Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development 5/2017

Open Access 09-01-2017 | Original Article

Being on Your Own or Feeling Lonely? Loneliness and Other Social Variables in Youths with Autism Spectrum Disorders

Auteurs: Anne Deckers, Peter Muris, Jeffrey Roelofs

Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development | Uitgave 5/2017

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

A cross-sectional study was conducted to examine loneliness and its correlates in children (7 to 11 years) and adolescents (12 to 18 years) with autism spectrum disorders (ASD, n = 73) and control groups of clinically referred (ADHD, n = 76) and non-clinical (n = 106) youths. Youths completed questionnaires on loneliness and desire for social interaction, while parents and teachers filled out scales on other aspects of children’s social functioning. Results indicated that only at an adolescent age, the ASD group reported higher levels of loneliness than the control groups. Further, the ASD group generally expressed relatively low levels of desire for social interaction, although these youths displayed a similar increase in the wish to belong during adolescence as participants in the control groups. Finally, the ASD group exhibited lower levels of social competence and social skills and higher levels of social problems and social anxiety than the control groups, and in all groups these social variables correlated in a theoretically meaningful with loneliness.

Introduction

The need to belong is an essential characteristic of human beings and a strong motive guiding our cognitions, emotions, and behavior [1]. Although from an evolutionary perspective the importance of belonging nowadays might be less crucial for survival and reproduction as compared to our ancestors, it is still thought to be a critical factor determining people’s emotional well-being [2]. When the need to belong is not satisfactorily met, feelings of loneliness may arise [3]. Loneliness is a subjective and negative experience [4] that derives from a discrepancy between the interpersonal relationships people desire and the quantity and quality of these relationships that are actually perceived [5].
Loneliness involves intra-individual characteristics like self-esteem and shyness as well as inter-individual experiences referring to positive and negative peer interactions varying from social acceptance and friendship to bullying and victimization [6]. Loneliness appears to be the result of a complex interplay between a person’s desires, social abilities, perceptions, and interpretations, and social interactions and thus reciprocal processes with others. While it is perfectly normal to feel lonely every now and then, it is also clear that persistent and increased feelings of loneliness have to be considered as clinically relevant. This is supported by research findings demonstrating that loneliness is associated with mental health problems, like anxiety disorders and depression, and even physical complaints and diseases [3].
Loneliness is a prevalent phenomenon during adolescence and can be considered a common experience in this developmental phase [3]. Adolescents might be more susceptible to feelings of loneliness for several reasons. During adolescence youths tend to shift from family to peers as their most important companions. While they become more individuated and autonomous towards their parents and involvement in the peer group becomes more important, they also have to handle their pubertal maturation and their identity formation process [7]. Social interactions become more complicated, and the awareness of social competence of the self and others increases. During this transition, it might be quite difficult to fulfil their expectations about social relationships and to satisfy their need to belong resulting in loneliness at some point [3]. For youngsters with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) it might be particularly difficult to keep up with their peers and to be involved with and accepted by them.
Deficits in communication and problems with social interaction are core characteristics of children and adolescents with ASD [8, 9]. For example, youth with ASD have less social skills [10], are more often victim of bullying [11], experience poorer friendship quality [12], spend less time interacting with others [13], and have a less central position in social networks [14, 15]. Given these social difficulties, one might expect that children and adolescents with ASD are at increased risk for experiencing feelings of loneliness. Several studies have shown that children and adolescents with ASD indeed report higher levels of loneliness relative to their non-clinical peers [14, 1620]. However, Chamberlain and colleagues [21] did not find elevated levels of loneliness for younger children with ASD as compared to typically developing control children. It has been suggested that the experience of loneliness in youth with ASD might be age-dependent and becomes more pronounced during adolescence [19]. In fact, previous studies that documented elevated levels of loneliness in relation to ASD mainly relied on samples that predominantly included adolescents [14, 1820]. Besides that, the sample sizes of these studies were often relatively small, and so in studies that did include younger children it was not possible to explore the experience of loneliness in youths with ASD of various ages [16, 17].
Meanwhile, some scholars have suggested that youths with ASD have less desire for social interaction [22] or a stronger wish to be on their own [23], and as such children and adolescents with ASD might be less prone to develop feelings of loneliness. However, it should be noted that a stronger desire for aloneness and loneliness are not mutually exclusive [3, 24]. In addition, previous research assessing the desire for social interaction in 7- to 12-year-old children with ASD demonstrated that these youngsters did show a desire for social interaction on an implicit level (as measured by means of an experimental approach-avoidance task), while they expressed less desire for social interaction on an explicit level (as assessed with a questionnaire) as compared to typically developing children [25]. It might be that social deficits hinder youths with ASD to openly express their desire for social interaction. That is, it is possible that they do experience a desire for social interaction or a need to belong, but lack the skills and/or need specific circumstances in order to be able to translate this wish into action.
It is unclear whether increased levels of loneliness are specific for ASD or are more generally related to developmental disorders or psychopathology in youths [19]. As mentioned before, loneliness is related to different types of psychopathology like depression and anxiety, but the direction of causality is still ambiguous and might also be bi-directional [3, 26]. However, for other types of psychopathology loneliness is more likely to be a consequence rather than cause of the symptomatology. For example, it could be possible that youths with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) experience higher levels of loneliness, but loneliness alone is unlikely to cause ADHD. This makes youth with ADHD an interesting clinical comparison group, although it should be admitted that the studies conducted so far have yielded quite mixed results. Heiman [27] documented increased levels of parent- and teacher-rated loneliness, but not on self-reported loneliness when comparing an ADHD group with a non-clinical control group. However, in a more recent study, Houghton and colleagues [28] found comparable levels of loneliness in children and adolescents with ADHD and their typically developing counterparts.
In the present study we took a closer look at the phenomenon of loneliness in youths with ASD, adopting a multi-informant approach using assessments of children themselves, parents, and teachers. Three groups were included in this investigation: an ASD group, a clinical control group of youths with ADHD, and a typically developing control group. Each group consisted of children (i.e., age 7–11 years) as well as adolescents (i.e., age 12–18 years). It was hypothesized that (1) the ASD group would show increased levels of loneliness as compared to the typically developing and the clinical control group, and (2) adolescents would display higher levels of loneliness than children. In addition to loneliness, we also measured youths’ desire for social interaction, social skills, social problems, social competence, and social anxiety, which enabled us to explore their relation with loneliness. For these other social variables, we also made group comparisons. Here we expected to find (3) lower levels of desire for social interaction, lower levels of social skills and social competence, and higher levels of social problems and social anxiety in the ASD group as compared to both control groups. Our predictions regarding differences between the clinical and non-clinical control groups were less clear, but on the basis of what has been found in the literature it might be expected that children and adolescents with ADHD would show lower levels of social skills and social competence, and higher levels of social problems than their typically developing counterparts [2931]. Finally, the relations between loneliness and the other social variables were explored. Here it was hypothesized that (4) loneliness would correlate positively with desire for social interaction, social problems, and social anxiety, and negatively with social skills and social competence.

