (1996). From cognition to biomechanics and back: The end-state comfort effect and the middle-is-faster effect. Acta Psychologica, 71, 59–85.CrossRef
go back to reference Solnik, S., Pazin, N., Coelho, C. J., Rosenbaum, D. A., Zatsiorsky, V. M., & Latash, M. L. (2014). Postural sway and perceived comfort in pointing tasks. Neuroscience Letters, 569, 18–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Solnik, S., Pazin, N., Coelho, C. J., Rosenbaum, D. A., Zatsiorsky, V. M., & Latash, M. L. (2014). Postural sway and perceived comfort in pointing tasks. Neuroscience Letters, 569, 18–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Taylor-Clarke, M., Kennett, S., & Haggard, P. (2002). Vision modulates somatosensory cortical processing. Current Biology, 12, 233–236.CrossRefPubMed Taylor-Clarke, M., Kennett, S., & Haggard, P. (2002). Vision modulates somatosensory cortical processing. Current Biology, 12, 233–236.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tversky, A. (1972). Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. Psychological Review, 79, 281–299.CrossRef Tversky, A. (1972). Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. Psychological Review, 79, 281–299.CrossRef
go back to reference Vaughan, J., Barany, D. A., Sali, A. W., Jax, S. A., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2010). Extending Fitts’ Law to three-dimensional obstacle-avoidance movements: Support for the posture-based motion planning model. Experimental Brain Research, 207, 133–138.CrossRefPubMed Vaughan, J., Barany, D. A., Sali, A. W., Jax, S. A., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2010). Extending Fitts’ Law to three-dimensional obstacle-avoidance movements: Support for the posture-based motion planning model. Experimental Brain Research, 207, 133–138.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vaughan, J., Rosenbaum, D. A., & Meulenbroek, R. G. J. (2001). Planning reaching and grasping movements: The problem of obstacle avoidance. Motor Control, 5, 116–135.CrossRefPubMed Vaughan, J., Rosenbaum, D. A., & Meulenbroek, R. G. J. (2001). Planning reaching and grasping movements: The problem of obstacle avoidance. Motor Control, 5, 116–135.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Winters, J. M., & Kleweno, D. G. (1993). Effect of initial upper-limb alignment on muscle contributions to isometric strength curves. Journal of Biomechanics, 26, 143–153.CrossRefPubMed Winters, J. M., & Kleweno, D. G. (1993). Effect of initial upper-limb alignment on muscle contributions to isometric strength curves. Journal of Biomechanics, 26, 143–153.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Motor prediction. Current Biology, 11, 729–732.CrossRef Wolpert, D. M., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Motor prediction. Current Biology, 11, 729–732.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Bimanual comfort depends on how extreme either hand’s posture is, not on which hand is in the more extreme posture
Auteurs
Kate M. Chapman
David A. Rosenbaum
Publicatiedatum
26-09-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0708-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2017

Psychological Research 1/2017 Naar de uitgave