Abundant research has indicated that parental psychological control, or parents’ behaviors aimed at manipulating the child to think, feel, and behave in prescribed ways (Barber,
1996), is detrimental to children’s psychological functioning (Barber and Harmon,
2002). Yet, our knowledge within this domain remains limited in crucial aspects, given that most previous studies have assessed psychological control as a uniform construct (Soenens and Beyers,
2012; Yu et al.,
2015). Recent research increasingly highlights the multidimensional nature of psychologically controlling parenting (Cheah et al.,
2019; Yu et al.,
2015). Differentiating psychological control between internal or covert (e.g., guilt induction) and external or overt (e.g., disrespecting the child by constraining their verbal expressions, or showing hostility toward them) could perhaps better explain the sometimes-inconsistent findings in the literature (Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
2010). This differentiation could also shed light on the distinct and less explored associations between specific types of psychological control and child outcomes (Romm et al.,
2020) in less heavily investigated cultural settings (see Scharf & Goldner,
2018; Yu et al.,
2015). From a universalistic perspective, such parenting practices frustrate children’s basic psychological needs and hence are detrimental across all cultures (Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
2010; Deci & Ryan,
2000). From a culture-specific perspective, however, specific psychological control practices, such as guilt induction, are normative and compatible with the cultural values of interdependency and group harmony in collectivistic cultural settings. Therefore, the negative effects of internal psychological control are claimed to be limited to individualistic cultural contexts (Fung & Lau,
2012; Yu et al.,
2019). Thus, further research is needed to investigate different psychological control forms in diverse cultural settings.
Moreover, research on psychological control has been conducted mainly among adolescents and has only recently covered earlier ages, such as toddlerhood and the preschool period. Although this limited research has revealed that the adverse effects of psychological control extend to earlier stages of development (Brenning et al.,
2020; Olsen et al.,
2002; Stone et al.,
2013), we know less about how internal and external psychological control - especially internal - relate to certain behavioral patterns in those ages. Lastly, previous research has mainly focused on mothers (Xing et al.,
2017; Zarra-Nezhad et al.,
2015). The challenges associated with gathering data from fathers, and assuming mothers as the primary caregivers, especially during the early stages of development, may have contributed to this reliance on a single caregiver (Mitchell et al.,
2007). However, fathers across many countries are getting increasingly involved in childcare (Gimenez-Nadal & Sevilla-Sanz,
2012; Schober,
2015). Recent research on fathers and young children also points to the unique value of fathers in children’s social-emotional outcomes (Havighurst et al.,
2019). So, it is important to incorporate fathers while investigating the role of internal and external psychological control in children’s adjustment to better understand fathers’ evolving role in it (Lamb,
2000).
We focused on social-emotional competence because it is synonymous with successful functioning within social surroundings (Rose-Krasnor & Denham,
2009). On a broader level, social competence reflects children’s effectiveness in social interactions and could manifest through social skills, popularity, positive social relationships, and successful pursuit of social goals (Rose-Krasnor,
1997). The extant literature on social competence has shown that inadequate social competence relates to psychopathology among preschoolers (Huber et al.,
2019) and that social competence is closely linked with emotional competence (as indexed, among others, through emotional regulation, emotion expressiveness, and emotion knowledge) (Denham et al.,
2003; Lemerise & Arsenio,
2000). Therefore, preschoolers’ social competence is an important developmental milestone that deserves further attention as it can grossly affect children’s subsequent adjustment and functioning (Caprara et al.,
2000; Letcher et al.,
2009).
Parental Psychological Control and Early Social-Emotional Competence
Parental psychological control refers to intrusive and manipulative parenting practices attempting to control children to think, act, or feel in specific ways (Barber,
1996; Barber & Harmon,
2002). To illustrate, parents might induce feelings of guilt and shame to encourage their children to comply with their requests. Abundant research has shown that parental psychological control is detrimental to children’s development as it relates, for instance, to lower self-esteem, decreased academic performance, and psychopathology (see Barber & Harmon,
2002, for a review; see Pinquart,
2017a; Pinquart,
2017b, for meta-analyses). According to the self-determination theory (SDT; Ryan & Deci,
2017), psychologically controlling parents are more likely to undermine their children’s well-being and psychological adjustment because such practices are expected to frustrate children’s psychological needs for autonomy (e.g., by pressuring children to think or behave in specific ways), relatedness (e.g., through love withdrawal and conditional regard), and competence (e.g., by providing negative feedback with expressions of disappointment) (Ahmad et al.,
2013; Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
2010).