Method

Design and Procedure

A cross-sectional research design was applied using multiple informants. Children (7–11 years) and adolescents (12–18 years) with ASD were compared to clinical and non-clinical control groups of children and adolescents. The participants in the typical control group were recruited from regular primary and secondary schools in the Netherlands. The participants in the ASD group and the clinical control group were recruited at the community mental health center (Virenze-RIAGG Maastricht, The Netherlands). For the ASD group, the main inclusion criterion was a formal diagnosis of Autistic Disorder, Asperger’s Disorder, or Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS) in terms of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition, text revision (DSM-IV-TR) [8]. For the clinical control group, a formal diagnosis of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder was required, whereas suspicion of autism spectrum problems was an exclusion criterion. For both the ASD and clinical control group the presence of comorbid (psychiatric) diagnoses was allowed. The clinical diagnosis was based on an extensive diagnostic procedure according to the longitudinal-expert-all data (LEAD) principle [32]. That is, the diagnoses were made by a multidisciplinary team including licensed psychiatrists and psychologists, using information from multiple sources (i.e., interviews with the child, parents and teacher, psychiatric examination, psychological assessment, and clinical observations) during a longitudinal diagnostic and/or treatment process. Children with severe cognitive (i.e., estimated IQ <70) or language impairments were not included in the study. Parents and teachers of the children and adolescents in all three groups were also invited to participate. The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of Psychology at Maastricht University and participation only occurred after written informed consent was given. Parents gave informed consent for their own participation in the research and for the participation of their child. All children were informed about the research and children aged 12 years and older also had to sign an informed consent form. Teachers were only invited to participate after parents had given written permission for an assessment via this informant. If this was the case, teachers received information about the study and eventually gave consent for their contribution.

Participants

The total sample consisted of 255 children and adolescents: 73 in the ASD group (47 children and 26 adolescents), 76 in the clinical control (ADHD) group (36 children and 40 adolescents), and 106 in the non-clinical control group (54 children and 52 adolescents). Complete assessments of the child via all three informants were conducted in 66% of the cases. In other cases, only parent- and self-report (24%), teacher- and self-report (4%), or self-report (3%) measurements were carried out. Table 1 shows descriptive characteristics for each of the three groups. In accordance with epidemiological studies, skewed gender differences were found in both clinical groups [33, 34]. Thus, in the ASD and the clinical control groups, there were clearly more boys than girls, whereas in the non-clinical group both genders were more equally present [χ2(2, 255) = 14.42, p = .001]. The mean age of the total sample was 11.6 years (SD 2.55) and age did not differ among the three groups [F(2,255) = 1.00, p = .37]. The sample consisted predominantly of Caucasian participants. In the ASD group, about 73% of the participants attended regular education, whereas 27% were in special schools. In the clinical control group, 89% of the children and adolescents followed regular education and 11% were in special education.
Table 1
Descriptive characteristics (gender and age distributions) of the total sample and each of the three groups
Descriptive characteristics
Total sample
(N = 255)
ASD group
(n = 73)
Clinical control
(n = 76)
Non-clinical control
(n = 106)
Male/female (numbers)
 Total
178/77
62/11
54/22
62/44
 Child
104/33
42/5
26/10
36/18
 Adolescent
74/44
20/6
28/12
26/26
Age (M, SD)
 Total
11.55 (2.53)
11.22 (2.42)
11.79 (2.48)
11.61 (2.63)
 Child
9.56 (1.17)
9.81 (1.06)
9.61 (1.13)
9.31 (1.26)
 Adolescent
13.86 (1.50)
13.77 (2.07)
13.75 (1.55)
14.00 (1.10)
In the child ASD group, 63.8% was diagnosed with PDD-NOS, 25.5% with Asperger’s Disorder, and 8.5% with Autistic Disorder. In the adolescent ASD group, the distribution was comparable: 69.2% was diagnosed with PDD-NOS, 19.2% with Asperger’s Disorder, and 11.5% with Autistic Disorder. The majority of the child clinical control group was diagnosed with ADHD of the combined subtype (81%), while the other children had ADHD of the inattentive subtype (19%). In the adolescent clinical control group, 75% was diagnosed with ADHD of the combined subtype, 22.5% with the inattentive subtype, and 2.5% with the hyperactive-impulsive subtype. In both clinical groups, comorbidity rates were equally high [χ2(1, 149) <1] (see Table 2). In the ASD group, 55% of the youths were diagnosed with at least one comorbid diagnosis, while 23% even met the criteria for two or more comorbid conditions. In the clinical control group, these figures were respectively 62% and 33%.
Table 2
Overview of the frequencies (percentages) of comorbid diagnoses in the two clinical groups
Comorbid clinical diagnosis
ASD group
(n = 73)
Clinical control
(n = 76)
No comorbid diagnosis
33 (45.2)
29 (38.2)
Adjustment disorders
1 (1.4)
4 (5.3)
ADHD
17 (23.3)
*
Anxiety disorders
9 (12.3)
5 (6.6)
Disruptive behavior disorders
0 (0.0)
6 (7.9)
Eating disorders
2 (2.7)
0 (0.0)
Identity/personality problems
0 (0.0)
3 (3.9)
Learning disorders
4 (5.5)
18 (23.7)
Mood disorders
1 (1.4)
4 (5.3)
Relational problems
23 (31.5)
34 (44.7)
Somatoform disorders
0 (0.0)
2 (2.6)
Other disorder or diagnosis deferred
2 (2.7)
9 (11.8)
*All youths in the clinical control group had a diagnosis of ADHD

Questionnaires

The Louvain scale of Loneliness and Aloneness in Children and Adolescents (LACA; [35]) consists of four subscales: Loneliness in the relationships with parents, loneliness in the relationships with peers, aversion to aloneness, and affinity for aloneness. In the present study, only the loneliness in the relationships with peers subscale was used and regarded as the primary measure. This subscale contains 12 items (e.g., “I think I have fewer friends than others”) for which participants indicate how often the item applies to them using a Likert scale ranging from never (1) to often (4). A total score can be calculated ranging between 12 and 48, with higher scores indicating higher levels of loneliness in the relationships with peers. The internal consistency of the LACA has been shown to be high and its validity is satisfactory in large samples of elementary-school children and adolescents [36, 37]. The Cronbach’s alpha of the LACA subscale that was employed in the current study was also good (α = 0.88).
The desire for social contact with others was assessed by means of the Wish for Social Interaction Scale (WSIS; [25]). The WSIS contains 64 items referring to possibly conducting future social activities with unfamiliar individuals. Participants are asked whether they want to engage in 8 different social activities with 8 unknown persons by asking questions such as “Would you like to have a little chat with this person?” or “Would you like to play with this person?” Each item is accompanied by a side-view picture of the face of an unknown person displayed on a computer screen. Each question has to be answered on a yes/no format, and thus a total score can be calculated by summing the number of positive responses (range 0–64). The child and adolescent participants in the present study received similar questions. The pictures of the (male and female) adult faces were the same for both groups, but the pictures of the peers were adjusted for each age group and thus different. The Cronbach’s alpha of the WSIS was found to be excellent in the current study (α = 0.93) which is comparable to what was found in our previous study [25]. Further, pilot data collected in our own lab show that youths’ WSIS scores are substantially correlated (r = .56) with the Desire for Future Interaction Scale (DFI; [38]), which provides support for the validity of the scale.
The level of social skills was assessed using the Social Skills Observation (SSO; [39]), which was completed by both parents and teachers. The SSO asks parents and teachers to evaluate the behavior of the children and adolescents in various types of social situations during the past week: greeting, conversation, and play. Respondents have to indicate whether or not the child or adolescent engaged in these types of social interactions with other persons. In case that this is indeed the case, additional questions are asked pertaining to the specific social skills displayed by the child/adolescent (e.g., “Did he/she make eye contact?”; 36 items). Subsequently, 7 items about more general social skills are asked (e.g., “Did he/she remain at an appropriate distance from the other person?”). The SSO was slightly adapted for the use in the adolescent participants. That is, the items referring to “play” were altered in “joint activity”. A total score reflecting the level of social skills can be calculated by summing the “yes” responses. In the current study, the internal consistency of both the parent (α = 0.93) and teacher (α = 0.92) version of the SSO was excellent. The correlation between the parent and teacher version of the SSO was moderate (r = .26), but nevertheless supports the validity of the scale.
The Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment [40] was used to assess the level of social problems and social competence. The Social Problems and Social Competence scales of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) were filled out by the parents, while teachers completed the Social Problems subscale of the Teacher Report Form (TRF). The Social Problems subscale of both the CBCL and the TRF consists of 11 items which have to be rated on a 3-point scale (0 = not true, 1 = somewhat or sometimes true, and 2 = very true or often true). One of the items pertains to “Complains of loneliness”, and in order to prevent problems of shared variance, this item was discarded when computing a total score for the Social Problems subscale of the CBCL and TRF. The internal consistency coefficients of the parent (α = 0.83) and teacher (α = 0.75) versions of the Social Problems subscale were satisfactory in the current study. The Social Competence scale measures the involvement and contribution of the child in sporting clubs and other recreational activities, the number of friends and other social contacts, and the quality of the interpersonal behavior. The Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment is a reliable, valid, and widely used scale to assess psychopathological symptoms in children and adolescents in the general population as well as in clinical samples [40, 41]. The psychometric properties of the CBCL are also well-established in children and adolescents with ASD [e.g., 42].
In order to measure the level of social anxiety, parents completed a subscale of the Screen for Child Anxiety and Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED-71; [43]). Parents rated how often (0 = almost never, 1 = sometimes, and 2 = often) their child experienced social anxiety symptoms (9 items; e.g. “I don’t like to be with people I don’t know well”). According to previous research conducted in non-clinical and clinically referred youths including children and adolescents with ASD, the reliability and validity of the parent version of the SCARED-71 are convincing, and this also applies to the social anxiety subscale [4345]. In the present study the Cronbach’s alpha was also good (α = 0.89).

Analyses

In order to evaluate differences across various diagnostic and age groups, 3 (ASD vs. clinical control vs. non-clinical control) × 2 (children vs. adolescents) multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs) were conducted on the data of each informant separately (i.e., children/adolescents, parents, and teachers). Given the unequal gender distributions among the three groups, we decided to include this variable as a covariate in the analyses (i.e., MANCOVAs). The primary analysis was the MANCOVA involving youths’ self-report of loneliness on the LACA, which also included the WSIS desire for social contact as dependent variable. The MANCOVA performed on the parent data included SSO social skills, SCARED social anxiety, and the CBCL social problems and social competence scales as dependent variables. The MANCOVA carried out on the teacher data included TRF social problems and SSO social skills as the dependent variables. As the analyses were carried out separately for each informant, the N’s differed from one MANCOVA to another. Furthermore, cases were only included in the MANCOVA when all measures of the pertinent informant were available. Percentages of missing data were 1% for the self-report, 10% for the parent-report, and 27% for the teacher-report scales.
In order to explore the relations between loneliness and the other social variables, partial correlations controlling for gender were calculated for the total sample as well as for the three diagnostic groups separately.

Results

Group Comparisons

Children and Adolescents’ Self-Report

The 3 (diagnostic group: ASD vs. clinical control vs. non-clinical control) × 2 (age group: children vs. adolescents) MANCOVA on self-reported loneliness and desire for social interaction revealed statistically significant main effects of diagnostic group [F(4,492) = 14.01, p < .001, η2 = 0.010], age group [F(2,245) = 27.21, p < .001, η2 = 0.108], and an interaction effect of diagnostic group and age group [F(4,492) = 3.10, p < .05, η2 = 0.030]. Follow-up ANCOVAs showed that the main effects were significant for both the LACA and the WSIS, whereas the interaction effect only attained significance in the case of the LACA (see Table 3) Post-hoc analyses indicated that in general the ASD group displayed higher levels of loneliness as compared to the clinical and non-clinical control groups, while the clinical and non-clinical control group did not statistically differ from each other. Further, in contrast with our hypothesis, children reported significantly higher levels of loneliness than adolescents, although this was only true for the clinical and non-clinical control groups. In the ASD group, no significant difference in loneliness between both age groups was found, although adolescents scored slightly higher than children (see Fig. 1panel a). Note that within the child age group, no statistical differences in loneliness between the three diagnostic groups were noted. However, within the adolescent age group, the three diagnostic groups did significantly differ: the ASD group clearly displayed the highest levels of loneliness, followed by the non-clinical control group and the clinical control group, which exhibited the lowest scores.
Table 3
Mean scores (standard deviations) on loneliness and other social variables split by diagnostic and age groups, and results of the follow-up ANCOVAs
Variable
Age group
ASD group
Clinical control
Non-clinical control
Diagnostic group
Age group
Interaction
Self-report
    
F(2,253)
F(1,253)
F(2,253)
 LACA loneliness
Child
21.77 (7.98)a
19.54 (7.98)a
20.32 (6.14)a
12.84***
3.98*
3.77*
Adolescent
23.50 (7.04)a
15.48 (3.35)b
18.12 (4.58)c
   
 WSIS desire for interaction
Child
14.15 (10.89)a
18.57 (11.49)a
27.76 (11.65)b
16.82***
51.52***
2.43
Adolescent
26.04 (12.31)c
32.00 (11.87)d
34.71 (9.51)d
   
Parent-report
    
F(2,229)
F(1,229)
F(2,229)
 SSO social skills
Child
19.01 (9.46)a
26.50 (8.76)b
32.14 (8.12)c
24.71***
0.35
1.74
Adolescent
19.41 (10.31)a
27.76 (9.90)b
28.46 (8.66)b
   
 CBCL social problems
Child
7.94 (4.28)a
4.42 (3.55)b
4.10 (3.92)b
29.25***
2.89
1.31
Adolescent
7.55 (3.41)a
4.24 (3.63)b
2.28 (2.05)c
   
 CBCL social competence
Child
6.28 (2.04)a
7.71 (1.74)b
8.03 (1.90)b
17.33***
0.74
0.65
Adolescent
5.77 (2.10)a
7.40 (2.22)b
8.29 (2.17)b
   
 SCARED social anxiety
Child
8.50 (5.20)a
4.17 (3.65)b
4.06 (3.61)b
23.26***
0.73
0.10
Adolescent
7.77 (5.00)a
3.95 (4.16)b
3.85 (2.79)b
   
Teacher-report
    
F(2,185)
F(1,185)
F(2,185)
 SSO social skills
Child
18.51 (7.75)a
26.34 (9.23)b
28.76 (9.35)b
15.27***
0.07
0.56
Adolescent
19.61 (8.85)a
23.87 (10.54)ab
29.59 (8.86)b
   
 TRF social problems
Child
4.83 (3.28)a
3.83 (2.78)ab
2.92 (3.03)b
13.60***
4.95*
1.65
Adolescent
4.83 (3.11)a
2.74 (2.07)b
1.00 (1.91)c
   
LACA loneliness and aloneness in children and adolescents, WSIS wish for social interaction scale, SSO social skills observation, CBCL child behavior checklist, SCARED screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders, TRF teacher report form. For each variable, within-row and within-column means not sharing similar subscript letters are significantly different at p < .05
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001
As can be seen in Table 3 and Fig. 1 (panel B), the ASD group exhibited lower levels of desire for social interaction than the non-clinical control group, whereas the clinical control group scored in between. Further, in all groups, adolescents had statistically higher levels of desire for social interaction as compared to children.