Prior research has provided evidence regarding the debilitating effects of psychologically controlling parenting on young children’s socio-emotional adjustment. For instance, studies with three-year-old boys (Verhoeven et al.,
2010), and five- to eight-year-old children (Stone et al.,
2013; Xing et al.,
2017) reported positive associations between maternal psychological control and children’s internalizing (e.g., loneliness) and externalizing (e.g., aggression) problem behaviors. A few studies have further indicated that the relations between maternal psychological control and children’s maladjustment may depend on various factors such as children’s gender (e.g., mother-daughters dyads; see Casas et al.,
2006), negative reactivity (Morris et al.,
2002), ethnic or cultural background (Akcinar & Baydar,
2014), and whether maladjustment outcomes refer to internalizing or externalizing behavior problems (Olsen et al.,
2002).
Regarding the role of fathers’ psychological control, the literature has reported mixed findings. On the one hand, some studies reported that paternal psychological control was not predictive of child outcomes at all (e.g., Aunola & Nurmi,
2005; Hart et al.,
1998). Some studies showed that paternal psychological control is related to children’s behavioral problems. However, once the maternal effect was controlled for, these relations lost significance (Verhoeven et al.,
2010; Xing et al.,
2017). Some other studies suggested that paternal psychological control can have predictive value for certain sub-groups of children, such as only for temperamentally difficult children (e.g., Zarra-Nezhad et al.,
2015), or for certain relationships, such as father-son dyads (e.g., Nelson et al.,
2013). On the other hand, research with adolescents generally provides evidence for the unique predictive power of paternal psychological control cross-sectionally (e.g., Arim & Shapka,
2008), longitudinally (e.g., Lansford et al.,
2014), and cross-culturally (e.g., Soenens et al.,
2012).
A possible reason for the differentiations in the pattern of the relations between paternal and, to a lesser extent, maternal psychological control and maladjustment may reside in the way psychological control has been conceptualized and assessed (Barber & Xia,
2013). Specifically, whereas psychological control can take more overt forms such as displaying inconsistent emotional behavior toward the child and hostile verbal criticism, it could also manifest in more covert forms, such as guilt induction and love withdrawal (Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
2010).
Internal and External Psychological Control
Building on Schaefer’s work (
1965), Barber (
1996) conceptualized psychological control as consisting of guilt induction, erratic emotional behavior, personal attack, constraining verbal expressions, invalidating feelings, and love withdrawal practices. Researchers interested in psychological control have widely adopted this conceptualization (Soenens & Beyers,
2012). In light of the SDT (Ryan & Deci,
2017), psychologically controlling parenting could be distinguished into internally and externally controlling forms (Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
2010; Vansteenkiste et al.,
2005). Strategies such as guilt induction and love withdrawal map onto internally controlling parenting as they covertly evoke pressure on children to comply with parental demands to avoid guilt, shame, or anxiety. More overt strategies such as invalidating feelings or constraining verbal expressions align with externally controlling parenting as children experience external pressure to conform to the ways approved by their parents. This distinction is important because internally and externally controlling parenting has been noted to have differential effects on children’s internalization process (Assor et al.,
2004; Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
2010). Externally controlling parenting mostly elicits external regulation in children, rendering them more likely to obey their parents’ requests or to defy them whenever this is possible. On the other hand, internally controlling parenting mainly gives rise to partial internalization of parental demands to avoid feelings of guilt, shame, or anxiety they would feel in case they disappoint their parents (Deci & Ryan,
2008). Children of these parents experience an inner conflict and resentment toward parents as their need for autonomy and need for relatedness are pitted against each other. However, at the behavioral level, children with internally controlling parents may show more compliance with parental requests, compared to their peers who experience externally controlling parenting (Assor et al.,
2004; Grolnick,
2003; Roth et al.,
2009).
Theory and empirical research indicate that although both types of controlling parenting are detrimental to children’s psychological well-being, their behavioral correlates may differ (Ryan & Deci,
2017). Specifically, Soenens and Vansteenkiste (
2010) theorized that internal psychological control, which includes relational practices like love withdrawal and guilt induction, is more likely to relate to children’s internalizing rather than externalizing problems. This specialized relation could be attributed to the inner conflict internal psychological control generates. A partial internalization of parental demands might not manifest in overt behaviors (as reflected through externalizing problems) but may still surreptitiously undermine psychological well-being (as indexed through internalizing problems). Instead, more overt forms of (i.e., external) psychological control such as erratic emotional behavior seem to relate to children’s externalizing problems. According to the social learning theory (Bandura,
1973), children of parents with more overt, hostile, threatening practices are more likely to model their parents’ behavior and thus exhibit aggressive behavior (Nelson et al.,
2013; Soenens and Vansteenkiste,
2010).