Parent Report

The 3 (diagnostic group) × 2 (age group) MANCOVA only revealed a significant main effect of diagnostic group [F(8,440) = 10.95, p < .001, η2 = 0.017]. As expected, the follow-up ANCOVAs indicated that this was mainly due to the ASD group which deviated on all measures from the two control groups. More precisely, regardless of age, the ASD group scored higher on social problems and social anxiety but lower on social skills and social competence than the clinical and non-clinical control groups (see Table 3and Fig. 1 panels c–f).

Teacher Report

The MANCOVA performed on the teacher-report data only revealed a statistically main effect of diagnostic group [F(4,356) = 10.15, p < .001, η2 = 0.010] and a borderline significant effect of age group [F(2,177) = 2.86, p = .06, η2 = 0.003]. Follow-up ANCOVAs revealed that the main effect of diagnostic group showed itself for both teacher-rated social skills and social problems. Post-hoc analyses indicated that teachers provided lower ratings of social skills for children and adolescents in the ASD group than for youths in the clinical and non-clinical control groups (see Table 3panel g). With regard to the teacher-rated social problems, it was found that the ASD group displayed the highest scores, followed by the clinical control group, and the lowest scores for the non-clinical control group. Note further that in general teachers reported lower levels of social problems for adolescents than for children.

Correlations

Table 4 shows the correlations between loneliness and the other social variables for the total sample and for the three diagnostic groups separately. Within the total sample, loneliness correlated with all other variables. Surprisingly, a negative correlation was found between loneliness and the desire for social interaction, indicating that youth with higher levels of loneliness expressed less desire for social interaction. Further, loneliness was positively associated with parent and teacher ratings of social problems and parent ratings of social anxiety, whereas negative links were noted with parent and teacher ratings of social skills and competence. Other correlations in the total sample were as predicted and suggested that higher levels of social skills and competence were associated with a stronger desire for social interaction, and lower levels of social problems and social anxiety. Note that parent- and teacher-rated social skills and social problems correlated significantly suggesting a convincing interrater agreement.
Table 4
Partial correlations (corrected for gender) between loneliness and the other social variables as computed for the total group and the three diagnostic groups separately
 
Self-report
Parent-rated
Teacher-rated
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
1. LACA loneliness
        
2. WSIS desire for social interaction
        
 Total sample
−0.16*
       
 ASD group
0.06
       
 Clinical control
−0.13
       
 Non-clinical control
−0.23*
       
3. SSO social Skills
        
 Total sample
−0.21**
0.17**
      
 ASD group
−0.04
−0.07
      
 Clinical control
−0.24*
0.06
      
 Non-clinical control
−0.13
0.12
      
4. CBCL social problems
        
 Total sample
0.38***
−0.17*
−0.39***
     
 ASD group
0.13
−0.11
−0.12
     
 Clinical control
0.35**
−0.13
−0.37**
     
 Non-clinical control
0.47***
0.18
−0.22*
     
5. CBCL social competence
        
 Total sample
−0.24***
0.14*
0.31***
−0.43***
    
 ASD group
−0.25*
0.02
0.14
−0.31*
    
 Clinical control
−0.03
−0.10
0.22
−0.37**
    
 Non-clinical control
−0.25*
0.14
0.18
−0.22*
    
6. SCARED social anxiety
        
 Total sample
0.29***
−0.18**
−0.52***
0.42***
−0.30***
   
 ASD group
0.16
−0.09
−0.47***
0.18
0.04
   
 Clinical control
0.29*
0.02
−0.46***
0.44***
−0.26*
   
 Non-clinical control
0.21
−0.12
−0.12
0.24*
−0.27*
   
7. SSO social skills
        
Total sample
−0.19**
0.21**
0.26**
−0.25**
0.19**
−0.24**
  
 ASD group
−0.02
0.15
0.14
−0.05
0.01
−0.22
  
 Clinical control
−0.06
−0.11
−0.02
0.02
0.15
−0.13
  
 Non-clinical control
−0.21
−0.21
0.07
−0.15
−0.06
0.14
  
8. TRF SOCIAL problems
        
 Total sample
0.29***
−0.18*
−0.12
0.47***
−0.28***
0.10
−0.35***
 
 ASD group
0.14
0.04
0.21
0.47**
−0.27
−0.10
0.08
 
 Clinical control
0.21
−0.15
−0.12
0.19
0.03
−0.00
−0.35*
 
 Non-clinical control
0.37*
−0.15
0.05
0.44***
−0.20
−0.04
−0.37**
 
LACA loneliness and aloneness in children and adolescents, WSIS wish for social interaction scale, SSO social skills observation, CBCL child behavior checklist, SCARED screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders, TRF teacher report form. Correlations are based on variable numbers of participants: self- and parent-report scales, n = 227, self- and teacher-report measures, n = 184, and parent- and teacher-report questionnaires, n = 169
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001
The correlations computed for each diagnostic group separately were conducted on smaller numbers of participants and thus obviously subject to power problems. Nevertheless, loneliness showed some anticipated links with the other social variables (see Table 4). In the ASD group, loneliness correlated negatively with parent-rated social competence. Further, in the clinical control group, loneliness was positively related to parent-rated social problems and social anxiety and negatively associated with parent-rated social skills. Finally, in the non-clinical control group, loneliness correlated positively with parent- and teacher-rated social problems and negatively with self-rated desire for social interaction and parent-rated social competence.

Discussion

In the present study, the phenomenon of loneliness in youths with ASD was examined in more detail. Children and adolescents with ASD completed a self-report questionnaire for measuring this construct and their scores were compared with those of a clinical control group of youths with ADHD and a non-clinical control group of typically developing children and adolescents. In line with the first hypothesis, the ASD group overall reported elevated levels of loneliness as compared to the clinical and non-clinical control groups, but this was mainly true for youths in adolescent age range. In fact, no significant differences between the three diagnostic groups were found in the younger participants. These findings are well in line with previous studies showing that youths with ASD are indeed more prone to feelings of loneliness and that, in comparison with clinical and non-clinical control groups, this problem becomes really manifest during adolescence [14, 1621].
In contrast to what has been suggested in the loneliness literature (hypothesis 2; [3, 7]), the results of our study demonstrate that adolescents in general displayed lower levels of loneliness than children. One explanation for this unexpected finding is concerned with the fact that loneliness appears to be a multidimensional construct and that various aspects of loneliness may manifest themselves during different developmental stages [3]. In relation to this point, it should be kept in mind that in the present study only one specific dimension of loneliness, namely peer-related loneliness. Maes and colleagues [36] also noted that that this specific aspect of loneliness decreased from childhood to adolescence, whereas other dimensions of loneliness (i.e., parent-related loneliness) increased as youths became older. Another possibility is that children complete a measure like the LACA with a somewhat different perspective than adolescents, resulting in differential loneliness scores for both age groups. Finally, it is also possible that most adolescents are capable of successfully taking the developmental hurdle of connecting to the peer group [46]. It should be borne in mind, however, that most studies on loneliness including the present investigation are cross-sectional in nature, and that more prospective research is necessary to test these possibilities. Interestingly, there is a recent longitudinal trajectory study in non-clinical youths (N = 586) by Qualter and colleagues [47] which shows that there are more young people who exhibit stable low (37%) or declining (23%) levels of loneliness for the interval between 7 and 17 years, whereas a minority of 22% and 18% display respectively stable high or increasing levels. It would be interesting to include youths with ASD in such a study and learn more about the developmental pathway of loneliness in youth with this developmental disorder.
In line with our previous study [25], the ASD group exhibited lower levels of self-reported desire for social interaction as compared to the non-clinical control group, while the clinical control group scored in between. Overall, there was a significant effect of age group for this variable, which reflects that adolescents indeed have a stronger wish for social interaction than children [7]. Interestingly, this age effect was not only observable in the clinical and non-clinical control groups, but also in the ASD group, which suggests that even these youngsters have a stronger “wish to belong” during the developmental stage of adolescence.
In keeping with hypothesis 3, statistically significant group differences were found with regard to the parent- and teacher-rated social variables. As expected, the ASD group displayed lower levels of social skills and social competence but higher levels of social problems and social anxiety as compared to the clinical and non-clinical control groups, which is hardly surprising given that deficits in social functioning are one of the defining features of autism spectrum problems [8, 9]. On some of these social variables, the clinical control group also deviated significantly from the non-clinical control group. For example, according to the parents, children and adolescents with ADHD displayed lower levels of social skills and higher levels of social problems than typically developing youths. These findings are in line with previous research demonstrating that ADHD is also to some extent associated with social difficulties [2931, 48, 49].
The correlational analysis generally showed that the expected links between loneliness and other social variables. That is, negative correlations were found with social skills and social competence, while positive correlations emerged with social problems and social anxiety. These results are well in line with previous research showing that high levels of loneliness are associated with poor social skills, weak social competence [3, 50], and high levels of social anxiety and other social problems [51]. Most of these relationships are thought to be bi-directional in nature, indicating that on the one hand loneliness may be the result of poor social functioning, but that one the other hand loneliness is likely to promote social difficulties [3]. To gain more insight in the role of loneliness in social functioning, especially in youths with ASD, it might be worthwhile to employ the experience sampling method (ESM). ESM repeatedly asks participants to record feelings, cognitions, and behaviors within the context of their daily life, and represents a very informative way to study psychopathology and related phenomena [52].
The correlation between loneliness and the desire for social interaction was negative, which is on first sight surprising as one might expect loneliness to be coupled with a stronger wish to interact with other people. This seems to indicate that the relation between loneliness and desire for social interaction is more complex and probably subject to moderating variables. For example, it may well be that when combined with positive affect, feelings of loneliness may fuel the wish for social interaction, whereas feelings of loneliness which are associated with negative feelings (dysphoria, shyness, or social anxiety) may undermine the desire for social interaction and prompt withdrawal behavior. Other factors possibly involved are the chronicity of loneliness, victimization, children’s self-esteem, and the level of social skills [3, 6].
As noted earlier, one drawback of this study was the cross-sectional design, which not only prevented the investigation of developmental trends but also hindered us in testing causal relationships between loneliness and other social variables. The role of loneliness in the social and emotional well-being of youths with ASD is likely to be rather complex, and requires a more elaborated, longitudinal investigation that should also include other relevant variables such as depression [53]. A further limitation of the present study was the lack of standardized instruments for diagnosing ASD (e.g., Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised; [54]), ADHD, or comorbid psychopathology (e.g., Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Childhood Disorders; [55]) in the clinical groups. Note in passing that almost one quarter (i.e., 23.3%) of the children and adolescents in the ASD group also had a diagnosis of ADHD, which of course somewhat obscures the comparison with the clinical control group that consisted of youths who had a primary diagnosis of ADHD (but no ASD). The coexistence ASD and ADHD is a common phenomenon [56], and in spite of the fact that we did not control for this diagnostic overlap, we did document significant differences in loneliness and other social variables between these two groups, which of course suggests that the presence of ASD was the determining factor. Another shortcoming pertains to the fact that DSM-IV-TR diagnoses were still used in the present study [8]. Since the majority of the youth in the ASD group were diagnosed with PDD-NOS, it might be that not all participants of the ASD group would fulfill the DSM-5 (Diagnostic Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition; [9]) criteria for ASD [57, 58]. Our ASD group can best be considered as a high-functioning ASD group with mild to moderate ASD symptoms in terms of the DSM-5. A final demerit has to do with the unequal gender distribution across the three groups: the ASD group and the clinical control group of youths with ADHD clearly contained more boys than girls [33, 34], whereas in the nonclinical control group the gender distribution was more equal. Available studies suggest that there are no pronounced differences in loneliness between boys and girls [e.g., 36], but it should be admitted that research in clinical samples (such as youths with ASD) is lacking and so this remains a topic of future inquiry.
The results of the present study are also relevant for clinical practice. Loneliness seems to be a rather overlooked problem that deserves more attention. Clinicians should focus more on understanding the implications of social impairments and the unmet desire for social interaction in youths with ASD. These youngsters often have a learning history of social failure and disappointment and might simply give up and choose to protect themselves against future difficulties by means of withdrawal and avoidance behavior, thereby becoming trapped in isolation and feelings of loneliness [59]. Interventions that enhance social skills might be of great value. In fact, it has been demonstrated that social skills training for children with ASD can successfully decrease feelings of loneliness [60]. It is recommended to implement this type of intervention before the transition from primary to secondary school so that children are better equipped to face the social challenges of adolescence.