Indeed, research has shown that parental use of external coercive practices that include slapping, yelling, and grabbing predicted children’s aggressive behaviors, while internal psychological control did not (Hart et al.,
1998). Such coercive parenting practices have been repeatedly related to children’s externalizing problems (Joussemet et al.,
2008; Pinquart,
2017a; Roskam,
2019). In contrast, maternal guilt induction has been associated with children’s social withdrawal (Nelson et al.,
2006), a finding which has been replicated in a series of longitudinal studies where maternal guilt induction predicted children’s increased negative emotions (Aunola et al.,
2013; Zarra-Nezhad et al.,
2015). Studies conducted with older children have yielded similar results. For instance, parental guilt induction was found to positively relate to adolescents’ internalizing problems (Levitt et al.,
2020; Rakow et al.,
2009), and the same was true for love withdrawal, which, along with guilt induction, longitudinally predicted early adolescents’ increased internalizing behavior problems (Xu et al.,
2020). Further, an intervention study demonstrated that less use of parental guilt induction lowered subsequent internalizing problems in children (McKee et al.,
2014). Taken together, the literature reviewed thus far suggests that internal and external psychological control forms may differentially relate to the types of problems children experience.
Although several studies with samples from non-Western backgrounds have documented positive associations between internal psychological control and children’s internalizing problems (e.g., Cheah et al.,
2019; Xu et al.,
2020), some others failed to do so. For instance, a cross-cultural study involving participants from the US and China demonstrated that external psychological control related to increased internalizing and externalizing problems (even after accounting for parental rejection) only in the American sample. In the Chinese sample, neither internal nor external psychological control showed a significant association with either internalizing or externalizing behavioral problems (Fung & Lau,
2012). Likewise, guilt induction was found to be unrelated to depressive symptoms among Chinese adolescents (Fang et al.,
2022) or even to predict less aggressive bullying behaviors among Chinese-American preschoolers (Yu et al.,
2019). Most likely, such non-harmful effects of guilt induction in the Chinese milieu could be explained by Chinese adolescents’ tendency to perceive internal psychological control as less controlling and less need-frustrating compared to their Western counterparts (Chen et al.,
2016). Therefore, how internal and external psychological control is associated with the manifestation of children’s behavior problems in non-Western cultural contexts deserves further investigation.
The Present Study
Grounded in the self-determination theory, this study examined the cross-sectional relations between mothers’ and fathers’ psychologically controlling parenting and preschoolers’ social-emotional functioning as indicated by anger-aggression, anxiety-withdrawal, and social competence. Contrary to the conventional approach of assessing psychological control as a uniform construct (Yu et al.,
2019), we aimed to distinguish between internal (i.e., guilt induction) and external (i.e., constraining verbal expressions and erratic emotional behavior) forms in line with the SDT perspective (Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
2010) and to explore their unique relations to children’s social-emotional adjustment. Considering the aforementioned debate questioning the universality of internal psychological control, we focused on an under-investigated cultural setting in our study. Although Turkish culture is positioned in the middle of Hofstede’s et al. (
2010) individualism-collectivism spectrum, it has been considered a collectivistic society due to its strong emphasis on close interpersonal and familial relations (Aycicegi-Dinn & Caldwell-Harris,
2011).
Further, contrary to previous research which has predominantly examined psychologically controlling parenting among adolescents, we focused on the preschool period to contribute to the limited amount of research in the field. Also, we included social-emotional functioning indicators as outcomes in this study since research has consistently shown that early social-emotional competence, or the lack of it, has concurrent and long-term implications for children’s functioning (Huber et al.,
2019; Stump et al.,
2009). Recent research also points to the evolving role of fathers in childcare and emphasizes the need to further include fathers in parenting and child development research (Cabrera et al.,
2000). We aimed to address this gap by exploring the relations between psychological control and children’s social-emotional functioning in both maternal and paternal contexts.