Summary

The results of the current study revealed that youths with ASD generally reported higher levels of loneliness as compared to clinical (ADHD) and non-clinical control youths, but this difference was only statistically significant at the adolescent age. Youths with ASD expressed lower levels of desire for social interaction than the control groups, and findings also indicated that even these youngster display an increase in the desire for social interaction during adolescence. Further, as expected, the ASD group clearly displayed lower levels of social competence and social skills and higher levels of social problems and social anxiety as compared to the control groups. These findings seem to fit with the picture that youths with ASD, especially during the adolescent years when they experience at least some desire for social interaction, often fail to realize satisfactory social interactions resulting in feelings of loneliness [17, 25]. This also suggests that social skills training might be particularly valuable in terms of intervention [60]. That is, by increasing the social abilities of young people with ASD, they might be able to adequately meet their limited but still present wish for interacting with others, thereby preventing or alleviating feelings of loneliness.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Literatuur
1.
go back to reference Baumeister RF, Leary MR (1995) The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol Bull 117:497–529CrossRefPubMed Baumeister RF, Leary MR (1995) The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol Bull 117:497–529CrossRefPubMed
2.
go back to reference Hagerty BM, Williams RA, Coyne JC, Early MR (1996) Sense of belonging and indicators of social and psychological functioning. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 10:235–244CrossRefPubMed Hagerty BM, Williams RA, Coyne JC, Early MR (1996) Sense of belonging and indicators of social and psychological functioning. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 10:235–244CrossRefPubMed
3.
go back to reference Heinrich LM, Gullone E (2006) The clinical significance of loneliness: a literature review. Clin Psychol Rev 26:695–718CrossRefPubMed Heinrich LM, Gullone E (2006) The clinical significance of loneliness: a literature review. Clin Psychol Rev 26:695–718CrossRefPubMed
4.
go back to reference Gierveld JDJ, Van Tilburg T (2006) A 6-item scale for overall, emotional, and social loneliness confirmatory tests on survey data. Res Aging 28:582–598CrossRef Gierveld JDJ, Van Tilburg T (2006) A 6-item scale for overall, emotional, and social loneliness confirmatory tests on survey data. Res Aging 28:582–598CrossRef
5.
go back to reference Peplau LA, Perlman D (1982) Perspectives on loneliness. Peplau LA, Perlman D, In: Loneliness: a sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy. Wiley, New York, pp 1–18 Peplau LA, Perlman D (1982) Perspectives on loneliness. Peplau LA, Perlman D, In: Loneliness: a sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy. Wiley, New York, pp 1–18
6.
go back to reference Vanhalst J, Luyckx K, Goossens L (2014) Experiencing loneliness in adolescence: A matter of individual characteristics, negative peer experiences, or both? Soc Dev 23:100–118CrossRef Vanhalst J, Luyckx K, Goossens L (2014) Experiencing loneliness in adolescence: A matter of individual characteristics, negative peer experiences, or both? Soc Dev 23:100–118CrossRef
7.
go back to reference Laursen B, Hartl AC (2013) Understanding loneliness during adolescence: developmental changes that increase the risk of perceived social isolation. J Adolesc 36:1261–1268CrossRefPubMed Laursen B, Hartl AC (2013) Understanding loneliness during adolescence: developmental changes that increase the risk of perceived social isolation. J Adolesc 36:1261–1268CrossRefPubMed
8.
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edition), text revision. APA, Washington DC American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edition), text revision. APA, Washington DC
9.
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edition). APA, Washington DCCrossRef American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edition). APA, Washington DCCrossRef
10.
go back to reference Macintosh K, Dissanayake C (2006) Social skills and problem behaviours in school aged children with high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 36:1065–1076CrossRefPubMed Macintosh K, Dissanayake C (2006) Social skills and problem behaviours in school aged children with high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 36:1065–1076CrossRefPubMed
11.
go back to reference Cappadocia MC, Weiss JA, Pepler D (2012) Bullying experiences among children and youth with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 42:266–277CrossRefPubMed Cappadocia MC, Weiss JA, Pepler D (2012) Bullying experiences among children and youth with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 42:266–277CrossRefPubMed
12.
go back to reference Calder L, Hill V, Pellicano E (2013) ‘Sometimes I want to play by myself’: understanding what friendship means to children with autism in mainstream primary schools. Autism 17:296–316CrossRefPubMed Calder L, Hill V, Pellicano E (2013) ‘Sometimes I want to play by myself’: understanding what friendship means to children with autism in mainstream primary schools. Autism 17:296–316CrossRefPubMed
13.
go back to reference McConnell SR (2002) Interventions to facilitate social interaction for young children with autism: Review of available research and recommendations for educational intervention and future research. J Autism Dev Disord 32:351–372CrossRefPubMed McConnell SR (2002) Interventions to facilitate social interaction for young children with autism: Review of available research and recommendations for educational intervention and future research. J Autism Dev Disord 32:351–372CrossRefPubMed
14.
go back to reference Locke J, Ishijima EH, Kasari C, London N (2010) Loneliness, friendship quality and the social networks of adolescents with high-functioning autism in an inclusive school setting. J Res Spec Educ Needs 10:74–81CrossRef Locke J, Ishijima EH, Kasari C, London N (2010) Loneliness, friendship quality and the social networks of adolescents with high-functioning autism in an inclusive school setting. J Res Spec Educ Needs 10:74–81CrossRef
15.
go back to reference Kasari C, Locke J, Gulsrud A, Rotheram-Fuller E (2011) Social networks and friendships at school: comparing children with and without ASD. J Autism Dev Disord 41:533–544CrossRefPubMed Kasari C, Locke J, Gulsrud A, Rotheram-Fuller E (2011) Social networks and friendships at school: comparing children with and without ASD. J Autism Dev Disord 41:533–544CrossRefPubMed
16.
go back to reference Bauminger N, Kasari C (2000) Loneliness and friendship in high-functioning children with autism. Child Dev 71:447–456CrossRefPubMed Bauminger N, Kasari C (2000) Loneliness and friendship in high-functioning children with autism. Child Dev 71:447–456CrossRefPubMed
17.
go back to reference Bauminger N, Shulman C, Agam G (2003) Peer interaction and loneliness in high-functioning children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 33:489–507CrossRefPubMed Bauminger N, Shulman C, Agam G (2003) Peer interaction and loneliness in high-functioning children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 33:489–507CrossRefPubMed
18.
go back to reference Kalyva E (2010) Multirater congruence on the social skills assessment of children with asperger syndrome: self, mother, father, and teacher ratings. J Autism Dev Disord 40:1202–1208CrossRefPubMed Kalyva E (2010) Multirater congruence on the social skills assessment of children with asperger syndrome: self, mother, father, and teacher ratings. J Autism Dev Disord 40:1202–1208CrossRefPubMed
19.
go back to reference Lasgaard M, Nielsen A, Eriksen ME, Goossens L (2010) Loneliness and social support in adolescent boys with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 40:218–226CrossRefPubMed Lasgaard M, Nielsen A, Eriksen ME, Goossens L (2010) Loneliness and social support in adolescent boys with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 40:218–226CrossRefPubMed
20.
go back to reference Whitehouse AJ, Durkin K, Jaquet E, Ziatas K (2009) Friendship, loneliness and depression in adolescents with Asperger’s Syndrome. J Adolesc 32:309–322CrossRefPubMed Whitehouse AJ, Durkin K, Jaquet E, Ziatas K (2009) Friendship, loneliness and depression in adolescents with Asperger’s Syndrome. J Adolesc 32:309–322CrossRefPubMed
21.
go back to reference Chamberlain B, Kasari C, Rotheram-Fuller E (2007) Involvement or isolation? The social networks of children with autism in regular classrooms. J Autism Dev Disord 37:230–242CrossRefPubMed Chamberlain B, Kasari C, Rotheram-Fuller E (2007) Involvement or isolation? The social networks of children with autism in regular classrooms. J Autism Dev Disord 37:230–242CrossRefPubMed
23.
go back to reference Kanner L (1943) Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child 2:217–250 Kanner L (1943) Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child 2:217–250
24.
go back to reference Kasari C, Sterling L (2014) Loneliness and Social Isolation in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. In: Coplan RJ, Bowker JC, The handbook of solitude: Psychological perspectives on social Isolation, social withdrawal, and being alone. Wiley, Chichester, pp 409–426 Kasari C, Sterling L (2014) Loneliness and Social Isolation in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. In: Coplan RJ, Bowker JC, The handbook of solitude: Psychological perspectives on social Isolation, social withdrawal, and being alone. Wiley, Chichester, pp 409–426
25.
go back to reference Deckers A, Roelofs J, Muris P, Rinck M (2014) The desire for social interaction in children with an autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord 8:449–453CrossRef Deckers A, Roelofs J, Muris P, Rinck M (2014) The desire for social interaction in children with an autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord 8:449–453CrossRef
26.
go back to reference Lasgaard M, Goossens L, Bramsen RH, Trillingsgaard T, Elklit A (2011) Different sources of loneliness are associated with different forms of psychopathology in adolescence. J Res Pers 45:233–237CrossRef Lasgaard M, Goossens L, Bramsen RH, Trillingsgaard T, Elklit A (2011) Different sources of loneliness are associated with different forms of psychopathology in adolescence. J Res Pers 45:233–237CrossRef
27.
go back to reference Heiman T (2005) An examination of peer relationships of children with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Sch Psychol Int 26:330–339CrossRef Heiman T (2005) An examination of peer relationships of children with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Sch Psychol Int 26:330–339CrossRef
28.
go back to reference Houghton S, Roost E, Carroll A, Brandtman M (2015) Loneliness in children and adolescents with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 37:27–37CrossRef Houghton S, Roost E, Carroll A, Brandtman M (2015) Loneliness in children and adolescents with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 37:27–37CrossRef
29.
go back to reference Frankel F, Feinberg D (2002) Social problems associated with ADHD vs. ODD in children referred for friendship problems. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 33:125–146CrossRefPubMed Frankel F, Feinberg D (2002) Social problems associated with ADHD vs. ODD in children referred for friendship problems. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 33:125–146CrossRefPubMed
30.
go back to reference Hoza B, Mrug S, Gerdes AC, Hinshaw SP, Bukowski WM, Gold JA, Kraemer HC, Pelham WE, Wigal T, Arnold LE (2005) What aspects of peer relationships are impaired in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? J Consult Clin Psychol 73:411–423CrossRefPubMed Hoza B, Mrug S, Gerdes AC, Hinshaw SP, Bukowski WM, Gold JA, Kraemer HC, Pelham WE, Wigal T, Arnold LE (2005) What aspects of peer relationships are impaired in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? J Consult Clin Psychol 73:411–423CrossRefPubMed
31.
go back to reference Ronk MJ, Hund AM, Landau S (2011) Assessment of social competence of boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: problematic peer entry, host responses, and evaluations. J Abn Child Psychol 39:829–840CrossRef Ronk MJ, Hund AM, Landau S (2011) Assessment of social competence of boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: problematic peer entry, host responses, and evaluations. J Abn Child Psychol 39:829–840CrossRef
32.
go back to reference Spitzer RL (1983) Psychiatric diagnosis: are clinicians still necessary? Compr Psychiatry 24:399–411CrossRefPubMed Spitzer RL (1983) Psychiatric diagnosis: are clinicians still necessary? Compr Psychiatry 24:399–411CrossRefPubMed
33.
go back to reference Fombonne E (2003) Epidemiological surveys of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders: an update. J Autism Dev Disord 33:365–382CrossRefPubMed Fombonne E (2003) Epidemiological surveys of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders: an update. J Autism Dev Disord 33:365–382CrossRefPubMed
34.
go back to reference Ramtekkar UP, Reiersen AM, Todorov AA, Todd RD (2010) Sex and age differences in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and diagnoses: Implications for DSM-V and ICD-11. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49:217–228PubMedPubMedCentral Ramtekkar UP, Reiersen AM, Todorov AA, Todd RD (2010) Sex and age differences in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and diagnoses: Implications for DSM-V and ICD-11. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49:217–228PubMedPubMedCentral
35.
go back to reference Marcoen A, Goossens L, Caes P (1987) Loneliness in pre- through late adolescence: exploring the contributions of a multidimensional approach. J Youth Adolesc 16:561–577CrossRefPubMed Marcoen A, Goossens L, Caes P (1987) Loneliness in pre- through late adolescence: exploring the contributions of a multidimensional approach. J Youth Adolesc 16:561–577CrossRefPubMed
36.
go back to reference Maes M, Klimstra T, Van den Noortgate W, Goossens L (2015) Factor structure and measurement invariance of a multidimensional loneliness scale: comparisons across gender and age. J Child Fam Stud 24:1829–1837CrossRef Maes M, Klimstra T, Van den Noortgate W, Goossens L (2015) Factor structure and measurement invariance of a multidimensional loneliness scale: comparisons across gender and age. J Child Fam Stud 24:1829–1837CrossRef
37.
go back to reference Goossens L, Beyers W (2002) Comparing measures of childhood loneliness: internal consistency and confirmatory factor analysis. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 31:252–262CrossRefPubMed Goossens L, Beyers W (2002) Comparing measures of childhood loneliness: internal consistency and confirmatory factor analysis. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 31:252–262CrossRefPubMed
39.
go back to reference Barry TD, Klinger LG, Lee JM, Palardy N, Gilmore T, Bodin SD (2003) Examining the effectiveness of an outpatient clinic-based social skills group for high-functioning children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 33:685–701CrossRefPubMed Barry TD, Klinger LG, Lee JM, Palardy N, Gilmore T, Bodin SD (2003) Examining the effectiveness of an outpatient clinic-based social skills group for high-functioning children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 33:685–701CrossRefPubMed
40.
go back to reference Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA (2001) Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families, Burlington VT Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA (2001) Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families, Burlington VT
41.
go back to reference Bérubé RL, Achenbach TM (2010) Bibliography of published studies using the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment: 2006 Edition. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families, Burlington VT Bérubé RL, Achenbach TM (2010) Bibliography of published studies using the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment: 2006 Edition. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families, Burlington VT
42.
go back to reference Pandolfi V, Magyar CI, Dill CA (2012) An initial psychometric evaluation of the CBCL 6–18 in a sample of youth with autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord 6:96–108CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pandolfi V, Magyar CI, Dill CA (2012) An initial psychometric evaluation of the CBCL 6–18 in a sample of youth with autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord 6:96–108CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