Importantly, we used different informants to assess internal and external psychological control (by asking parents about the practices they use toward their children) and children’s social-emotional adjustment (by asking preschool teachers to rate preschoolers’ anger-aggression, anxiety-withdrawal, and social competence). In that way, we were able to contain the problem of mono-method bias which typically inflates the derived relations when the same informants are used (Podsakoff et al.,
2012).
In line with assumptions derived from the self-determination theory (Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
2010) and prior empirical research (e.g., Nelson et al.,
2006; Stone et al.,
2013), we expected external psychological control to mainly relate to teacher-reported externalizing problems, and internal psychological control to relate to internalizing problems. Specifically, we formulated two main hypotheses and one exploratory research question. Based on the social learning theory (Bandura,
1973), we reasoned that the more parents admitted that they used external psychological control, the more their children would be rated by their teachers to employ these overtly aggressive and controlling practices in their relationships with peers. So, we anticipated external psychological control to relate negatively to children’s social competence (Hypothesis 1a), and positively to anger-aggression (Hypothesis 1b). We expected no relation to children’s anxiety-withdrawal (Hypothesis 1c), as we anticipated that most of its variance would be mainly explained by internal psychological control (see Hypothesis 2a, below). Drawing from the SDT’s notion of introjected regulation (Ryan & Deci,
2017), we reasoned that children whose parents would report more extensive use of internal psychological control would tend to comply with their parents’ requests, aiming to avoid guilt, garner parental approval, and maintain a close relationship with their parents, at the expense of suppressing their needs and thus internalizing any likely distress (Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
2010; Xu et al.,
2020). Therefore, we expected internal psychological control to relate positively to children’s anxiety-withdrawal (Hypothesis 2a), but not to social competence (Hypothesis 2b) or anger-aggression (Hypothesis 2c).
Considering the conflicting findings in the literature involving fathers in early childhood that we reviewed previously, we aimed to explore whether fathers’ internal and external psychological control would relate to children’s socio-emotional adjustment in the same ways as maternal variables do.
Discussion
The present study aimed to contribute to the limited, yet uprising, research drawing attention to the multidimensional nature of psychologically controlling parenting, with an understudied sample of preschoolers (Romm et al.,
2020; Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
2010). In line with the self-determination theory (Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
2010), internal (i.e., guilt induction) and external (i.e., constraining verbal expressions and erratic emotional behavior) forms of psychologically controlling parenting were uniquely related to children’s teacher-rated social-emotional adjustment in terms of social competence, anger-aggression, and anxiety-withdrawal. As hypothesized, controlling for child gender and parental education, external psychological control was associated with children’s social competence in a negative direction, and anger-aggression in a positive direction. In contrast, the link between external psychological control and children’s anxiety-withdrawal was not significant. Furthermore, in line with our hypotheses, internal psychological control did not show significant associations with children’s social competence or anger-aggression. However, contrary to our expectations, internal psychological control was negatively related to children’s anxiety-withdrawal. Fathers’ internal and external psychologically controlling practices related to children’s social-emotional adjustment in the same way as mothers’ practices did.
According to the self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci,
2017), both external and internal psychological control are detrimental as they undermine children’s need satisfaction, and hence well-being (Chen et al.,
2016; Ahmad et al.,
2013). However, it seems that external and internal types of parental psychological control may differentially relate to children’s socio-emotional functioning and the expression of behavior problems (Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
2010). The findings of the current study supported this argument. Therefore, despite the general tendency in the literature to assess psychological control as a uniform construct, keeping apart different dimensions of psychological control could be more informative, and can partly resolve the sometimes-mixed findings reported in the literature (Yu et al.,
2015; Soenens & Beyers,
2012).
External, or overt, psychological control reflects coercive parenting practices that limit children’s self-expression and convey parental negativity, such as losing one’s temper easily with the child or going back and forth between being warm and critical toward the child. From a social learning theory perspective (Bandura,
1973), we expected that children observing their externally controlling parents would model these behaviors and, hence be more likely to display outward-directed problems, rather than internalizing problems. In line with this reasoning, we found in our study that parents who used more external psychological control tactics had children who were rated by their teachers as displaying more anger-aggression, and as less socially skilled. Instead, parents’ use of external psychological control was not significantly associated with children’s anxiety-withdrawal. These findings are consistent with our hypotheses, the social learning theory (Bandura,
1973), and previous research which identified coercive parenting practices as a risk factor for children’s externalizing problems (see Hart et al.,
2003, for a review; Joussemet et al.,
2008; Scaramella et al.,
2008). Further, from a self-determination theory standpoint, individuals cope with need-thwarting contexts with compensatory and defensive reactions (Deci & Ryan,
2000; Vansteenkiste & Ryan,
2013), including oppositional defiance (Vansteenkiste & Ryan,
2013), which in turn may lead to externalizing problems. Previous research has documented the hypothesized mechanism with adolescents (Brenning et al.,
2019; Van Petegem et al.,
2015), and further investigation is encouraged to replicate these findings with early childhood samples.