43.
go back to reference Bodden DHM, Bögels SM, Muris P (2009) The diagnostic utility of the screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders-71 (SCARED-71). Behav Res Ther 47:418–425CrossRefPubMed Bodden DHM, Bögels SM, Muris P (2009) The diagnostic utility of the screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders-71 (SCARED-71). Behav Res Ther 47:418–425CrossRefPubMed
44.
go back to reference Van Steensel FJA, Deutschman AACG, Bögels SM (2013) Examining the Screen for child Anxiety-Related Emotional Disorder-71 as an assessment tool for anxiety in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Autism 17:681–692CrossRefPubMed Van Steensel FJA, Deutschman AACG, Bögels SM (2013) Examining the Screen for child Anxiety-Related Emotional Disorder-71 as an assessment tool for anxiety in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Autism 17:681–692CrossRefPubMed
45.
go back to reference Muris P, Merckelbach H, Damsma E (2000) Threat perception bias in nonreferred socially anxious children. J Clin Child Psychol 29:348–359CrossRefPubMed Muris P, Merckelbach H, Damsma E (2000) Threat perception bias in nonreferred socially anxious children. J Clin Child Psychol 29:348–359CrossRefPubMed
46.
go back to reference Kerig PK, Ludlow A, Wenar C (2012) Developmental psychopathology. MacGraw-Hill, Columbus, OH Kerig PK, Ludlow A, Wenar C (2012) Developmental psychopathology. MacGraw-Hill, Columbus, OH
47.
go back to reference Qualter P, Brown SL, Rotenberg KJ, Vanhalst J, Harris RA, Goossens L et al (2013) Trajectories of loneliness during childhood and adolescence: predictors and health outcomes. J Adolesc 36:1283–1293CrossRefPubMed Qualter P, Brown SL, Rotenberg KJ, Vanhalst J, Harris RA, Goossens L et al (2013) Trajectories of loneliness during childhood and adolescence: predictors and health outcomes. J Adolesc 36:1283–1293CrossRefPubMed
48.
go back to reference McQuade JD, Hoza B (2008) Peer problems in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: current status and future directions. Dev Disabil Res Rev 14:320–324CrossRefPubMed McQuade JD, Hoza B (2008) Peer problems in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: current status and future directions. Dev Disabil Res Rev 14:320–324CrossRefPubMed
49.
go back to reference Nijmeijer JS, Minderaa RB, Buitelaar JK, Mulligan A, Hartman CA, Hoekstra PJ (2008) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and social dysfunctioning. Clin Psychol Rev 28:692–708CrossRefPubMed Nijmeijer JS, Minderaa RB, Buitelaar JK, Mulligan A, Hartman CA, Hoekstra PJ (2008) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and social dysfunctioning. Clin Psychol Rev 28:692–708CrossRefPubMed
50.
go back to reference Zeedyk SM, Cohen SR, Eisenhower A, Blacher J (2015) Perceived social competence and loneliness among young children with ASD: child, parent and teacher reports. J Autism Dev Disord 46:436–449CrossRef Zeedyk SM, Cohen SR, Eisenhower A, Blacher J (2015) Perceived social competence and loneliness among young children with ASD: child, parent and teacher reports. J Autism Dev Disord 46:436–449CrossRef
51.
go back to reference Cavanagh AM, Buehler C (2016) Adolescent loneliness and social anxiety: the role of multiple sources of support. J Soc Pers Relation 33:149–170CrossRef Cavanagh AM, Buehler C (2016) Adolescent loneliness and social anxiety: the role of multiple sources of support. J Soc Pers Relation 33:149–170CrossRef
52.
go back to reference Myin-Germeys I, Oorschot M, Collip D, Lataster J, Delespaul P, Van Os J (2009) Experience sampling research in psychopathology: opening the black box of life. Psychol Med 39:1533–1547CrossRefPubMed Myin-Germeys I, Oorschot M, Collip D, Lataster J, Delespaul P, Van Os J (2009) Experience sampling research in psychopathology: opening the black box of life. Psychol Med 39:1533–1547CrossRefPubMed
53.
go back to reference Ebesutani C, Fierstein M, Viana AG, Trent L, Young J, Sprung M (2015) The role of loneliness in the relationship between anxiety and depression in clinical and school-based youth. Psychol Sch 52:223–234CrossRef Ebesutani C, Fierstein M, Viana AG, Trent L, Young J, Sprung M (2015) The role of loneliness in the relationship between anxiety and depression in clinical and school-based youth. Psychol Sch 52:223–234CrossRef
54.
go back to reference Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A (1994) Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 24:659–685CrossRefPubMed Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A (1994) Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 24:659–685CrossRefPubMed
55.
go back to reference Hien D, Matzner FJ, First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW (1994) Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-child edition (version 1.0). Columbia University, New York Hien D, Matzner FJ, First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW (1994) Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-child edition (version 1.0). Columbia University, New York
56.
go back to reference Gargaro BA, Rinehart NJ, Bradshaw JL, Tonge BJ, Sheppard DM (2011) Autism and ADHD: how far have we come in the comorbidity debate? Neurosc Biobehav Rev 35:1081–1088CrossRef Gargaro BA, Rinehart NJ, Bradshaw JL, Tonge BJ, Sheppard DM (2011) Autism and ADHD: how far have we come in the comorbidity debate? Neurosc Biobehav Rev 35:1081–1088CrossRef
57.
go back to reference Smith IC, Reichow B, Volkmar FR (2015) The effects of DSM-5 criteria on number of individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review. J Autism Dev Disord 45:2541–2552CrossRefPubMed Smith IC, Reichow B, Volkmar FR (2015) The effects of DSM-5 criteria on number of individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review. J Autism Dev Disord 45:2541–2552CrossRefPubMed
58.
go back to reference Van Steensel FJ, Bögels SM, De Bruin EI (2015) DSM-IV versus DSM-5 autism spectrum disorder and social anxiety disorder in childhood: similarities and differences. J Child Fam Stud 24:2752–2756CrossRef Van Steensel FJ, Bögels SM, De Bruin EI (2015) DSM-IV versus DSM-5 autism spectrum disorder and social anxiety disorder in childhood: similarities and differences. J Child Fam Stud 24:2752–2756CrossRef
59.
go back to reference Kasari C, Sterling L (2013) Loneliness and social isolation in children with autism spectrum disorders. In: Coplan RJ, Bowker JC (eds) The handbook of solitude: psychological perspectives on social isolation, social withdrawal, and being alone. Wiley, New York, pp 409–426CrossRef Kasari C, Sterling L (2013) Loneliness and social isolation in children with autism spectrum disorders. In: Coplan RJ, Bowker JC (eds) The handbook of solitude: psychological perspectives on social isolation, social withdrawal, and being alone. Wiley, New York, pp 409–426CrossRef
60.
go back to reference Deckers A, Muris P, Roelofs J, Arntz A (2016) A group-administered social skills training for 8- to 12-year-old, high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders: an evaluation of its effectiveness in a naturalistic outpatient treatment setting. J Autism Dev Disord 46:3493–3504CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Deckers A, Muris P, Roelofs J, Arntz A (2016) A group-administered social skills training for 8- to 12-year-old, high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders: an evaluation of its effectiveness in a naturalistic outpatient treatment setting. J Autism Dev Disord 46:3493–3504CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metagegevens
Titel
Being on Your Own or Feeling Lonely? Loneliness and Other Social Variables in Youths with Autism Spectrum Disorders
Auteurs
Anne Deckers
Peter Muris
Jeffrey Roelofs
Publicatiedatum
09-01-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Child Psychiatry & Human Development / Uitgave 5/2017
Print ISSN: 0009-398X
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3327
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-016-0707-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2017

Child Psychiatry & Human Development 5/2017 Naar de uitgave