Although our hypotheses are grounded primarily in the self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci,
2017), and the social learning theory (Bandura,
1973), alternative theoretical models may also offer explanations for the current relations. For instance, the tripartite model of familial influence (Morris et al.,
2007), which is also consistent with SDT’s assumption, presumes that parenting practices shape children’s development by affecting their emotion regulation (Ryan et al.,
2016). According to this line of reasoning, the present positive link between external psychological control and children’s anger-aggression may reflect children’s impoverished emotion regulation in the face of external psychological control (Cui et al.,
2014; Eisenberg et al.,
2001; Morris et al.,
2007).
The lack of a statistically significant relation between external psychological control and children’s anxiety-withdrawal was also in line with our expectations. Based on the social learning theory, we expected that children would model these behaviors in their relations, and would be more likely to act out, rather than internalize distress. It is also consistent with ample previous research demonstrating the less dominant role that coercive parenting has, compared to over-protective parenting, in children’s anxiety-withdrawal (Bayer et al.,
2006; see McLeod et al.,
2007 for a meta-analysis; Rubin et al.,
2009).
Internal psychological control (i.e., guilt induction) represents a more covert and insidious way of pressuring socialization (Soenens & Vansteenkiste,
2010). Drawing on the SDT’s behavioral regulation model (Assor et al.,
2004; Deci & Ryan,
2000), and inspired by Soenens and Vansteenkiste’s reasoning (
2010) that internally controlling parenting puts children at a greater risk of developing internalizing problems due to the inner conflict and emotional distress they experience, we anticipated internal psychological control not to relate to children’s social competence or anger-aggression. However, we expected a positive relationship with anxiety-withdrawal. As expected, internal psychological control was not associated with children’s social competence or anger-aggression. These findings are corroborated by past research conducted with European-American and Chinese children (Fung & Lau,
2012), which indicated that internal psychological control (measured as a combination of love withdrawal, guilt induction, and social comparison) was not significantly associated with children’s externalizing problems in either sample, after controlling for parental rejection. Specifically for guilt induction, the bivariate correlations were not significant either. Likewise, in a recent study, Levitt et al. (
2020) found that guilt induction did not have a significant link with children’s externalizing problems, while externally controlling parenting did.
However, contrary to our hypothesis, the more mothers and fathers admitted using internal psychological control, the less their children were rated on anxiety-withdrawal by their teachers. This finding contrasts the positive relation between guilt induction and preschoolers’ anxiety-withdrawal reported by Nelson et al. (
2006). One explanation for this counterintuitive association may concern the abovementioned compensatory and defensive reactions in response to need frustration (Vansteenkiste & Ryan,
2013). Children, who feel pressured to conform to parent-promoted behaviors to gain their approval, may increasingly turn to peers as a substitute for the approval they lack from their parents. This behavior may serve as an attempt to alleviate the need for frustration they experience. Perhaps this is the reason why the negative relation between fathers’ internal psychological control and anxiety-withdrawal was significant among fathers who reported low but not high external psychological control. If internal psychological control is accompanied by external psychological control, then perhaps any attempt to compensate for the lack of parental approval is canceled out due to external psychological pressure. Children turning to peers as substitutes is problematic in the sense that it would render them more vulnerable to adverse peer relations. Indeed, Soenens and colleagues (
2007) found a positive association between psychologically controlling parenting (albeit as a unidimensional construct) and adolescents’ deviant peer affiliations. Another explanation may be that anxious-withdrawn children may elicit less internal psychological control from their parents. Specifically, parents of children who are already highly anxious and withdrawn may feel less urge to use strategies such as guilt induction with them.
On the other hand, this unexpected association between internal psychological control and children’s decreased anxiety-withdrawal can be interpreted from a culture-specific perspective as well. According to this perspective, such parenting practices are normative and consistent with the culturally valued socialization goals (e.g., group harmony and relatedness) that prevail in collectivistic cultures (Yu et al.,
2019). In these cultures, internal psychological control practices seem to reflect parental care rather than parental negativity (Fung & Lau,
2012). Indeed, Kara and Sumer (
2022) found positive correlations between parental internal psychological control and warmth in a Turkish sample, which somehow complements previous findings showing that internal psychological control is more strongly related to external psychological control in individualistic cultures (such as the US) than collectivistic ones (such as India) (Rudy et al.,
2014). So, could parental warmth buffer the detrimental effects of internal psychological control in collectivistic cultures? Future research should investigate this possibility with cross-cultural experimental designs.
In the case of fathers, our findings indicated that both internal and external forms of paternal psychological control related to children’s social-emotional adjustment, in terms of social competence, anger-aggression, and anxiety-withdrawal, in the same ways as maternal forms did. These findings are parallel with previous research conducted with adolescents (Arim and Shapka,
2008; Lansford et al.,
2014), which may imply the increased involvement of fathers with their children. This idea is supported by the fact that the current sample mostly consisted of highly educated parents (Sen et al.,
2014). These findings extend previous psychological control research with early childhood samples, which has widely portrayed mothers as the main source of influence in children’s parental socialization (e.g., Aunola & Nurmi,
2005; Xing et al.,
2017).
Limitations and Implications
The findings of this study are suggestive yet limited in several ways. First of all, the present research is cross-sectional, so the direction and causality of the observed relations remain unclear. Longitudinal research addressing the directionality of relations between parental psychological control and adolescent outcomes has provided evidence for reciprocal relations (e.g., Janssens et al.,
2017; Soenens et al.,
2008). Such findings imply that just as parents’ use of external psychological control can lead to children being less socially skilled and more prone to anger-aggression, children may also elicit more external psychological control practices from their parents. Future research should aim for longitudinal investigations to clarify the direction of relations between different psychological control forms and children’s functioning with young children. Further, considering the low mean scores of parenting variables in this study, a spouse-report method, that is, parents reporting on each other’s parenting (Yang et al.,
2004), or age-appropriate child-reports of parenting (e.g., Morris et al.,
2002) can be recommended for future researchers to tackle social desirability issue. Another consideration is that the present study did not include further dimensions of parenting and did not consider (because of unavailable information) the shared variance of teachers’ reports (given that each teacher-rated multiple children). Previous research has demonstrated the moderating roles of other parenting dimensions on the effects of psychological control (Aunola & Nurmi,
2005; Caron et al.,
2006). Investigating psychological control while taking other parenting dimensions and any nested effects into consideration can provide a more comprehensive perspective to understand the interplay between parenting practices and child outcomes.
On the other hand, an important strength of the current study is the use of different informants. In this way, response bias (e.g., parents’ possible desire to present their child in a specific way), and response sets (e.g., the informants’ proneness to rate generally high, or low, on Likert scales) problems, hence associated inflated correlation risk is tackled (Campbell & Fiske,
1959; Podsakoff et al.,
2012). Apart from this, teachers are valuable sources to get information about children’s social-emotional competence as they have the opportunity to observe children under varying circumstances (LaFreniere & Dumas,
1996; Morris et al.,
2002). Notably, the present study included both fathers and mothers while focusing on the preschool period. We found consistent patterns in the significance of the relations between maternal and paternal variables and child outcomes. Our results support previous research involving adolescents (Lansford et al.,
2014; Yang et al.,
2004) and reinforce the idea of fathers’ increased involvement in childcare. This underscores the importance of incorporating fathers in future research and targeting fathers in interventions related to child socio-emotional adjustment.
Another potentially noteworthy aspect of this research is that we distinguished between external and internal psychological control and explored their unique relations to young children’s social-emotional functioning. Our findings provide support for the differential value of psychological control types in predicting child outcomes. This offers implications for research and practice. More specifically, the present findings highlight the complexity of parental psychological control and the need to explore the unique relations between psychological control subtypes and child outcomes. The findings also point to the importance of addressing psychological control forms at an early age to lessen their adverse effects on later development.
In conclusion, our study contributes to the growing body of research on psychologically controlling parenting, particularly in the context of preschool-aged children. By distinguishing between internal and external psychological control, our findings enrich our understanding of the differential relations of these practices with children’s social-emotional adjustment. These insights can inform future research and interventions aimed at promoting healthier parent-child relationships.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.